Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
1.
PLoS Biol ; 19(9): e3001400, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34529650

RESUMEN

Purkinje cell (PC) discharge, the only output of cerebellar cortex, involves 2 types of action potentials, high-frequency simple spikes (SSs) and low-frequency complex spikes (CSs). While there is consensus that SSs convey information needed to optimize movement kinematics, the function of CSs, determined by the PC's climbing fiber input, remains controversial. While initially thought to be specialized in reporting information on motor error for the subsequent amendment of behavior, CSs seem to contribute to other aspects of motor behavior as well. When faced with the bewildering diversity of findings and views unraveled by highly specific tasks, one may wonder if there is just one true function with all the other attributions wrong? Or is the diversity of findings a reflection of distinct pools of PCs, each processing specific streams of information conveyed by climbing fibers? With these questions in mind, we recorded CSs from the monkey oculomotor vermis deploying a repetitive saccade task that entailed sizable motor errors as well as small amplitude saccades, correcting them. We demonstrate that, in addition to carrying error-related information, CSs carry information on the metrics of both primary and small corrective saccades in a time-specific manner, with changes in CS firing probability coupled with changes in CS duration. Furthermore, we also found CS activity that seemed to predict the upcoming events. Hence PCs receive a multiplexed climbing fiber input that merges complementary streams of information on the behavior, separable by the recipient PC because they are staggered in time.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Movimiento
2.
PLoS Biol ; 16(8): e2004344, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30067764

RESUMEN

The cerebellum allows us to rapidly adjust motor behavior to the needs of the situation. It is commonly assumed that cerebellum-based motor learning is guided by the difference between the desired and the actual behavior, i.e., by error information. Not only immediate but also future behavior will benefit from an error because it induces lasting changes of parallel fiber synapses on Purkinje cells (PCs), whose output mediates the behavioral adjustments. Olivary climbing fibers, likewise connecting with PCs, are thought to transport information on instant errors needed for the synaptic modification yet not to contribute to error memory. Here, we report work on monkeys tested in a saccadic learning paradigm that challenges this concept. We demonstrate not only a clear complex spikes (CS) signature of the error at the time of its occurrence but also a reverberation of this signature much later, before a new manifestation of the behavior, suitable to improve it.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Cerebelo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Axones/fisiología , Cerebelo/anatomía & histología , Cerebelo/citología , Electrodos Implantados , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/citología , Técnicas Estereotáxicas , Sinapsis/fisiología
3.
J Vis ; 17(9): 19, 2017 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28837965

RESUMEN

Scrutiny of the visual environment requires saccades that shift gaze to objects of interest. In case the object should be moving, smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) try to keep the image of the object within the confines of the fovea in order to ensure sufficient time for its analysis. Both saccades and SPEM can be adaptively changed by the experience of insufficiencies, compromising the precision of saccades or the minimization of object image slip in the case of SPEM. As both forms of adaptation rely on the cerebellar oculomotor vermis (OMV), most probably deploying a shared neuronal machinery, one might expect that the adaptation of one type of eye movement should affect the kinematics of the other. In order to test this expectation, we subjected two monkeys to a standard saccadic adaption paradigm with SPEM test trials at the end and, alternatively, the same two monkeys plus a third one to a random saccadic adaptation paradigm with interleaved trials of SPEM. In contrast to our expectation, we observed at best marginal transfer which, moreover, had little consistency across experiments and subjects. The lack of consistent transfer of saccadic adaptation decisively constrains models of the implementation of oculomotor learning in the OMV, suggesting an extensive separation of saccade- and SPEM-related synapses on P-cell dendritic trees.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Animales , Probabilidad
4.
J Neurosci ; 35(8): 3403-11, 2015 Feb 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25716840

RESUMEN

Microsaccades, the small saccades made when we try to keep the eyes still, were once believed to be inconsequential for vision, but recent studies suggest that they can precisely relocate gaze to tiny visual targets. Because the cerebellum is necessary for motor precision, we investigated whether microsaccades may exploit this neural machinery in monkeys. Almost all vermal Purkinje cells, which provide the eye-related output of the cerebellar cortex, were found to increase or decrease their simple spike firing rate during microsaccades. At both the single-cell and population level, microsaccade-related activity was highly similar to macrosaccade-related activity and we observed a continuous representation of saccade amplitude that spanned both the macrosaccade and microsaccade domains. Our results suggest that the cerebellum's role in fine-tuning eye movements extends even to the oculomotor system's smallest saccades and add to a growing list of observations that call into question the classical categorical distinction between microsaccades and macrosaccades.


Asunto(s)
Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
5.
Eur J Neurosci ; 44(8): 2531-2542, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27255776

RESUMEN

Recent studies have suggested that microsaccades, the small amplitude saccades made during fixation, are precisely controlled. Two lines of evidence suggest that the cerebellum plays a key role not only in improving the accuracy of macrosaccades but also of microsaccades. First, lesions of the fastigial oculomotor regions (FOR) cause horizontal dysmetria of both micro- and macrosaccades. Secondly, our previous work on Purkinje cell simple spikes in the oculomotor vermis (OV) has established qualitatively similar response preferences for these two groups of saccades. In this work, we investigated the control signals for micro- and macrosaccades in the FOR, the target of OV Purkinje cell axons. We found that the same FOR neurons discharged for micro- and macrosaccades. For both groups of saccades, FOR neurons exhibited very similar dependencies of their discharge strength on direction and amplitude and very similar burst onset time differences for ipsi- and contraversive saccades and, in both, response duration reflected saccade duration, at least at the population level. An intriguing characteristic of microsaccade-related responses is that immediate pre-saccadic firing rates decreased with distance to the target center, a pattern that strikingly parallels the eye position dependency of both microsaccade metrics and frequency, which may suggest a potential neural mechanism underlying the role of FOR in fixation. Irrespective of this specific consideration, our study supports the view that microsaccades and macrosaccades share the same cerebellar circuitry and, in general, further strengthens the notion of a microsaccade-macrosaccade continuum.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Axones/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Cerebelo/fisiología , Macaca mulatta
6.
Proc Biol Sci ; 282(1816): 20151020, 2015 Oct 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26446808

RESUMEN

Human eye-gaze is a powerful stimulus, drawing the observer's attention to places and objects of interest to someone else ('eye-gaze following'). The largely homogeneous eyes of monkeys, compromising the assessment of eye-gaze by conspecifics from larger distances, explain the absence of comparable eye-gaze following in these animals. Yet, monkeys are able to use peer head orientation to shift attention ('head-gaze following'). How similar are monkeys' head-gaze and human eye-gaze following? To address this question, we trained rhesus monkeys to make saccades to targets, either identified by the head-gaze of demonstrator monkeys or, alternatively, identified by learned associations between the demonstrators' facial identities and the targets (gaze versus identity following). In a variant of this task that occurred at random, the instruction to follow head-gaze or identity was replaced in the course of a trial by the new rule to detect a change of luminance of one of the saccade targets. Although this change-of-rule rendered the demonstrator portraits irrelevant, they nevertheless influenced performance, reflecting a precise redistribution of spatial attention. The specific features depended on whether the initial rule was head-gaze or identity following: head-gaze caused an insuppressible shift of attention to the target gazed at by the demonstrator, whereas identity matching prompted much later shifts of attention, however, only if the initial rule had been identity following. Furthermore, shifts of attention prompted by head-gaze were spatially precise. Automaticity and swiftness, spatial precision and limited executive control characterizing monkeys' head-gaze following are key features of human eye-gaze following. This similarity supports the notion that both may rely on the same conserved neural circuitry.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Cara , Cabeza , Visión Ocular
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 22(4): 877-91, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21725035

RESUMEN

Lesion studies suggest that the oculomotor vermis (OMV) is critical for the initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements (SPEMs); yet, its specific role has remained elusive. In this study, we tested the hypothesis that vermal Purkinje cells (PCs) may be needed to fine-tune the kinematic description of SPEM initiation. Recording from identified PCs from the monkey OMV, we observed that SPEM-related PCs were characterized by a formidable diversity of response profiles with typically only modest reflection of eye movement kinematics. In contrast, the PC population discharge could be perfectly predicted based on a linear combination of eye acceleration, velocity, and position. This finding is in full accord with a role of the OMV in shaping eye movement kinematics. It, moreover, supports the notion that this shaping action is based on a population code, whose anatomic basis is the convergence of PCs on target neurons in the cerebellar nuclei.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Cerebelo/citología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Modelos Lineales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
8.
Cereb Cortex ; 22(2): 345-62, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21670098

RESUMEN

The pontine nuclei (PN) are the major intermediary elements in the corticopontocerebellar pathway. Here we asked if the PN may help to adapt the spatial reference frames used by cerebrocortical neurons involved in the sensory guidance of movement to a format potentially more appropriate for the cerebellum. To this end, we studied movement-related neurons in the dorsal PN (DPN) of monkeys, most probably projecting to the cerebellum, executing fixed vector saccades or, alternatively, fixed vector hand reaches from different starting positions. The 83 task-related neurons considered fired movement-related bursts before saccades (saccade-related) or before hand movements (hand movement-related). About 40% of the SR neurons were "oculocentric," whereas the others were modulated by eye starting position. A third of the HMR neurons encoded hand reaches in hand-centered coordinates, whereas the remainder exhibited different types of dependencies on starting positions, reminiscent in general of cortical responses. All in all, pontine reference frames for the sensory guidance of movement seem to be very similar to those in cortex. Specifically, the frequency of orbital position gain fields of SR neurons is identical in the DPN and in one of their major cortical inputs, lateral intraparietal area (LIP).


Asunto(s)
Movimiento/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Puente/citología , Puente/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Fijación Ocular , Mano , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
9.
Primates ; 63(5): 535-546, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35838928

RESUMEN

Gaze aversion is a behavior adopted by several mammalian and non-mammalian species in response to eye contact, and is usually interpreted as a reaction to a perceived threat. Unlike many other primate species, common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are thought to have a high tolerance for direct gaze, barely exhibiting gaze avoidance towards conspecifics and humans. Here we show that this does not hold for marmosets interacting with a familiar experimenter who suddenly establishes eye contact in a playful interaction (peekaboo). Video footage synchronously recorded from the perspective of the marmoset and the experimenter showed that the monkeys consistently alternated between eye contact and head-gaze aversion, and that these responses were often preceded by head-cocking. We hypothesize that this behavioral strategy helps marmosets to temporarily disengage from emotionally overwhelming social stimulation due to sight of another individual's face, in order to prepare for a new round of affiliative face-to-face interactions.


Asunto(s)
Callithrix , Juego e Implementos de Juego , Animales , Callithrix/fisiología , Humanos , Mamíferos
10.
J Neurosci ; 30(47): 15834-42, 2010 Nov 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21106822

RESUMEN

The physical properties of our bodies are subject to change (due to fatigue, heavy equipment, injury or aging) as we move around in the surrounding environment. The traditional definition of motor adaptation dictates that a mechanism in our brain needs to compensate for such alterations by appropriately modifying neural motor commands, if the vitally important accuracy of executed movements is to be preserved. In this article we describe how a repetitive eye movement task brings about changes in eye saccade kinematics that compromise accurate motor performance in the absence of a proper compensatory response. Surgical lesions in animals and human patient studies have previously demonstrated that an intact cerebellum is necessary for the compensation to arise and prevent the occurrence of hypometric movements. Here we identified the dynamic properties of the eye plant by recording from abducens motoneurons responsible for the required movement and measured the muscle response to microstimulation of the abducens nucleus in rhesus monkeys. The ensuing results demonstrate that the muscular periphery remains intact during the fatiguing eye movement task, while internal sources of noise (drowsiness, attentional modulation, neuronal fatigue etc.) must be responsible for a diminished oculomotor performance. This finding leads to the important realization that while supervising the accuracy of our movements, the nervous system takes additionally into account and adapts to any disruptive processes within the brain itself, clearly unrelated to the dynamical behavior of muscles or the environment. The existence of this supplementary mechanism forces a reassessment of traditional views of cerebellum-dependent motor adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fatiga Muscular/fisiología , Músculos Oculomotores/fisiología , Animales , Sistema Nervioso Central/fisiología , Macaca mulatta , Músculos Oculomotores/inervación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
11.
Neuroimage ; 54(2): 1643-53, 2011 Jan 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20832481

RESUMEN

Previous fMRI experiments showed an involvement of the STS in the processing of eye-gaze direction in joint attention. Since head-gaze direction can also be used for the assessment of another person's attentional focus, we compared the mechanisms underlying the processing of head- and eye-gaze direction using a combined psychophysical and fMRI approach. Subjects actively followed the head- or eye-gaze direction of a person in a photograph towards one of seven possible targets by moving their eyes. We showed that the right posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) as well as the right fusiform gyrus (FSG) were involved in both processing of head- as well as eye-gaze direction. Another finding was a bilateral deactivation of a distinct area in the middle STS (mSTS) as well as the left anterior STS (aSTS), that was stronger when subjects followed eye-gaze direction than when they followed head-gaze direction. We assume that this deactivation is based on an active suppression of information arising from the distracting other directional cue, i.e. head-gaze direction in the eye-gaze direction task and eye-gaze direction in the head-gaze direction task. These results further support the hypothesis that the human equivalent of the gaze sensitive area in monkeys lies in more anterior parts of the STS than previously thought.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Ojo , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Cabeza , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 105(20): 7309-14, 2008 May 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18477700

RESUMEN

The improvement of motor behavior, based on experience, is a form of learning that is critically dependent on the cerebellum. A well studied example of cerebellar motor learning is short-term saccadic adaptation (STSA). In STSA, information on saccadic errors is used to improve future saccades. The information optimizing saccade metrics is conveyed by Purkinje cells simple spikes (PC-SS) because they are the critical input to the premotor circuits for saccades. We recorded PC-SS of monkeys undergoing STSA to reveal the code used for improving behavior. We found that the discharge of individual PC-SS was unable to account for the behavioral changes. The PC-SS population burst (PB), however, exhibited changes that closely paralleled the qualitatively different changes of saccade kinematics associated with gain-increase and gain-decrease STSA, respectively. Gain-increase STSA, characterized by an increase in saccade duration, replicates the relationship between saccade duration and the end of the PB valid for unadapted saccades. In contrast, gain-decrease STSA, which sports normal saccade duration but reduced saccadic velocity, is characterized by a PB that ends well before the adapted saccade. This suggests that the duration of normal as well as gain-increased saccades is determined by appropriately setting the end of PB end. However, the duration of gain-decreased saccades is apparently not modified by the cerebellum because the PB signals ends too early to determine saccade end. In summary, STSA, and most probably cerebellar-dependent learning in general, is based on optimizing the shape of a PC-SS population response.


Asunto(s)
Cerebelo/patología , Aprendizaje , Destreza Motora , Células de Purkinje/citología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Potenciales de Acción , Animales , Conducta Animal , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Núcleos Cerebelosos , Electrofisiología , Macaca mulatta , Modelos Biológicos , Vías Nerviosas , Primates
13.
J Neurosci ; 29(19): 6154-66, 2009 May 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19439593

RESUMEN

Prevailing concepts on the control of goal-directed hand movements (HM) have focused on a network of cortical areas whose early parieto-occipital stages are thought to extract and integrate information on target and hand location, involving subsequent stages in frontal cortex forming and executing movement plans. The substantial experimental results supporting this "cortical network" concept for hand movements notwithstanding, the concept clearly needs refinement to account for the surprisingly mild HM disturbances resulting from disconnecting the parieto-occipital from the frontal stages of the network. Clinical observations have suggested the cerebropontocerebellar projection as an alternative pathway for the sensory guidance of HM. As a first step in assessing its role, we explored the pontine nuclei (PN) of rhesus monkeys, trained to make goal-directed hand and eye movements guided by spatial memory. We were indeed able to delineate a distinct cluster of neurons in the rostrodorsal PN, activated by the preparation and the execution of hand reaches, close to but distinct from the region in which saccade-related neurons may be observed. The movement-related activity of HM-related neurons starts earlier than that of saccade-related neurons and both neuron types are usually effector specific, i.e., they respond only to the movement of the preferred effector. This is also the case when motor synergies involving both effectors are executed. Our findings support the notion of a distinct precerebellar, pontine visuomotor channel for hand reaches that is anatomically and functionally largely separated from the one serving eye movements.


Asunto(s)
Mano/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Puente/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Microelectrodos , Desempeño Psicomotor , Factores de Tiempo
14.
J Neurosci ; 29(1): 250-62, 2009 Jan 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19129401

RESUMEN

The anatomical organization of the granular layer of the cerebellum suggests an important function for Golgi cells (GC) in the pathway conveying mossy fiber (MF) afferents to Purkinje cells. Based on such anatomic observations, early proposals have attributed a role in "gain control" for GCs, a function disputed by recent investigations, which assert that GCs instead contribute to oscillatory mechanisms. However, conclusive physiological evidence based on studies of cerebellum-dependent behavior supporting/dismissing the gain control proposition has been lacking as of yet. We addressed the possible function of this interneuron by recording the activity of a large number of both MFs and GCs during saccadic eye movements from the same cortical area of the monkey cerebellum, namely the oculomotor vermis (OMV). Our cellular identification conformed to previously established criteria, mainly to juxtacellular labeling studies correlating physiological parameters with cell morphology. Response patterns of both MFs and GCs were highly heterogeneous. MF discharges correlated linearly with eye saccade metrics and timing, showing directional preference and precise direction tuning. In contrast, GC discharges did not correlate strongly with the metrics or direction of movement. Their discharge properties were also unaffected by motor learning during saccadic adaptation. The OMV therefore receives a barrage of information about eye movements from different oculomotor areas over the MF pathway, which is not reflected in GCs. The unspecificity of GCs has important implications for the intricacies of neuronal processing in the granular layer, clearly discrediting their involvement in gain control and instead suggesting a more secluded role for these interneurons.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Cerebelo/citología , Cerebelo/fisiología , Interneuronas/fisiología , Fibras Nerviosas/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Corteza Cerebelosa/fisiología , Estimulación Eléctrica , Macaca mulatta , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales
15.
eNeuro ; 7(4)2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32513660

RESUMEN

Research on social perception in monkeys may benefit from standardized, controllable, and ethologically valid renditions of conspecifics offered by monkey avatars. However, previous work has cautioned that monkeys, like humans, show an adverse reaction toward realistic synthetic stimuli, known as the "uncanny valley" effect. We developed an improved naturalistic rhesus monkey face avatar capable of producing facial expressions (fear grin, lip smack and threat), animated by motion capture data of real monkeys. For validation, we additionally created decreasingly naturalistic avatar variants. Eight rhesus macaques were tested on the various videos and avoided looking at less naturalistic avatar variants, but not at the most naturalistic or the most unnaturalistic avatar, indicating an uncanny valley effect for the less naturalistic avatar versions. The avoidance was deepened by motion and accompanied by physiological arousal. Only the most naturalistic avatar evoked facial expressions comparable to those toward the real monkey videos. Hence, our findings demonstrate that the uncanny valley reaction in monkeys can be overcome by a highly naturalistic avatar.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Expresión Facial , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Movimiento (Física) , Percepción Social
16.
Eur J Neurosci ; 29(8): 1732-8, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19385988

RESUMEN

Human and non-human primates follow the gaze of their respective conspecific to identify objects of common interest. Whereas humans rely on eye-gaze for such purposes, monkeys preferentially use head-gaze information. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have delineated an area in the human superior temporal sulcus (STS), which is specifically activated when subjects actively follow the eye-gaze of others. Similarly, using fMRI, we have identified an analogous region in the monkey's middle STS responding to gaze following. Hence, although humans and monkeys might rely on different directional cues guiding their attention, they seem to deploy a similar and possibly homologous cortical area to follow the gaze of a conspecific. Our results support the idea that the eyes developed a new social function in human evolution, most likely to support cooperative mutual social interactions building on a phylogenetically old STS module for the processing of head cues.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/anatomía & histología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Animales , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Color , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Percepción Visual/fisiología
17.
J Vis ; 9(9): 5.1-13, 2009 Aug 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19761338

RESUMEN

During pursuit eye movements, the world around us remains perceptually stable despite the retinal-image slip induced by the eye movement. It is commonly held that this perceptual invariance is achieved by subtracting an internal reference signal, reflecting the eye movement, from the retinal motion signal. However, if the reference signal is too small or too large, a false eye-movement-induced motion of the external world, the Filehne illusion (FI), will be perceived. A reference signal of inadequate size can be simulated experimentally by asking human subjects to pursue a target across backgrounds with externally added motion that are perceived as moving. In the present study we asked if non-human primates respond to such manipulation in a way comparable to humans. Using psychophysical methods, we demonstrate that Rhesus monkeys do indeed experience a percept of pursuit-induced background motion. In this study we show that an FI can be predictably induced in Rhesus monkeys. The monkey FI shows dependencies on the size and direction of background movement, which is very similar to the ones characterizing the human FI. This congruence suggests that the perception of self-induced visual motion is based on similar inferential mechanisms in non-human and human primates.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Psicofísica , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Masculino , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Retina/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie
18.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 15292, 2019 10 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31653910

RESUMEN

The ability to extract the direction of the other's gaze allows us to shift our attention to an object of interest to the other and to establish joint attention. By mapping one's own intentions on the object of joint attention, humans develop a Theory of (the other's) Mind (TOM), a functional sequence possibly disrupted in autism. Gaze following of both humans and old world monkeys is orchestrated by very similar cortical architectures, strongly suggesting homology. Also new world monkeys, a primate suborder that split from the old world monkey line about 35 million years ago, have complex social structures and one member of this group, the common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) are known to follow human head-gaze. However, the question is if they use gaze following to establish joint attention with conspecifics. Here we show that this is indeed the case. In a free choice task, head-restrained marmosets prefer objects gazed at by a conspecific and, moreover, they exhibit considerably shorter choice reaction times for the same objects. These findings support the assumption of an evolutionarily old domain specific faculty shared within the primate order and they underline the potential value of marmosets in studies of normal and disturbed joint attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Callithrix/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Reflejo/fisiología , Animales , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
19.
Curr Biol ; 15(24): 2179-89, 2005 Dec 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16360681

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Cerebellar Purkinje cells (PC) generate two responses: the simple spike (SS), with high firing rates (>100 Hz), and the complex spike (CS), characterized by conspicuously low discharge rates (1-2 Hz). Contemporary theories of cerebellar learning suggest that the CS discharge pattern encodes an error signal that drives changes in SS activity, ultimately related to motor behavior. This then predicts that CS will discharge in relation to the error and at random once the error has been nulled by the new behavior. RESULTS: We tested this hypothesis with saccadic adaptation in macaque monkeys as a model of cerebellar-dependent motor learning. During saccadic adaptation, error information unconsciously changes the endpoint of a saccade prompted by a visual target that shifts its final position during the saccade. We recorded CS from PC of the posterior vermis before, during, and after saccadic adaptation. In clear contradiction to the "error signal" concept, we found that CS occurred at random before adaptation onset, i.e., when the error was maximal, and built up to a specific saccade-related discharge profile during the course of adaptation. This profile became most pronounced at the end of adaptation, i.e., when the error had been nulled. CONCLUSIONS: We suggest that CS firing may underlie the stabilization of a learned motor behavior, rather than serving as an electrophysiological correlate of an error.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Cerebelo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Animales , Electrofisiología , Modelos Teóricos
20.
Eur J Neurosci ; 27(4): 991-1002, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18333969

RESUMEN

We are usually unaware of retinal image motion resulting from our own movement. For instance, during slow-tracking eye movements the world around us remains perceptually stable despite the retinal image slip induced by the eye movement. It is commonly held that this example of perceptual invariance is achieved by subtracting an internal reference signal, reflecting the eye movement, from the retinal motion signal. If the two cancel each other, visual objects, which do not move, will also be perceived as non-moving. If, however, the reference signal is too small or too large, a false eye movement-induced motion of the external world, the Filehne illusion, will be perceived. We have exploited our ability to manipulate the size of the reference signal in an attempt to identify neurons in the visual cortex of monkeys, influenced by the percept of self-induced visual motion or the reference signal rather than the retinal motion signal. We report here that such 'percept-related' neurons can already be found in the primary visual cortex area, although few in numbers. They become more frequent in areas middle temporal and medial superior temporal in the superior temporal sulcus, and comprise almost 50% of all neurons in area visual posterior sylvian (VPS) in the posterior part of the lateral sulcus. In summary, our findings suggest that our ability to perceive a visual world, which is stable despite self-motion, is based on a neuronal network, which culminates in the VPS located in the lateral sulcus below the classical dorsal stream of visual processing.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Neuronas/citología , Corteza Visual/citología , Vías Visuales/citología , Animales , Electrofisiología , Haplorrinos , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA