RESUMEN
We report two experiments using the artificial-grammar task that demonstrate order dependence in implicit learning. Studying grammatical training strings in different orders did not affect participants' discrimination of grammatical from ungrammatical test strings, but it did affect their judgments about specific test strings. Current accounts of learning in the artificial-grammar task focus on category-level discrimination and largely ignore item-level discrimination. Hence, the results highlight the importance of moving theory from a category- to an item-level of analysis and point to a new way to evaluate and to refine accounts of implicit learning.
Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Lingüística , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , MasculinoRESUMEN
In artificial grammar learning experiments, participants study strings of letters constructed using a grammar and then sort novel grammatical test exemplars from novel ungrammatical ones. The ability to distinguish grammatical from ungrammatical strings is often taken as evidence that the participants have induced the rules of the grammar. We show that judgements of grammaticality are predicted by the local redundancy of the test strings, not by grammaticality itself. The prediction holds in a transfer test in which test strings involve different letters than the training strings. Local redundancy is usually confounded with grammaticality in stimuli widely used in the literature. The confounding explains why the ability to distinguish grammatical from ungrammatical strings has popularized the idea that participants have induced the rules of the grammar, when they have not. We discuss the judgement of grammaticality task in terms of attribute substitution and pattern goodness. When asked to judge grammaticality (an inaccessible attribute), participants answer an easier question about pattern goodness (an accessible attribute).
Asunto(s)
Teoría de la Información , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología/fisiología , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Lingüística , Modelos TeóricosRESUMEN
The permutation test follows directly from the procedure in a comparative experiment, does not depend on a known distribution for error, and is sometimes more sensitive to real effects than are the corresponding parametric tests. Despite its advantages, the permutation test is seldom (if ever) applied to factorial designs because of the computational load that they impose. We propose two methods to limit the computation load. We show, first, that orthogonal contrasts limit the computational load and, second, that when combined with Gill's (2007) algorithm, the factorial permutation test is both practical and efficient. For within-subjects designs, the factorial permutation test is equivalent to an ANOVA when the latter's assumptions have been met. For between-subjects designs, the factorial test is conservative. Code to execute the routines described in this article may be downloaded from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.
Asunto(s)
Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Psicología Experimental/métodos , Proyectos de Investigación/estadística & datos numéricos , Algoritmos , Humanos , Programas InformáticosRESUMEN
The authors examined priming within the test sequence in 3 recognition memory experiments. A probe primed its successor whenever both probes shared a feature with the same studied item (interjacent priming), indicating that the study item like the probe is central to the decision. Interjacent priming occurred even when the 2 probes did not themselves share any features: A lure that shared a single feature with a study item primed a lure that shared a different feature with the same study item. The experiments distinguished interjacent priming from other types of facilitation. Interjacent priming indicates that a study item that is like the probe is more relevant to the decision than other study items, contrary to global memory models. It also shows that negative decisions depend on contradiction, not insufficient familiarity, because lures, as well as targets, benefited. The data are discussed in terms of a recall check within a dual-process theory, but the authors prefer a single-process resonance model with separate decision mechanisms for yes and no responses (D. J. K. Mewhort & E. E. Johns, 2005).
Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Aprendizaje Seriado/fisiología , Atención , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Formación de Concepto/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiologíaRESUMEN
To account for natural variability in cognitive processing, it is standard practice to optimize a model's parameters by fitting it to behavioral data. Although most language-related theories acknowledge a large role for experience in language processing, variability reflecting that knowledge is usually ignored when evaluating a model's fit to representative data. We fit language-based behavioral data using experiential optimization, a method that optimizes the materials that a model is given while retaining the learning and processing mechanisms of standard practice. Rather than using default materials, experiential optimization selects the optimal linguistic sources to create a memory representation that maximizes task performance. We demonstrate performance on multiple benchmark tasks by optimizing the experience on which a model's representation is based.
Asunto(s)
Memoria , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicolingüística , Semántica , HumanosRESUMEN
Recall decreases across a series of subspan immediate-recall trials but rebounds if the semantic category of the words is changed, an example of release from proactive interference (RPI). The size of the rebound depends on the semantic categories used and ranges from 0% to 95%. We used a corpus of novels to create vectors representing the meaning of about 40,000 words using the BEAGLE algorithm. The distance between categories and spread within categories jointly predicted the size of the RPI. We used a holographic model for recall equipped with a lexicon of BEAGLE vectors representing the meaning of words. The model captured RPI using a hologram as an interface to bridge information from episodic and semantic memory; it is the first account of RPI to capture release at the level of individual words in categorized lists.
Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicolingüística , Semántica , HumanosRESUMEN
People remember words that they read aloud better than words that they read silently, a result known as the production effect. The standing explanation for the production effect is that producing a word renders it distinctive in memory and, thus, memorable at test. By 1 key account, distinctiveness is defined in terms of sensory feedback. We formalize the sensory-feedback account using MINERVA 2, a standard model of memory. The model accommodates the basic result in recognition as well as the fact that the mixed-list production effect is larger than its pure-list counterpart, that the production effect is robust to forgetting, and that the production and generation effects have additive influences on performance. A final simulation addresses the strength-based account and suggests that it will be more difficult to distinguish a strength-based versus distinctiveness-based explanation than is typically thought. We conclude that the production effect is consistent with existing theory and discuss our analysis in relation to Alan Newell's (1973) classic criticism of psychology and call for an analysis of psychological principles instead of laboratory phenomena. (PsycINFO Database Record
Asunto(s)
Memoria Episódica , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Lectura , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , HumanosRESUMEN
Studies of implicit learning often examine peoples' sensitivity to sequential structure. Computational accounts have evolved to reflect this bias. An experiment conducted by Neil and Higham [Neil, G. J., & Higham, P. A.(2012). Implicit learning of conjunctive rule sets: An alternative to artificial grammars. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1393-1400] points to limitations in the sequential approach. In the experiment, participants studied words selected according to a conjunctive rule. At test, participants discriminated rule-consistent from rule-violating words but could not verbalize the rule. Although the data elude explanation by sequential models, an exemplar model of implicit learning can explain them. To make the case, we simulate the full pattern of results by incorporating vector representations for the words used in the experiment, derived from the large-scale semantic space models LSA and BEAGLE, into an exemplar model of memory, MINERVA 2. We show that basic memory processes in a classic model of memory capture implicit learning of non-sequential rules, provided that stimuli are appropriately represented.
Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Modelos Teóricos , Semántica , Simulación por Computador , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental/fisiologíaRESUMEN
People behave as if they know the structure of their environment. Because people rarely study that structure explicitly, several theorists have postulated an implicit learning system that abstracts that structure automatically. An alternative view is that people respond to local structure that derives from global structure. Measures are developed that quantify structure in a set of stimuli, in individual stimuli, and in encoded stimuli. The authors apply the measures to examine serial recall for sequences of colors generated using a stationary Markov grammar. They demonstrate that the 3 kinds of redundancy are confounded and show that the memorial advantage for grammatical stimuli reflects participants' use of local expressions of grammatical structure to aid learning.
Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Lingüística , Teoría Psicológica , Humanos , Lingüística/estadística & datos numéricos , Recuerdo Mental , Modelos Psicológicos , Tiempo de ReacciónRESUMEN
We present a holographic theory of human memory. According to the theory, a subject's vocabulary resides in a dynamic distributed representation-a hologram. Studying or recalling a word alters both the existing representation of that word in the hologram and all words associated with it. Recall is always prompted by a recall cue (either a start instruction or the word just recalled). Order of report is a joint function of the item and associative information residing in the hologram at the time the report is made. We apply the model to archival data involving simple free recall, learning in multitrial free recall, simple serial recall, and learning in multitrial serial recall. The model captures accuracy and order of report in both free and serial recall. It also captures learning and subjective organisation in multitrial free recall. We offer the model as an alternative to the short- and long-term account of memory postulated in the modal model.
Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , HumanosRESUMEN
The authors varied the similarity between negative probes and study items in a short-term item-recognition task. Current models treat similarity as a function of the number of occurrences of the probe's features in the study set, a factor that is often confounded with the number of the probe's features occurring in the study set. Unconfounded comparisons showed that performance reflected only the latter factor, with response time a linear function of the number of probe features in the study set. The effect was obtained for both stimuli with manipulated features (colored shapes) and words. Number of presented features is a global property of the study list, but existing global models calculate familiarity by averaging across item matches and cannot readily accommodate the data. The authors proposed that the probe's features are compared with a global representation of the study set's features.
Asunto(s)
Atención , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Adulto , Percepción de Color , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de ReacciónRESUMEN
The repetition deficit associated with rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) has been explained as a repetition-induced blindness, that is, as a perceptual or encoding failure. The repetition deficit was replicated in a standard free-recall RSVP task, and it was shown that participants were able to report the lost item when they were prompted with a retrieval probe. The authors argue that both copies of the repeated items were available in memory but that they were not accessible for report. Hence, they conclude that the repetition deficit in the RSVP task reflects a retrieval failure, not a perceptual failure.
Asunto(s)
Atención , Recuerdo Mental , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Aprendizaje Seriado , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , PsicofísicaRESUMEN
Part 1 concerns representation: We demonstrate pop-out for a target that can be distinguished from the distractors only by the relative position of its components and thereby show that simple shape information is represented preattentively. We discuss our findings in terms of theories of search and texture segregation. Part 2 concerns selection: We demonstrate strong learning in a varied-mapping paradigm and show that preattentive shape information can be used for selection. Finally, we suggest an account fo the learning, namely the group scale hypothesis, and present a final experiment to test it. Our results confirm and extend the emphasis placed by Duncan and Humphreys' (1989) attentional engagement theory on grouping processes in visual search.
Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción de Forma , Percepción Espacial , Percepción de Color , Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Práctica Psicológica , Tiempo de ReacciónRESUMEN
Cohen, Dunbar, and McClelland's (1990) model was tested for Strooplike interference tasks by studying the shape of the distribution of response latencies produced by Ss and by the model. The model correctly anticipates changes in mean response latency (M(RT)) across congruent and incongruent conditions. It does not, however, correctly anticipate changes in the shape of the distributions, even though changes in the shape of the distributions underlie the changes in M(RT). Thus the model predicts M(RT) successfully but for the wrong reason. It is concluded that the model is not an adequate account of Ss' performance in the Stroop task.
Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción de Color , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Semántica , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Tiempo de ReacciónRESUMEN
Three experiments studied the perception of tone sequences having various degrees of musical structure. Ratings of perceived structure and ease of recognition in transposition were both influenced by harmonic progression (as defined by music theory), the contour (directional changes in pitch), and the excursion or repetition pattern within the sequence. The relation between the original and transposed sequence also affected ease of recognition in accordance with the number of tones shared between the two sequences. The results are described in terms of the abstraction and analysis of levels of pitch relations, an analysis conducted even by musically untrained listeners. The conceptual framework emphasizes the application of musical rules as an illustration of rules governing auditory sequences in general.
Asunto(s)
Música , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Humanos , Juicio , Discriminación de la Altura Tonal , PsicoacústicaRESUMEN
In studies of stimulus control of sexual arousal, some researchers transform raw data to z-scores. Earls, Quinsey and Castonguay [Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 493-500 (1987)] have argued that the proportion of variance due to stimulus presentations was greater in z-scores than in the raw data or in a percent score. We present analyses of sexual arousal data that show that z-score transformations may distort the information inherent in the raw data and may increase random error. In addition, we present Monte Carlo analyses indicating that the z-score transformation compromises the estimates of Type I error, and, depending on specific circumstances, sometimes increases and sometimes decreases the power of the statistical test. Transformation of raw data to percent of full erection does not distort the information in the raw data, does not compromise the data analyses and, therefore, is the preferred transformation.
Asunto(s)
Método de Montecarlo , Trastornos Parafílicos/diagnóstico , Erección Peniana/fisiología , Pletismografía/estadística & datos numéricos , Delitos Sexuales , Análisis de Varianza , Sesgo , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos Parafílicos/epidemiología , Violación , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , MuestreoRESUMEN
We introduce and evaluate via a Monte Carlo study a robust new estimation technique that fits distribution functions to grouped response time (RT) data, where the grouping is determined by sample quantiles. The new estimator, quantile maximum likelihood (QML), is more efficient and less biased than the best alternative estimation technique when fitting the commonly used ex-Gaussian distribution. Limitations of the Monte Carlo results are discussed and guidance provided for the practical application of the new technique. Because QML estimation can be computationally costly, we make fast open source code for fitting available that can be easily modified to use QML in the estimation of any distribution function.
Asunto(s)
Funciones de Verosimilitud , Tiempo de Reacción , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Humanos , Método de Montecarlo , Distribución Normal , Reproducibilidad de los ResultadosRESUMEN
The power function is treated as the law relating response time to practice trials. However, the evidence for a power law is flawed, because it is based on averaged data. We report a survey that assessed the form of the practice function for individual learners and learning conditions in paradigms that have shaped theories of skill acquisition. We fit power and exponential functions to 40 sets of data representing 7,910 learning series from 475 subjects in 24 experiments. The exponential function fit better than the power function in all the unaveraged data sets. Averaging produced a bias in favor of the power function. A new practice function based on the exponential, the APEX function, fit better than a power function with an extra, preexperimental practice parameter. Clearly, the best candidate for the law of practice is the exponential or APEX function, not the generally accepted power function. The theoretical implications are discussed.
Asunto(s)
Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Aprendizaje , Práctica Psicológica , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Dinámicas no LinealesRESUMEN
When letters and words are presented tachistoscopically, material from the right visual field (RVF) can be reported more accurately than that from the left visual field (LVF). The RVF superiority may reflect either left hemispheric dominance for language or directional scanning. Previous studies have deliberately focused on the cerebral asymmetry factor while "controlling" scanning and, thus, have cast some doubt on the potency of the scanning factor. Two experiments were conducted to show that scanning can induce a RVF superiority comparable to that often associated with cerebral asymmetry. The first experiment required bilingual subjects to report six English or six Hebrew letters, shown briefly in either the LVF or RVF, with order of report controlled. A RVF superiority found with English characters was matched by an equal but opposite LVF effect with Hebrew. In a second experiment, five English characters were shown briefly in either the LVF or RVF, and subjects had to identify a single character indicated by a post exposural cue. Using a spatial cue to by pass scanning, there were no field differences; with an ordinal position cue--a procedure thought to force scanning--there was a strong RVF superiority. The results show clearly that scanning can induce visual field differences.
Asunto(s)
Lectura , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Dominancia Cerebral , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Campos VisualesRESUMEN
We argue that rule-like phenomena in naming and lexical decision reflect the collapsing of information that occurs during retrieval from the lexicon, and that complex patterns in performance reflect the pattern of correlation that exists in the reader's lexicon rather than mapping rules wired into, or learned by, the processing apparatus. By using a lexicon built to scale, we show that simple retrieval operations applied to a large corpus of words correctly predict an interaction of word frequency by spelling-to-sound regularity in naming, a frequency main effect in lexical decision, sensitivity to orthographically defined syllable-like structures in lexical decision, and an interaction of number of syllables with word frequency in naming.