Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 15 de 15
Filtrar
1.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 17: 1301318, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38249127

RESUMEN

Introduction: Although eye movements such as saccades are related to internal cognitive processes and are independent of visual processing, few studies have investigated whether non-visual cognitive tasks simultaneously affect horizontal and vertical saccades in younger and older adults. Methods: We recruited 28 younger adults aged 20-29 years and 26 older adults aged >60 years through advertisements in community settings. All participants were free of major psychiatric, neurological, or ocular diseases. All participants performed the mental arithmetic task (MAT) and verbal fluency task (VFT). The primary measures were saccade parameters, including frequency, mean amplitude, and mean velocity. Results: During MAT and VFT, the frequencies of horizontal and vertical saccades increased (p = 0.0005 for horizontal saccade in MAT; p < 0.0001 for horizontal saccade in VFT; p = 0.012 for vertical saccade in MAT; p = 0.001 for vertical saccade in VFT), but were comparable between MAT and VFT. The old group showed a slower vertical saccade than the young group during the tasks (p = 0.011 in the MAT phase; p = 0.006 in the VFT phase). The amplitude of the horizontal saccade decreased in both groups during MAT compared to the resting period (p = 0.013), but did not change significantly during VFT. Discussion: Saccade parameters can change during non-visual cognitive tasks with differences between age groups and saccade directions. This study significantly contributes to our understanding of the distinct dynamics of horizontal and vertical saccades across various age group in cognitive aging, despite its restricted focus on specific saccade parameters and cognitive tasks, and inclusion solely of cognitively normal individuals. This study highlights the importance of saccade analysis in elucidating age-related cognitive changes. In conclusion, saccades should be examined in future studies as a potential non-invasive biomarker for early detection of cognitive decline and neurodegenerative diseases.

2.
Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw ; 25(2): 135-139, 2022 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34962156

RESUMEN

This study investigated whether the degree of cybersickness varies depending on different virtual reality experience modes (playing vs. watching) and whether specific eye movement parameters reflect changes in cybersickness. Simulator Sickness Questionnaire results from 20 participants (10 playing and 10 watching) showed that cybersickness was much more severe in the watching mode, particularly during the second of the three total trials. Moreover, cybersickness' changing pattern was reflected in the center gaze ratio and scan-path length. These findings imply the importance of physiological measurements for a deeper understanding of cybersickness in theoretical and practical respects.


Asunto(s)
Mareo por Movimiento , Realidad Virtual , Movimientos Oculares , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
3.
Cogn Emot ; 25(6): 998-1013, 2011 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21432641

RESUMEN

While previous research has linked executive attention to emotion regulation, the current study investigated the role of attentional alerting (i.e., efficient use of external warning cues) on younger (N=39) and older (N=44) adults' use of gaze to regulate their mood in real time. Participants viewed highly arousing unpleasant images while reporting their mood and were instructed to deliberately manage how they felt and to minimise the effect of those stimuli on their mood. Fixations toward the most negative areas of the images were recorded with eye tracking. We examined whether looking less at the most negative regions, compared to each individual's own tendency, was a beneficial mood regulatory strategy and how it interacted with age and alerting ability. High alerting older adults, who rely more on external cues to guide their attention, experienced a smaller decline in mood over time by activating a less-negative-looking approach (compared to their own average tendency), effectively looking away from the most negative areas of the images. More negative gaze patterns predicted better mood for younger adults, though this effect decreased over time. Alerting did not moderate gaze-mood links in younger adults. Successful mood regulation may thus depend on particular combinations of age, fixation, and attention.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
4.
Psychol Aging ; 21(4): 790-803, 2006 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17201498

RESUMEN

The authors examined age differences in adults' allocation of effort when reading text for either high levels of recall accuracy or high levels of efficiency. Participants read a series of sentences, making judgments of learning before recall. Older adults showed less sensitivity than the young to the accuracy goal in both reading time allocation and memory performance. Memory accuracy and differential allocation of effort to unlearned items were age equivalent, so age differences in goal adherence were not attributable to metacognitive factors. However, comparison with data from a control reading task without monitoring showed that learning gains among older adults across trial were reduced relative to those of the young by memory monitoring, suggesting that monitoring may be resource consuming for older learners. Age differences in the responsiveness to (information-acquisition) goals could be accounted for, in part, by independent contributions from working memory and memory self-efficacy. Our data suggest that both processing capacity ("what you have") and beliefs ("knowing you can do it") can contribute to individual differences in engaging resources ("what you do") to effectively learn novel content from text.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Objetivos , Controles Informales de la Sociedad , Percepción del Habla , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Lectura , Autoeficacia , Vocabulario , Escalas de Wechsler
5.
Psychol Aging ; 30(2): 356-68, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26030774

RESUMEN

Despite many studies on the age-related positivity effect and its role in visual attention, discrepancies remain regarding whether full attention is required for age-related differences to emerge. The present study took a new approach to this question by varying the contextual demands of emotion processing. This was done by adding perceptual distractions, such as visual and auditory noise, that could disrupt attentional control. Younger and older participants viewed pairs of happy-neutral and fearful-neutral faces while their eye movements were recorded. Facial stimuli were shown either without noise, embedded in a background of visual noise (low, medium, or high), or with simultaneous auditory babble. Older adults showed positive gaze preferences, looking toward happy faces and away from fearful faces; however, their gaze preferences tended to be influenced by the level of visual noise. Specifically, the tendency to look away from fearful faces was not present in conditions with low and medium levels of visual noise but was present when there were high levels of visual noise. It is important to note, however, that in the high-visual-noise condition, external cues were present to facilitate the processing of emotional information. In addition, older adults' positive gaze preferences disappeared or were reduced when they first viewed emotional faces within a distracting context. The current results indicate that positive gaze preferences may be less likely to occur in distracting contexts that disrupt control of visual attention.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Atención/fisiología , Emociones , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Optimismo/psicología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Señales (Psicología) , Expresión Facial , Miedo , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Ruido , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
6.
Psychol Aging ; 29(4): 891-906, 2014 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25402337

RESUMEN

While a training model of cognitive intervention targets the improvement of particular skills through instruction and practice, an engagement model is based on the idea that being embedded in an intellectually and socially complex environment can impact cognition, perhaps even broadly, without explicit instruction. We contrasted these 2 models of cognitive enrichment by randomly assigning healthy older adults to a home-based inductive reasoning training program, a team-based competitive program in creative problem solving, or a wait-list control. As predicted, those in the training condition showed selective improvement in inductive reasoning. Those in the engagement condition, on the other hand, showed selective improvement in divergent thinking, a key ability exercised in creative problem solving. On average, then, both groups appeared to show ability-specific effects. However, moderators of change differed somewhat for those in the engagement and training interventions. Generally, those who started either intervention with a more positive cognitive profile showed more cognitive growth, suggesting that cognitive resources enabled individuals to take advantage of environmental enrichment. Only in the engagement condition did initial levels of openness and social network size moderate intervention effects on cognition, suggesting that comfort with novelty and an ability to manage social resources may be additional factors contributing to the capacity to take advantage of the environmental complexity associated with engagement. Collectively, these findings suggest that training and engagement models may offer alternative routes to cognitive resilience in late life.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Refuerzo Biomédico/métodos , Cognición/fisiología , Ambiente , Actividades Humanas , Apoyo Social , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Conducta Competitiva , Femenino , Procesos de Grupo , Humanos , Lógica , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Psicológicos , Cooperación del Paciente , Pacientes Desistentes del Tratamiento , Solución de Problemas/fisiología
7.
Emotion ; 13(2): 238-49, 2013 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23163713

RESUMEN

Although age-related declines in facial expression recognition are well documented, previous research has relied mostly on isolated faces devoid of context. The authors investigated the effects of context on age differences in recognition of facial emotions and in visual scanning patterns of emotional faces. While their eye movements were monitored, younger and older participants viewed facial expressions (i.e., anger, disgust) in contexts that were emotionally congruent, incongruent, or neutral to the facial expression to be identified. Both age groups had the highest recognition rates of facial expressions in the congruent context, followed by the neutral context, and recognition rates in the incongruent context were the lowest. These context effects were more pronounced for older adults. Compared to younger adults, older adults exhibited a greater benefit from congruent contextual information, regardless of facial expression. Context also influenced the pattern of visual scanning characteristics of emotional faces in a similar manner across age groups. In addition, older adults initially attended more to context overall. Our data highlight the importance of considering the role of context in understanding emotion recognition in adulthood.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Emociones , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
8.
Front Psychol ; 3: 311, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22969741

RESUMEN

Although previous research has shown that positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) modulate attentional functioning in distinct ways, few studies have considered whether the links between affect and attentional functioning may vary as a function of age. Using the Attention Network Test (Fan et al., 2002), we tested whether participants' current state of PA and NA influenced distinct attentional functions (i.e., alerting, orienting, and executive attention) and how the relationships between affective states and attentional functioning differ in younger (18-25 years) and older (60-85 years) age groups. While there were age differences in alerting efficiency, these age differences were mediated by PA, indicating that the higher state PA found in older adults may contribute to age differences in alerting. Furthermore, age group moderated the relationship between PA and orienting as well as NA and orienting. That is, higher levels of PA and lower levels of NA were associated with enhanced orienting efficiency in older adults. Neither PA nor NA had any influence on executive attention. The current results suggest that PA and NA may influence attentional functioning in distinct ways, but that these patterns may depend on age groups.

9.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22149149

RESUMEN

The present study was an examination of how exposure to print affects sentence processing and memory in older readers. A sample of older adults (N = 139; Mean age = 72) completed a battery of cognitive and linguistic tests and read a series of sentences for recall. Word-by-word reading times were recorded and generalized linear mixed effects models were used to estimate components representing attentional allocation to word-level and textbase-level processes. Older adults with higher levels of print exposure showed greater efficiency in word-level processing and in the immediate instantiation of new concepts, but allocated more time to semantic integration at clause boundaries. While lower levels of working memory were associated with smaller wrap-up effects, individuals with higher levels of print exposure showed a reduced effect of working memory on sentence wrap-up. Importantly, print exposure was not only positively associated with sentence memory, but was also found to buffer the effects of working memory on sentence recall. These findings suggest that the increased efficiency of component reading processes that come with life-long habits of literacy buffer the effects of working memory decline on comprehension and contribute to maintaining skilled reading among older adults.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Reserva Cognitiva , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Memoria , Recuerdo Mental , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Comprensión , Humanos , Lenguaje , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lectura , Semántica
10.
Soc Personal Psychol Compass ; 5(8): 505-517, 2011 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21837251

RESUMEN

In this paper, we link age differences in gaze patterns toward emotional stimuli to later mood outcomes. While one might think that looking at more positive emotional material leads to better moods, and looking at more negative material leads to worse moods, it turns out that links between emotional looking and mood depend on age as well as individual differences. Though older people can feel good by looking more at positive material, in some cases young adults actually feel better by engaging visually with the negative. These age effects are further moderated by attentional abilities. Such findings suggest that different age groups may use looking differently, and this may reflect their preferences for using distinct emotion regulatory strategies. This work also serves as a reminder that regulatory efforts are not always successful at improving mood.

11.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 18(1): 83-8, 2011 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21327368

RESUMEN

The Effortfulness Hypothesis suggests that sensory impairment (either simulated or age-related) may decrease capacity for semantic integration in language comprehension. We directly tested this hypothesis by measuring resource allocation to different levels of processing during reading (i.e., word vs. semantic analysis). College students read three sets of passages word-by-word, one at each of three levels of dynamic visual noise. There was a reliable interaction between processing level and noise, such that visual noise increased resources allocated to word-level processing, at the cost of attention paid to semantic analysis. Recall of the most important ideas also decreased with increasing visual noise. Results suggest that sensory challenge can impair higher-level cognitive functions in learning from text, supporting the Effortfulness Hypothesis.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Formación de Concepto , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Lectura , Adolescente , Comprensión , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Tiempo de Reacción , Semántica , Adulto Joven
12.
Psychol Aging ; 26(3): 738-43, 2011 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21443348

RESUMEN

The current study investigated the nature of the flow state among older adults. Flow is a pleasurable experiential state that occurs during full-capacity engagement in which an individual is performing at a level that is matched with the demands of the task. Each participant completed a scale assessing dimensions of flow in a particular activity selected by the participant. More cognitively demanding activities elicited higher levels of flow for those with higher fluid ability, but lower levels of flow for those with lower fluid ability. This pattern was reversed for activities that were low in demand. Our data highlight the potential importance of considering motivational states such as flow in understanding cognitive optimization in adulthood.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Placer , Pruebas Psicológicas , Pensamiento
13.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 63(7): 1430-55, 2010 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19941199

RESUMEN

This research examined age differences in the accommodation of reading strategies as a consequence of explicit instruction in conceptual integration. In Experiment 1, young, middle-aged, and older adults read sentences for delayed recall using a moving-window method. Readers in an experimental group received instruction in making conceptual links during reading while readers in a control group were simply encouraged to allocate effort. Regression analysis to decompose word-by-word reading times in each condition isolated the time allocated to conceptual processing at the point in the text at which new concepts were introduced, as well as at clause and sentence boundaries. While younger adults responded to instructions by differentially allocating effort to sentence wrap-up, older adults allocated effort to intrasentence wrap-up and on new concepts as they were introduced, suggesting that older readers optimized their allocation of effort to linguistic computations for textbase construction within their processing capacity. Experiment 2 verified that conceptual integration training improved immediate recall among older readers as a consequence of engendering allocation to conceptual processing.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Formación de Concepto/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Lectura , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
14.
Mem Cognit ; 37(6): 769-78, 2009 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19679857

RESUMEN

Understanding a narrative situation depends on keeping track of multiple characters that enter and exit dynamically as the plot unfolds. We investigated age differences in this process during narrative comprehension. In Experiment 1, we used a probe recognition paradigm to examine the effect of age on the accessibility of a previous character when another character was subsequently introduced. In Experiment 2, reading time was measured to examine age differences in the encoding of a new character after another had already been introduced. Our findings show that older readers have particular difficulty both in accessing the initial character after a new character is introduced and in thoroughly encoding a new character while other characters inhabit the discourse world. We attribute these differences to age differences in working memory that make it difficult to access a backgrounded character when a new character is in focus and to distinctively encode a new character when maintenance of another character is already consuming attentional resources.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Comprensión , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Narración , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Atención , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Lectura , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Adulto Joven
15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19468932

RESUMEN

We investigated interrelationships between the predisposition toward approaching experiences in a mindful and creative way, participation in specific activities, and cognition among older adults. Participants were administered a battery measuring cognition (i.e., working memory, processing speed, divergent thinking, inductive reasoning, visuo-spatial processing), activity level, and the predisposition towards mental engagement (Need for Cognition, Mindfulness, and Openness to Experience). Results indicated that predispositional engagement and activity engagement are distinct constructs that independently contribute to different aspects of fluid ability, highlighting the importance of considering both the predisposition toward mental engagement as well as the habitual tendency to participate in activities when exploring principles of cognitive optimization.


Asunto(s)
Conducta , Cognición , Personalidad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Procesos Mentales , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA