Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 56
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
BMC Psychiatry ; 23(1): 920, 2023 12 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38066477

RESUMEN

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is the most prevalent neuropsychiatric disorder in the world. Currently, the diagnosis is based mainly on interviews, resulting in uncertainties in the clinical assessment. While some neuropsychological tests are used, their specificity and selectivity are low, and more reliable biomarkers are desirable. Previous research indicates that ADHD is associated with morphological changes in the cerebellum, which is essential for motor ability and timing. Here, we compared 29 children diagnosed with ADHD to 96 age-matched controls on prism adaptation, eyeblink conditioning, and timed motor performance in a finger tapping task. Prism adaptation and timing precision in the finger tapping task, but not performance on eyeblink conditioning, differed between the ADHD and control groups, as well as between children with and without Deficits in Attention, Motor control, and Perception (DAMP) - a more severe form of ADHD. The results suggest finger tapping can be used as a cheap, objective, and unbiased biomarker to complement current diagnostic procedures.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad , Niño , Humanos , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Cerebelo , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
2.
BMC Public Health ; 22(1): 1554, 2022 08 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35971092

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Anxiety and depression are amongst the most prevalent mental health problems. Their pattern of comorbidity may inform about their etiology and effective treatment, but such research is sparse. Here, we document long-term prognosis of affective caseness (high probability of being a clinical case) of anxiety and depression, their comorbidity, and a no-caseness condition at three time-points across six years, and identify the most common prognoses of these four conditions. METHODS: Longitudinal population-based data were collected from 1,837 participants in 2010, 2013 and 2016. Based on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale they formed the four groups of anxiety, depression and comorbidity caseness, and no caseness at baseline. RESULTS: The three-year associations show that it was most common to recover when being an anxiety, depression or comorbidity caseness (36.8 - 59.4%), and when not being a caseness to remain so (89.2%). It was also rather common to remain in the same caseness condition after three years (18.7 - 39.1%). In comorbidity it was more likely to recover from depression (21.1%) than from anxiety (5.4%), and being no caseness it was more likely to develop anxiety (5.9%) than depression (1.7%). The most common six-year prognoses were recovering from the affective caseness conditions at 3-year follow-up (YFU), and remain recovered at 6-YFU, and as no caseness to remain so across the six years. The second most common prognoses in the affective conditions were to remain as caseness at both 3-YFU and 6-YFU, and in no caseness to remain so at 3-YFU, but develop anxiety at 6-YFU. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that only 37 - 60% of individuals in the general population with high probability of being a clinical case with anxiety, depression, and their comorbidity will recover within a three-year period, and that it is rather common to remain with these affective conditions after 6 years. These poor prognoses, for comorbidity in particular, highlight the need for intensified alertness of their prevalence and enabling treatment in the general population.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad , Depresión , Adulto , Ansiedad/psicología , Comorbilidad , Depresión/psicología , Humanos , Pronóstico , Estudios Prospectivos
3.
J Relig Health ; 61(1): 6-22, 2022 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34338953

RESUMEN

Schizophrenia is correlated with religious delusions but, heretofore, the relationship between schizophrenia prevalence and religiosity has not been explored at the national level. Examining this relationship, we find that national level schizophrenia prevalence is correlated with national level religiosity and strongly negatively correlated with national level atheism across 125 countries. When controlling for cognitive performance and economic development in multiple regression analyses, the proportion of the variance explained was 2.9% (p < .005) for Religiousness and 5.1% for Atheism (p < .00005). Alternative causal interpretations of this association are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Esquizofrenia , Deluciones , Humanos , Prevalencia , Religión , Esquizofrenia/epidemiología
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 238(4): 1051-1060, 2020 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32206850

RESUMEN

Associations between cognitive and motor timing performance are documented in hundreds of studies. A core finding is a correlation of about - 0.3 to - 0.5 between psychometric intelligence and time interval production variability and reaction time, but the nature of the relationship remains unclear. Here, we investigated whether this relation is subject to near and far transfer across a battery of cognitive and timing tasks. These tasks were administered pre- and post-five daily 30 min sessions of sensorimotor synchronization training with feedback for every interval. The training group exhibited increased sustained attention performance in Conners' Continuous Performance Test II, but no change in the block design and figure weights subtests from the WAIS-IV. A passive control group exhibited no change in performance on any of the timing or cognitive tests. These findings provide evidence for a direct involvement of sustained attention in motor timing as well as near transfer from synchronization to unpaced serial interval production. Implications for the timing-cognition relationship are discussed in light of various putative timing mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiología , Inteligencia/fisiología , Práctica Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Escalas de Wechsler , Adulto Joven
5.
J Relig Health ; 59(3): 1567-1579, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31587150

RESUMEN

Numerous studies have found a negative relationship between religiousness and IQ. It is in the region of - 0.2, according to meta-analyses. The reasons for this relationship are, however, unknown. It has been suggested that higher intelligence leads to greater attraction to science, or that it helps to override evolved cognitive dispositions such as for religiousness. Either way, such explanations assume that the religion-IQ nexus is on general intelligence (g), rather than some subset of specialized cognitive abilities. In other words, they assume it is a Jensen effect. Two large datasets comparing groups with different levels of religiousness show that their IQ differences are not on g and must, therefore, be attributed to specialized abilities. An analysis of the specialized abilities on which the religious and non-religious groups differ reveals no clear pattern. We cautiously suggest that this may be explicable in terms of autism spectrum disorder traits among people with high IQ scores, because such traits are negatively associated with religiousness.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Inteligencia , Religión , Espiritualidad , Humanos
6.
J Med Internet Res ; 20(10): e10754, 2018 10 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30305255

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Low adherence to recommended treatments is a multifactorial problem for patients in rehabilitation after myocardial infarction (MI). In a nationwide trial of internet-delivered cognitive behavior therapy (iCBT) for the high-risk subgroup of patients with MI also reporting symptoms of anxiety, depression, or both (MI-ANXDEP), adherence was low. Since low adherence to psychotherapy leads to a waste of therapeutic resources and risky treatment abortion in MI-ANXDEP patients, identifying early predictors for adherence is potentially valuable for effective targeted care. OBJECTIVES: The goal of the research was to use supervised machine learning to investigate both established and novel predictors for iCBT adherence in MI-ANXDEP patients. METHODS: Data were from 90 MI-ANXDEP patients recruited from 25 hospitals in Sweden and randomized to treatment in the iCBT trial Uppsala University Psychosocial Care Programme (U-CARE) Heart study. Time point of prediction was at completion of the first homework assignment. Adherence was defined as having completed more than 2 homework assignments within the 14-week treatment period. A supervised machine learning procedure was applied to identify the most potent predictors for adherence available at the first treatment session from a range of demographic, clinical, psychometric, and linguistic predictors. The internal binary classifier was a random forest model within a 3×10-fold cross-validated recursive feature elimination (RFE) resampling which selected the final predictor subset that best differentiated adherers versus nonadherers. RESULTS: Patient mean age was 58.4 years (SD 9.4), 62% (56/90) were men, and 48% (43/90) were adherent. Out of the 34 potential predictors for adherence, RFE selected an optimal subset of 56% (19/34; Accuracy 0.64, 95% CI 0.61-0.68, P<.001). The strongest predictors for adherence were, in order of importance, (1) self-assessed cardiac-related fear, (2) sex, and (3) the number of words the patient used to answer the first homework assignment. CONCLUSIONS: For developing and testing effective iCBT interventions, investigating factors that predict adherence is important. Adherence to iCBT for MI-ANXDEP patients in the U-CARE Heart trial was best predicted by cardiac-related fear and sex, consistent with previous research, but also by novel linguistic predictors from written patient behavior which conceivably indicate verbal ability or therapeutic alliance. Future research should investigate potential causal mechanisms and seek to determine what underlying constructs the linguistic predictors tap into. Whether these findings replicate for other interventions outside of Sweden, in larger samples, and for patients with other conditions who are offered iCBT should also be investigated. TRIAL REGISTRATION: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01504191; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01504191 (Archived at Webcite at http://www.webcitation.org/6xWWSEQ22).


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/terapia , Depresión/terapia , Internet/normas , Aprendizaje Automático/normas , Infarto del Miocardio/complicaciones , Infarto del Miocardio/psicología , Psicoterapia/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
7.
Am Heart J ; 191: 12-20, 2017 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28888265

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Psychosocial stress might trigger myocardial infarction (MI). Increased MI incidence coincides with recurrent time periods during the year perceived as particularly stressful in the population. METHODS: A stress-triggering hypothesis on the risk of MI onset was investigated with Swedish population data on MI hospital admission date and symptom onset date (N=156,690; 148,176) as registered from 2006 through 2013 in the national quality registry database Swedish Web-system for Enhancement and Development of Evidence-based care in Heart disease Evaluated According to Recommended Therapies (SWEDEHEART). Poisson regression was applied to analyze daily MI rates during days belonging to the Christmas and New Year holidays, turns of the month, Mondays, weekends, and summer vacation in July compared with remaining control days. RESULTS: Adjusted incidence rate ratios (IRRs) for MI rates were higher during Christmas and New Year holidays (IRR=1.07 [1.04-1.09], P<.001) and on Mondays (IRR=1.11 [1.09-1.13], P<.001) and lower in July (IRR=0.92 [0.90-0.94], P<.001) and over weekends (IRR=0.88 [0.87-0.89], P<.001), yet not during the turns of the month (IRR=1.01 [1.00-1.02], P=.891). These findings were also predominantly robust with symptom onset as alternative outcome, when adjusting for both established and some suggested-but-untested confounders, and in 8 subgroups. CONCLUSIONS: Fluctuations in daily MI incidence rates are systematically related to time periods of presumed psychosocial stress. Further research might clarify mechanisms that are amenable to clinical alteration.


Asunto(s)
Infarto del Miocardio/epidemiología , Sistema de Registros , Estrés Psicológico/complicaciones , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Incidencia , Masculino , Infarto del Miocardio/etiología , Estudios Retrospectivos , Factores de Riesgo , Suecia/epidemiología
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(3): 787-798, 2017 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27885405

RESUMEN

The nature of the relationship between timing and cognition remains poorly understood. Cognitive control is known to be involved in discrete timing tasks involving durations above 1 s, but has not yet been demonstrated for repetitive motor timing below 1 s. We examined the latter in two continuation tapping experiments, by varying the cognitive load in a concurrent task. In Experiment 1, participants repeated a fixed three finger sequence (low executive load) or a pseudorandom sequence (high load) with either 524-, 733-, 1024- or 1431-ms inter-onset intervals (IOIs). High load increased timing variability for 524 and 733-ms IOIs but not for the longer IOIs. Experiment 2 attempted to replicate this finding for a concurrent memory task. Participants retained three letters (low working memory load) or seven letters (high load) while producing intervals (524- and 733-ms IOIs) with a drum stick. High load increased timing variability for both IOIs. Taken together, the experiments demonstrate that cognitive control processes influence sub-second repetitive motor timing.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Aprendizaje Verbal/fisiología , Vocabulario , Adulto Joven
9.
BMC Med Inform Decis Mak ; 17(1): 99, 2017 Jul 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28679442

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Machine learning algorithms hold potential for improved prediction of all-cause mortality in cardiovascular patients, yet have not previously been developed with high-quality population data. This study compared four popular machine learning algorithms trained on unselected, nation-wide population data from Sweden to solve the binary classification problem of predicting survival versus non-survival 2 years after first myocardial infarction (MI). METHODS: This prospective national registry study for prognostic accuracy validation of predictive models used data from 51,943 complete first MI cases as registered during 6 years (2006-2011) in the national quality register SWEDEHEART/RIKS-HIA (90% coverage of all MIs in Sweden) with follow-up in the Cause of Death register (> 99% coverage). Primary outcome was AUROC (C-statistic) performance of each model on the untouched test set (40% of cases) after model development on the training set (60% of cases) with the full (39) predictor set. Model AUROCs were bootstrapped and compared, correcting the P-values for multiple comparisons with the Bonferroni method. Secondary outcomes were derived when varying sample size (1-100% of total) and predictor sets (39, 10, and 5) for each model. Analyses were repeated on 79,869 completed cases after multivariable imputation of predictors. RESULTS: A Support Vector Machine with a radial basis kernel developed on 39 predictors had the highest complete cases performance on the test set (AUROC = 0.845, PPV = 0.280, NPV = 0.966) outperforming Boosted C5.0 (0.845 vs. 0.841, P = 0.028) but not significantly higher than Logistic Regression or Random Forest. Models converged to the point of algorithm indifference with increased sample size and predictors. Using the top five predictors also produced good classifiers. Imputed analyses had slightly higher performance. CONCLUSIONS: Improved mortality prediction at hospital discharge after first MI is important for identifying high-risk individuals eligible for intensified treatment and care. All models performed accurately and similarly and because of the superior national coverage, the best model can potentially be used to better differentiate new patients, allowing for improved targeting of limited resources. Future research should focus on further model development and investigate possibilities for implementation.


Asunto(s)
Infarto del Miocardio/mortalidad , Sistema de Registros , Máquina de Vectores de Soporte , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Algoritmos , Causas de Muerte , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Cardiovasculares , Infarto del Miocardio/clasificación , Pronóstico , Estudios Prospectivos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Medición de Riesgo , Factores de Riesgo , Análisis de Supervivencia , Suecia/epidemiología
10.
J Strength Cond Res ; 31(2): 451-461, 2017 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27893472

RESUMEN

Aasa, U, Paulin, J, and Madison, G. Correspondence between physical self-concept and participation in, and fitness change after, biweekly body conditioning classes in sedentary women. J Strength Cond Res 31(2): 451-461, 2017-The aims of the study were (a) to investigate the effects of participation in low impact body conditioning classes on physical fitness in sedentary women at different ages and (b) to examine the correspondence between physical self-concept and participation in, and fitness change after, the participation. Ninety-two sedentary women (mean age 44.2 years) participated in 11 weeks of biweekly classes that included cardiovascular, strength, core, endurance, and mobility exercises, all performed in synchrony with music. Cardiorespiratory fitness, maximal lifting strength, mobility, and balance tests were performed before and after the exercise period and the short-form of the Physical Self-Description Questionnaire (PSDQ-S) was completed. Zero-order Spearman correlation analyses showed that women who rated the PSDQ-S dimension sport competence higher participated in a larger number of sessions (rs = 0.24, p = 0.040). At posttests, all participants had increased their balance, the participants aged 20-34 years had increased their lifting strength, and the participants aged 35-65 years had increased their cardiorespiratory fitness and mobility. Most PSDQ-S dimensions did not affect performance change, but the perception of being physically active was related to increased cardiovascular fitness. We conclude that women with a sedentary lifestyle who wish to increase their physical capacity benefit from music exercise and that inquiries about perceived sport competence and physical activity can improve recommendations made by strength and conditioning professionals.


Asunto(s)
Ejercicio Físico/fisiología , Ejercicio Físico/psicología , Aptitud Física/fisiología , Aptitud Física/psicología , Autoimagen , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fuerza Muscular/fisiología , Equilibrio Postural/fisiología , Estudios Prospectivos , Conducta Sedentaria , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
11.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e9, 2017 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28327219

RESUMEN

Why is social bias and its depressing effects on low-status or low-performing groups exaggerated? We show that the higher intelligence of academics has at best a very weak effect on reducing their bias, facilitates superficially justifying their biases, and may make them better at understanding the benefits of social conformity in general and competitive altruism specifically. We foresee a surge in research examining these mechanisms and recommend, meanwhile, reviving and better observing scientific ideals.


Asunto(s)
Altruismo , Percepción Social , Sesgo , Inteligencia , Conformidad Social
12.
Dev Sci ; 19(3): 504-12, 2016 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25939545

RESUMEN

The idea of far transfer effects in the cognitive sciences has received much attention in recent years. One domain where far transfer effects have frequently been reported is music education, with the prevailing idea that music practice entails an increase in cognitive ability (IQ). While cross-sectional studies consistently find significant associations between music practice and IQ, randomized controlled trials, however, report mixed results. An alternative to the hypothesis of cognitive transfer effects is that some underlying factors, such as shared genes, influence practice behaviour and IQ causing associations on the phenotypic level. Here we explored the hypothesis of far transfer within the framework of music practice. A co-twin control design combined with classical twin-modelling based on a sample of more than 10,500 twins was used to explore causal associations between music practice and IQ as well as underlying genetic and environmental influences. As expected, phenotypic associations were moderate (r = 0.11 and r = 0.10 for males and females, respectively). However, the relationship disappeared when controlling for genetic and shared environmental influences using the co-twin control method, indicating that a highly practiced twin did not have higher IQ than the untrained co-twin. In line with that finding, the relationship between practice and IQ was mostly due to shared genetic influences. Findings strongly suggest that associations between music practice and IQ in the general population are non-causal in nature. The implications of the present findings for research on plasticity, modularity, and transfer are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Pleiotropía Genética , Inteligencia/fisiología , Música , Adulto , Estudios de Cohortes , Femenino , Humanos , Inteligencia/genética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis Multivariante , Factores de Tiempo , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Dicigóticos/psicología , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/psicología
13.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 19(2): 87-96, 2016 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26948461

RESUMEN

Males and females score differently on some personality traits, but the underlying etiology of these differences is not well understood. This study examined genetic, environmental, and prenatal hormonal influences on individual differences in personality masculinity-femininity (M-F). We used Big-Five personality inventory data of 9,520 Swedish twins (aged 27 to 54) to create a bipolar M-F personality scale. Using biometrical twin modeling, we estimated the influence of genetic and environmental factors on individual differences in a M-F personality score. Furthermore, we tested whether prenatal hormone transfer may influence individuals' M-F scores by comparing the scores of twins with a same-sex versus those with an opposite-sex co-twin. On average, males scored 1.09 standard deviations higher than females on the created M-F scale. Around a third of the variation in M-F personality score was attributable to genetic factors, while family environmental factors had no influence. Males and females from opposite-sex pairs scored significantly more masculine (both approximately 0.1 SD) than those from same-sex pairs. In conclusion, genetic influences explain part of the individual differences in personality M-F, and hormone transfer from the male to the female twin during pregnancy may increase the level of masculinization in females. Additional well-powered studies are needed to clarify this association and determine the underlying mechanisms in both sexes.


Asunto(s)
Feminidad , Masculinidad , Trastornos de la Personalidad/genética , Personalidad/genética , Adulto , Ambiente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos de la Personalidad/fisiopatología , Embarazo , Caracteres Sexuales , Suecia , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética
14.
Behav Brain Sci ; 39: e11, 2016 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26948728

RESUMEN

The gods of monotheistic religions, which began amongst pastoralists and defeated exiles, are closer to Big Gods than those associated with ancient city-based polities. The development of Big Gods is contingent upon a need to reduce uncertainty and negative feelings in combination with a relatively high level of prosociality, rather than a need to induce or assess prosociality.


Asunto(s)
Religión , Incertidumbre
15.
Psychol Sci ; 25(9): 1795-803, 2014 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25079217

RESUMEN

The relative importance of nature and nurture for various forms of expertise has been intensely debated. Music proficiency is viewed as a general model for expertise, and associations between deliberate practice and music proficiency have been interpreted as supporting the prevailing idea that long-term deliberate practice inevitably results in increased music ability. Here, we examined the associations (rs = .18-.36) between music practice and music ability (rhythm, melody, and pitch discrimination) in 10,500 Swedish twins. We found that music practice was substantially heritable (40%-70%). Associations between music practice and music ability were predominantly genetic, and, contrary to the causal hypothesis, nonshared environmental influences did not contribute. There was no difference in ability within monozygotic twin pairs differing in their amount of practice, so that when genetic predisposition was controlled for, more practice was no longer associated with better music skills. These findings suggest that music practice may not causally influence music ability and that genetic variation among individuals affects both ability and inclination to practice.


Asunto(s)
Aptitud , Música , Práctica Psicológica , Gemelos Dicigóticos , Gemelos Monocigóticos , Adulto , Causalidad , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
16.
J Clin Nurs ; 22(9-10): 1203-16, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23574287

RESUMEN

AIMS AND OBJECTIVES: To provide a meta-analysis of the effects of music interventions on patients with dementia, separating, for the first time, between different types of interventions and different outcome measures, namely affective, behavioural, cognitive and physiological. BACKGROUND: Music therapy is an attractive form of intervention for the growing number of demented patients, for whom pharmacological interventions are not always effective and may lead to undesired side effects. While music is more frequently applied in clinical settings for each year, no meta-analysis has considered effects of music interventions on affective, behavioural, cognitive and physiological outcomes separately. DESIGN: A standard meta-analysis approach was applied. METHODS: We include all original studies found for the key words music and dementia. Mean effect sizes and confidence intervals are computed from study effect sizes according to standard methods, and these are considered for various common types of music interventions separately. RESULTS: Nineteen studies with a total of 478 dementia patients exhibit effect sizes ranging from 0·04-4·56 (M = 1·04). Many of these indicate large positive effects on behavioural, cognitive and physiological outcome measures, and medium effects on affective measures. CONCLUSIONS: Music interventions seem to be effective and have the potential of increasing the quality of life for patients with dementia. Many studies in this area suffer from poor methodological quality, which limits the reach of meta-analysis and the strength and generalisability of these conclusions. RELEVANCE TO CLINICAL PRACTICE: Being inexpensive and largely without adverse side effects, current knowledge seems to indicate that music interventions can be recommended for patients in all stages of dementia.


Asunto(s)
Demencia/terapia , Musicoterapia , Humanos , Resultado del Tratamiento
17.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(3): 217-8, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23663456

RESUMEN

The target article focuses on the predictive coding of "what" and "where" something happened and the "where" and "what" response to make. We extend that scope by addressing the "when" aspect of perception and action. Successful interaction with the environment requires predictions of everything from millisecond-accurate motor timing to far future events. The hierarchical framework seems appropriate for timing.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Ciencia Cognitiva/tendencias , Percepción/fisiología , Humanos
18.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 240: 104043, 2023 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37804701

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Understanding individual differences in psychology, and how they relate to specific addictions, may allow society to better identify those at most risk and even enact policies to ameliorate them. Internet addiction is a growing health concern, a research focus of which is to understand individual differences and the psychology of those most susceptible to developing it. Western countries are strongly overrepresented in this regard. METHOD: Here, sex and national differences in internet addiction are measured, using Young's 'Internet Addiction Test,' in two non-Western countries, Egypt and Saudi Arabia. >800 students aged 18 and 35 years (M = 20.65, SD = 1.48) completed a multidimensional internet addiction instrument. The instrument measures traits such as Withdrawal and Social Problems, Time Management and Performance and Reality Substitute. RESULTS: Analyses revealed that males scored higher than females and Saudis higher than Egyptians on nearly all scales, including the total score. Factor analysis of the 20-item instrument revealed three factors, all exhibiting sex and culture differences. CONCLUSIONS: These findings add to the body of evidence that males are higher than females in problematic internet use, as they are in addictive behaviors in general. Our findings may also imply that restrictions on male-female interaction, which are more pronounced in Saudi Arabia, may elevate the prevalence of internet addiction. The internet is also easier and cheaper to access in Saudi Arabia than in Egypt.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Adictiva , Trastorno de Adicción a Internet , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Arabia Saudita/epidemiología , Egipto/epidemiología , Conducta Adictiva/epidemiología , Conducta Adictiva/psicología , Estudiantes/psicología , Internet
19.
Behav Brain Sci ; 35(6): 440-1, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23164259

RESUMEN

Human behavior is guided by evolutionarily shaped brain mechanisms that make statistical predictions based on limited information. Such mechanisms are important for facilitating interpersonal relationships, avoiding dangers, and seizing opportunities in social interaction. We thus suggest that it is essential for analyses of prejudice and prejudice reduction to take the predictive accuracy and adaptivity of the studied prejudices into account.


Asunto(s)
Procesos de Grupo , Relaciones Interpersonales , Prejuicio , Identificación Social , Humanos
20.
Scientometrics ; 127(10): 6029-6046, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35990404

RESUMEN

Timely dissemination of knowledge is essential and fosters development of knowledge both within academe and the wider society, not least for knowledge that serves practises outside of academia. Here, we compare six disciplines which provide health-related knowledge that serve the health and social services. Most previous research compares the size and impact of the body of publications belonging to each discipline, which ignores the distribution of seniority, productivity, and impact amongst researchers. Instead, we consider the whole population of academics in Sweden employed or active within each discipline, including those who have nil publications. The disciplines form three clusters, where researchers in Public Health and Nursing and Caring science claim about 15 articles per author, Psychology about 10, and Education, Sociology and Social Work less than four. Their numbers of citations follow the same pattern, and are substantially correlated with the number of articles. Tenured or full professors had about 50% more publications and citations per publication than had associate professors. The distributions indicate clear modes at 0, 4, and 16 publications for each cluster, and provide the proportions of researchers within each discipline who have no such publications at all. We discuss the implications of these results for policy, practice, and knowledge quality in the social services and the welfare sector.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA