Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
1.
Clin Psychol Psychother ; 23(2): 176-82, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25994922

RESUMEN

Blushing-fearful individuals often expect that others will judge them negatively. In two studies, we tested if this could be explained by having relatively strict beliefs about what is appropriate social behaviour. Study 1 used a student sample (n = 74), whereas study 2 compared a clinical treatment-seeking sample of blushing-fearful individuals (n = 33) with a non-anxious control group (n = 31). In both studies, participants were asked to read descriptions of common behaviours that could be considered as breaching the prevailing social norms but not necessarily so. Participants indicated (i) to what extent they considered these behaviours as violating the prevailing norm and (ii) their expectation of observers' judgments. Study 1 showed that strict norms were indeed related to fear of blushing and that the tendency of fearful participants to expect negative judgments could at least partly explain this relationship. Study 2 showed that high-fearful and low-fearful individuals do indeed differ in the strictness of their norms and that especially the norms that individuals apply to themselves might be relevant. These findings may provide fresh clues for improving available treatment options. KEY PRACTITIONER MESSAGES: Blushing-fearful individuals attribute relatively strict social norms to other people about which behaviours are appropriate and which are not and have stricter personal norms as well. Blushing-fearful individuals' tendency to expect overly negative judgments in ambivalent social situations can partly be explained by their relatively strict social norms. Having relatively strict social norms may (also) explain why blushing-fearful individuals report to blush often and intensely. It may be worthwhile to address strict social norms in therapy for fear of blushing.


Asunto(s)
Sonrojo/psicología , Miedo/psicología , Juicio , Prejuicio/psicología , Normas Sociales , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Países Bajos , Conducta Social , Estudiantes/psicología , Adulto Joven
2.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry ; 82: 101908, 2024 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37690886

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Although cognitive behavioral group therapy (CBGT) is an effective treatment for social anxiety disorder, many socially anxious patients are still symptomatic after treatment. A possible improvement for CBGT could come from the more experiential group psychotherapy, psychodrama (PD). The integration of CBGT and PD (labeled CBPT) might offer an even more effective treatment than CBGT or PD alone. With the present study, we investigated first whether three kinds of group therapy (CBGT, PD, and CBPT) are superior to a waitlist (WL). Second, we investigated whether CBPT is more effective than CBGT or PD alone. METHODS: One hundred and forty-four social anxiety patients were randomly assigned to three active conditions or a WL. After wait, WL-participants were randomized over the active treatment conditions. RESULTS: The results of a multilevel analysis showed that all treatments were superior to WL in reducing social anxiety complaints. Only CBGT and CBPT differed significantly from WL in reducing fear of negative evaluations. There were no significant differences between active conditions in any of the variables after treatment and after six-month follow up, neither were there significant differences in treatment dropout. LIMITATIONS: First there is the lack of a long-term follow-up. Second, because of loss of participants, we did not reach the planned numbers in the active treatment groups in comparison to WL. Moreover, this study was not designed as a non-inferiority or equivalence trial. CONCLUSIONS: Although the integrative CBPT showed good results, it was not more effective than the other treatments.


Asunto(s)
Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual , Fobia Social , Psicodrama , Psicoterapia de Grupo , Humanos , Fobia Social/terapia , Fobia Social/psicología , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Resultado del Tratamiento , Psicoterapia de Grupo/métodos , Cognición
3.
Heliyon ; 10(3): e24587, 2024 Feb 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38317896

RESUMEN

Past research investigating the relation between social anxiety (SA), empathy and emotion recognition is marked by conceptual and methodological issues. In the present study, we aim to overcome these limitations by examining whether individuals with high (n = 40) vs. low (n = 43) social anxiety differed across these two facets of empathy and whether this could be related to their recognition of emotions. We employed a naturalistic emotion recognition paradigm in which participants watched short videos of individuals (targets) sharing authentic emotional experiences. After each video, we measured self-reported empathic concern and distress, as well as their ability to recognize the emotions expressed by the targets in the videos. Our results show that individuals with high social anxiety recognized the targets' emotions less accurately. Furthermore, high socially anxious individuals reported more personal distress than low socially anxious individuals, whereas no significant difference was found for empathic concern. The findings suggest that reduced recognition of emotions among SA individuals can be better explained by the negative effects of social stress than by a general deficit in empathy.

4.
Clin Psychol Psychother ; 19(6): 481-7, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21751296

RESUMEN

UNLABELLED: The clinical impression is that people who fear blushing do not easily seek psychological help for their complaints. Therefore, we designed a low-threshold psychoeducational group intervention to reduce fear of blushing. The intervention followed a cognitive-behavioural approach, but in a course setting, e.g., with 'participants' and 'teachers' instead of 'patients' and 'therapists'. The effectiveness of the course in reducing fear of blushing and social anxiety was tested in a group of blushing-fearful individuals (n = 47) by using an uncontrolled study design. The course consisted of six weekly sessions and one booster session 3 months after the last regular session. Assessments took place upon application, immediately before the intervention, after the sixth session, before the booster session, and at 1-year follow-up. Results showed that the course was effective in reducing fear of blushing as well as symptoms of social anxiety. The positive effect of the course on anxiety measures suggests that it might be a promising approach for treating fear of blushing. KEY PRACTITIONER MESSAGE: The course 'dealing with fear of blushing' is a cognitive-behavioural group intervention in a course setting, e.g., with 'participants' and 'teachers' instead of 'patients' and 'therapists'. The course was effective in reducing anxiety complaints. An effect size of 1.4 and a reduction of approximately 30 points on this Blushing, Trembling and Sweating Questionnaire are comparable with what was reported for individual cognitive-behavioural treatments. Participants evaluated the course positively.


Asunto(s)
Sonrojo/psicología , Terapia Cognitivo-Conductual/métodos , Trastornos Fóbicos/rehabilitación , Psicoterapia de Grupo/métodos , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Entrevista Psicológica , Masculino , Manuales como Asunto , Análisis Multivariante , Países Bajos , Aceptación de la Atención de Salud
5.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry ; 70: 101599, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32758672

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Negative beliefs about other's judgments play an important role in the development and maintenance of social anxiety disorder. The present experiment examined the effects of role-playing followed by role reversal compared to role-playing twice on altering these negative cognitions. METHODS: Thirty-six adult social anxiety patients were randomized into two conditions: a role-playing condition in which 18 participants role-played an anxiety-provoking social situation twice, or a role reversal condition in which 18 participants role-played an anxiety-provoking social situation followed by enacting the same situation using role reversal. Before the start of the experiment, patients were asked to report their negative cognitions about the other's judgments. Next, they were asked to rate the believability of these negative cognitions, as well as the probability and cost estimates of negative judgments by the other person, at three time-points: before the first block of role-playing, after the first block of role-playing, and after the second block of the experiment. RESULTS: Results demonstrated that role-playing followed by role reversal had a stronger effect on the most negative cognitions than role-playing twice. LIMITATIONS: The most important limitation of the present study is that there was no control group to assess the effects of role-playing alone. Moreover, the second block of the experiment was repetitive in role-playing, however, it was a new task in role reversal. CONCLUSIONS: The results support the hypothesis that role reversal is an effective technique that can be used to correct negative cognitions about other's judgments in SAD.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Cognición , Juicio , Fobia Social/psicología , Desempeño de Papel , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
6.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol ; 53: 25-33, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34358819

RESUMEN

Research has shown that patients with a social anxiety disorder (SAD) show social performance deficits. These deficits are a maintaining factor in SAD, as mending social behavior improves interpersonal judgments and reduces social anxiety. Thus finding ways to enhance social behavior is evidently of importance in the treatment of SAD. This double-blind, placebo-controlled study investigated the effect of an intranasal administration of the hormone oxytocin (24 IU) on social behavior and anxious appearance in SAD patients (N = 40) and healthy controls (N = 39). Forty minutes after oxytocin administration participants were submitted to two live social situations (i.e., a waiting room situation and a getting acquainted task). The participants ('self-rated') and observers ('observer-rated') scored participants' social behavior and anxious appearance. Participants also rated their positive and negative affect. Confirming the social performance deficits in SAD, observers regarded SAD patients as more anxious and less socially skilled than healthy controls. Results indicated oxytocin-induced improvement of observer-rated social behavior in SAD patients compared to placebo but only in the getting acquainted task. This effect was not perceived as such by patients themselves and did not improve their affect ratings. In conclusion, this study found support for the idea that oxytocin helps SAD patients to perform better in social interactions, although this improvement seemed context-dependent (i.e., only present in the getting-acquainted task) and 'not perceived by the patient.


Asunto(s)
Oxitocina , Fobia Social , Ansiedad , Miedo , Humanos , Oxitocina/farmacología , Oxitocina/uso terapéutico , Fobia Social/tratamiento farmacológico , Conducta Social
7.
Clin Psychol Eur ; 2(1): e2693, 2020 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36397983

RESUMEN

Background: Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is generally considered to be the most effective psychological treatment for social anxiety disorder (SAD). Nevertheless, many patients with SAD are still symptomatic after treatment. The present pilot study aimed to examine integrating CBT, with a focus on cognitive and behavioral techniques, and psychodrama, which focuses more on experiential techniques into a combined treatment (CBPT) for social anxious patients in a group format. This new intervention for SAD is described session-by-session. Method: Five adult female patients diagnosed with social anxiety disorder participated in a twelve-session CBPT in a group format. Pretest and posttest scores of social anxiety, avoidance, spontaneity, cost and probability estimates of negative social events, depression, and quality of life were compared, as were weekly assessments of fear of negative evaluation. Results: Results demonstrated a significant reduction of the fear of negative evaluation and social anxiety symptoms. It is noteworthy that also the scores of the probability and cost estimates decreased. However, there were no significant differences between pre and post measures in any of other measures. Conclusion: The current study suggests that group CBPT might be an effective treatment for SAD. However, our sample size was small and this was an uncontrolled study. Therefore, it is necessary to test this intervention in a randomized controlled trial with follow-up assessments.

8.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry ; 69: 101574, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32470686

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: This study aimed to unravel the relationship between socially anxious individuals' expectation of being (dis)liked and actual likeability by looking at the mediating role of both strategic and automatic social behavior: Self-disclosure as well as mimicry were examined. METHOD: Female participants (N = 91) with various levels of social anxiety participated in a social task with a confederate. Before the task, participants indicated their expectation of being liked by the confederate. Afterwards, objective video-observers rated the likeability of the participants before and after the social task as well as their level of self-disclosure and mimicry. RESULTS: Social anxiety correlated negatively with the expectation to be liked but was not related to observer ratings of likeability, self-disclosure or mimicry. However, degree of social anxiety moderated the relation between expectations and self-disclosure. As expected, participants with low levels of social anxiety disclosed more if they expected to be liked. A reversed pattern was found for the high socially anxious participants: Here, higher expectations of being liked were related to less self-disclosure. LIMITATIONS: The study used an analogue female sample. Our social interaction task was highly structured and does not reflect informal day-to-day conversations. CONCLUSION: Socially anxious individuals function rather well in highly structured social tasks. No support was found for declined likeability or disrupted mimicry. Nevertheless, high socially anxious individuals did have a cognitive bias and show a self-protective strategy: when expecting a neutral judgment they reduce their level of self-disclosure. This pattern probably adds to their feelings of social disconnectedness.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Revelación , Conducta Imitativa , Juicio , Conducta Social , Comunicación , Femenino , Humanos , Deseabilidad Social
9.
Emotion ; 9(2): 287-91, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19348542

RESUMEN

This study investigated the remedial value of blushing in the context of clear-cut predicaments. Besides testing the effects of displaying a blush on a neutral expression, we investigated whether blushing increased the remedial properties of shameful and embarrassed expressions. After reading a vignette describing either a transgression (Experiment 1; N = 66) or a mishap (Experiment 2; N = 62), participants saw pictures of people with or without a blush and rated them on several dimensions (e.g., sympathy, trustworthiness). The results of both experiments supported the hypothesis that blushing has remedial properties. In most instances, blushing actors were evaluated more favorably than their nonblushing counterparts. Although people often consider blushing to be an undesirable response, our results showed that, in the context of transgressions and mishaps, blushing is a helpful bodily signal with face-saving properties.


Asunto(s)
Sonrojo/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Comunicación no Verbal , Percepción Social , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Países Bajos , Vergüenza , Deseabilidad Social
10.
J Nonverbal Behav ; 42(1): 81-99, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29497222

RESUMEN

Socially anxiety may be related to a different pattern of facial mimicry and contagion of others' emotions. We report two studies in which participants with different levels of social anxiety reacted to others' emotional displays, either shown on a computer screen (Study 1) or in an actual social interaction (Study 2). Study 1 examined facial mimicry and emotional contagion in response to displays of happiness, anger, fear, and contempt. Participants mimicked negative and positive emotions to some extent, but we found no relation between mimicry and the social anxiety level of the participants. Furthermore, socially anxious individuals were more prone to experience negative emotions and felt more irritated in response to negative emotion displays. In Study 2, we found that social anxiety was related to enhanced mimicry of smiling, but this was only the case for polite smiles and not for enjoyment smiles. These results suggest that socially anxious individuals tend to catch negative emotions from others, but suppress their expression by mimicking positive displays. This may be explained by the tendency of socially anxious individuals to avoid conflict or rejection.

11.
Lancet ; 367(9528): 2059, 2006 Jun 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16798386
12.
Behav Res Ther ; 50(2): 158-62, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22206720

RESUMEN

It has been proposed that blushing-fearful individuals overestimate both the probability and the interpersonal costs of blushing. To study these judgmental biases, we presented a treatment-seeking sample of blushing-fearful individuals a series of vignettes describing social events and tested whether this clinical sample would overestimate the costs and probability of blushing compared to non-fearful controls. To test if blushing-fearfuls overestimate and/or low-fearful individuals underestimate the cost of displaying a blush, a second experiment examined the effects of blushing in these situations on observers' judgments. Experiment 1 showed that blushing-fearfuls indeed have judgmental biases for the probability and costs of blushing. Experiment 2 showed that the observers' judgments were very similar to the judgments anticipated by the low-fear group in Experiment 1. Thus the judgmental biases that were evident in the high-fearfuls can be best interpreted as an overestimation of the social costs of displaying a blush. These findings help improving our understanding of the mechanisms that may drive blushing phobia and also point to the clinical implication that it might be worthwhile to challenge blushing-fearfuls' judgmental biases.


Asunto(s)
Sonrojo/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Juicio , Trastornos Fóbicos/psicología , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Probabilidad , Conducta Social
13.
J Psychopathol Behav Assess ; 33(4): 540-546, 2011 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22081745

RESUMEN

To explain fear of blushing, it has been proposed that individuals with fear of blushing overestimate the social costs of their blushing. Current information-processing models emphasize the relevance of differentiating between more automatic and more explicit cognitions, as both types of cognitions may independently influence behavior. The present study tested whether individuals with fear of blushing expect blushing to have more negative social consequences than controls, both on an explicit level and on a more automatic level. Automatic associations between blushing and social costs were assessed in a treatment-seeking sample of individuals with fear of blushing who met DSM-IV criteria for social anxiety disorder (n = 49) and a non-anxious control group (n = 27) using a single-target Implicit Association Test (stIAT). In addition, participants' explicit expectations about the social costs of their blushing were assessed. Individuals with fear of blushing showed stronger associations between blushing and negative outcomes, as indicated by both stIAT and self-report. The findings support the view that automatic and explicit associations between blushing and social costs may both help to enhance our understanding of the cognitive processes that underlie fear of blushing.

14.
Emotion ; 11(2): 313-9, 2011 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21500900

RESUMEN

This study examined whether blushing after a sociomoral transgression remediates trustworthiness in an interdependent context. Participants (N = 196) played a computerized prisoner's dilemma game with a virtual opponent who defected in the second round of the game. After the defection, a photograph of the opponent was shown, displaying a blushing or a nonblushing face. In a subsequent Trust Task, the blushing opponent was entrusted with more money than the nonblushing opponent. In further support of the alleged remedial properties of the blush, participants also indicated that they trusted the blushing opponent more, expected a lower probability that she would defect again, and judged the blushing opponent more positively.


Asunto(s)
Sonrojo/psicología , Confianza/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Juegos Experimentales , Procesos de Grupo , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Joven
15.
Behav Res Ther ; 48(10): 984-91, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20638046

RESUMEN

Patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD) not only fear negative evaluation but are indeed less likeable than people without SAD. Previous research shows social performance to mediate this social anxiety-social rejection relationship. This study studied two pathways hypothesized to lead to poor social performance in social anxiety: increased self-focused attention and negative beliefs. State social anxiety was experimentally manipulated in high and low-blushing-fearful individuals by letting half of the participants believe that they blushed intensely during a 5 min getting-acquainted interaction with two confederates. Participants rated their state social anxiety, self-focused attention, and level of negative beliefs. Two confederates and two video-observers rated subsequently likeability (i.e., social rejection) and social performance of the participants. In both groups, the social anxiety-social rejection relationship was present. Although state social anxiety was related to heightened self-focused attention and negative beliefs, only negative beliefs were associated with relatively poor social performance. In contrast to current SAD models, self-focused attention did not play a key-role in poor social performance but seemed to function as a by-product of state social anxiety. Beliefs of being negatively evaluated seem to elicit changes in behavioral repertoire resulting in a poor social performance and subsequent rejection.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Rechazo en Psicología , Autoimagen , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Percepción Social , Adulto , Atención , Concienciación , Sonrojo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Modelos Psicológicos , Distancia Psicológica , Valores de Referencia , Deseabilidad Social
16.
J Psychopathol Behav Assess ; 32(2): 264-270, 2010 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20421905

RESUMEN

The present study examines two mechanisms that might explain why blushing-fearful individuals fear blushing: Judgmental biases for blushing in ordinary social situations that usually do not elicit a blush, and negative conditional cognitions about blushing irrespective of situation. A web-based self-report measure, linked to a German internet forum for people with fear of blushing, was completed by a group of high blushing-fearful participants (n = 155) and a low fear group (n = 61). Supporting the idea that cognitive biases are involved in fear of blushing, blushing-fearful participants showed inflated estimates of both the probability and the costs of blushing in these situations. In addition, blushing-fearful individuals were characterized by relatively negative conditional cognitions about blushing.

17.
Behav Res Ther ; 47(7): 541-7, 2009 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19358973

RESUMEN

In the present study, we investigate whether people attribute costs to displaying a blush. Individuals with and without fear of blushing were invited to have a short conversation with two confederates. During the conversation, half of the individuals received the feedback that they were blushing intensely. The study tested whether the belief that one is blushing leads to the anticipation that one will be judged negatively. In addition, the set-up permitted the actual physiological blush response to be investigated. In line with the model that we propose for erythrophobia, participants in the feedback condition expected the confederates to judge them relatively negatively, independent of their fear of blushing. Furthermore, sustaining the idea that believing that one will blush can act as a self-fulfilling prophecy, high-fearfuls showed relatively intense facial coloration in both conditions, whereas low-fearfuls only showed enhanced blush responses following false blush feedback.


Asunto(s)
Sonrojo/psicología , Autoimagen , Percepción Social , Sonrojo/fisiología , Miedo/psicología , Retroalimentación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA