Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 23
Filtrar
1.
J Soc Psychol ; 154(4): 311-22, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25154115

RESUMEN

Theories of self-regulation emphasize the special role that the symbolic self may play in approach and avoidance movements, but experimental evidence is lacking. In two experiments (total N = 157), participants moved either a self-relevant (e.g., "me") or non-self (e.g., "not me") agent to one of two locations, one occupied by a positive word and the other occupied by a negative word. In both experiments, the movement agent interacted with the destination valence such that it was only the symbolic self that moved more quickly to positive rather than negative locations. These results established a role for the symbolic self in approach/avoidance that had been questioned, thereby supporting both classic and contemporary self-related theories of approach and avoidance.


Asunto(s)
Reacción de Prevención , Juegos Experimentales , Tiempo de Reacción , Autoimagen , Identificación Social , Simbolismo , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto Joven
2.
Cogn Emot ; 27(3): 453-64, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22989107

RESUMEN

It was hypothesised that affect-amplifying individuals would be more reactive to affective events in daily life. Affect amplification was quantified in terms of overestimating the font size of positive and negative, relative to neutral, words in a basic perception task. Subsequently, the same (N=70) individuals completed a daily diary protocol in which they reported on levels of daily stressors, provocations, and social support as well as six emotion-related outcomes for 14 consecutive days. Individual differences in affect amplification moderated reactivity to daily affective events in all such analyses. For example, daily stressor levels predicted cognitive failures at high, but not low, levels of affect amplification. Affect amplification, then, appears to have widespread utility in understanding individual differences in emotional reactivity.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Percepción Visual , Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Neuroticismo , Estimulación Luminosa , Apoyo Social
3.
J Pers ; 80(2): 255-85, 2012 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21299556

RESUMEN

Dispositional variations in mindfulness and its facets have garnered considerable recent interest in the clinical and personality literatures. Theoretically, high mindful individuals have been characterized as more attuned to momentary sensations and perceptions and/or better able to execute behavior in a controlled manner, yet data of this relatively cognitive type have not been reported. In addition, perceptual attunement and executive control are distinct skills that may underlie, or at least correlate with, distinct facets of mindfulness. In 3 studies involving college students (N = 297), support for the latter idea was found. Individuals high in the observing (but not nonreactivity) facet of mindfulness demonstrated superior perceptual abilities in visual working memory (Study 1) and temporal order (Study 2) tasks. On the other hand, individuals high in the nonreactivity (but not observing) facet of mindfulness exhibited greater cognitive control flexibility (Study 3). Implications for understanding the cognitive basis of mindfulness facets are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Carácter , Cognición , Memoria/fisiología , Autoimagen , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Adulto Joven
4.
Psychosom Med ; 73(3): 250-6, 2011 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21217094

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To test in a laboratory setting the hypothesis that the most problematic daily outcomes should be particular to individuals displaying higher cortisol reactivity and deficits in executive functioning as assessed in a task-switching paradigm. METHODS: Thirty-eight volunteers completed a comprehensive assessment protocol. Individual differences in cortisol reactivity were quantified in an initial laboratory session involving a social stress speech task. Subsequently, individual differences in task-switching costs in a cognitive paradigm were assessed in a second session. Participants then reported on four problematic outcomes-error reactivity; worry; core aspects of negative emotionality; and aggression behavior frequency-for 15 consecutive days. RESULTS: Levels of cortisol reactivity did not predict task-switching costs. Instead, and as hypothesized, individual differences in cortisol reactivity and task-switching costs interacted to predict the problematic daily outcomes. The highest levels of such problematic outcomes were particular to high cortisol reactors also exhibiting greater task-switching costs. CONCLUSIONS: The findings support the dual vulnerability model proposed and are discussed from temperamental, health risk, and daily outcome perspectives. These findings indicate that cortisol is a risk factor, particularly when combined with deficiencies in task-switching.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Hidrocortisona/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Actividades Cotidianas/psicología , Agresión/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiología , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/análisis , Individualidad , Masculino , Solución de Problemas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Saliva/química , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Temperamento/fisiología
5.
J Pers ; 79(5): 875-904, 2011 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21204841

RESUMEN

Since the inception of the empirical study of personality, and even before it, individual differences in anxiety and distress have been viewed as key predictors of behavioral performance. Yet such literatures have always entertained 2 perspectives, one contending that anxious individuals are "driven" and the other contending that anxious individuals are "distracted." The present 3 studies (total N=289) sought to reconcile such discrepant views according to an ex-Gaussian parsing of reaction time performance tendencies in basic cognitive tasks. As hypothesized, a particular pattern marked by faster responding on the preponderance of trials (in terms of the ex-Gaussian µ parameter) in combination with slower responding on other trials (in terms of the ex-Gaussian τ parameter) was predictive of higher levels of anxiety. Implications for understanding neuroticism, distress, the anxiety-performance interface, and cognitive models of personality processes are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Cognición , Trastornos Neuróticos/psicología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Trastornos de Ansiedad/diagnóstico , Trastorno Depresivo/psicología , Humanos , Illinois , Trastornos Neuróticos/diagnóstico , Distribución Normal , Pruebas Psicológicas , Análisis de Regresión , Estrés Psicológico , Estudiantes , Universidades
6.
Cogn Emot ; 25(2): 307-27, 2011 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21432673

RESUMEN

Mental noise can be defined as less reliable information processing. Individuals with high levels of mental noise are thought to be disadvantaged in cognitive, emotional, and behavioural realms. The present five studies (total N=298) investigated such potential disadvantages among normally functioning college undergraduates. Mental noise was operationalised in terms of the reaction time coefficient of variation (RTCV), a measure of RT variability that corrects for average levels of mental speed. Individuals with higher RTCV exhibited less effective cognitive control (Studies 1 and 5), less controlled behaviour (Study 2), and were more prone to negative emotional experiences (Study 3) and depressive symptoms (Study 4). Study 5 extended these results and found that individuals higher (versus lower) in RTCV were more adversely affected by their attentional lapses in daily life. Results converge on the idea that mental noise is an important individual difference dimension with multiple adverse correlates and consequences.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Emociones , Individualidad , Procesos Mentales , Tiempo de Reacción , Atención , Depresión/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Desempeño Psicomotor , Controles Informales de la Sociedad
7.
J Behav Med ; 33(4): 282-92, 2010 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20204489

RESUMEN

The purpose of the present study was to examine whether stress-somatic symptom associations may be more pronounced among individuals whose bodies exhibit higher levels of cardiovascular reactivity to a laboratory social stress task. During an initial laboratory session, participants delivered a 5-min speech and individual differences in cardiovascular reactivity were quantified. The same participants subsequently completed a 15-day experience sampling protocol, in which daily levels of stress and somatic symptoms were assessed. Multi-level modeling was used to assess associations among laboratory cardiovascular reactivity, daily stress and somatic symptoms. Daily symptom reports included a set of commonly experienced physical symptoms reflective of general bodily dysfunction. Individuals displaying high levels of laboratory systolic blood pressure reactivity experienced more somatic symptoms on high-stress days, but this was not the case for individuals low in systolic blood pressure reactivity. The results bridge two hitherto distinct health psychology literatures showing that cardiovascular and somatic reactivity to stress are associated. Stress reactivity individual differences in one system may indicate more general differences in bodily reactivity across systems.


Asunto(s)
Presión Sanguínea/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Medio Social , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Adulto Joven
8.
Emotion ; 9(1): 70-82, 2009 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19186918

RESUMEN

Approach motivation consists of the active, engaged pursuit of one's goals. The purpose of the present three studies (N = 258) was to examine whether approach motivation could be cognitively modeled, thereby providing process-based insights into personality functioning. Behavioral facilitation was assessed in terms of faster (or facilitated) reaction time with practice. As hypothesized, such tendencies predicted higher levels of approach motivation, higher levels of positive affect, and lower levels of depressive symptoms and did so across cognitive, behavioral, self-reported, and peer-reported outcomes. Tendencies toward behavioral facilitation, on the other hand, did not correlate with self-reported traits (Study 1) and did not predict avoidance motivation or negative affect (all studies). The results indicate a systematic relationship between behavioral facilitation in cognitive tasks and approach motivation in daily life. Results are discussed in terms of the benefits of modeling the cognitive processes hypothesized to underlie individual differences motivation, affect, and depression.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Cognición , Motivación , Conducta Social , Adulto , Femenino , Objetivos , Humanos , Masculino , Castigo , Tiempo de Reacción , Recompensa
9.
Psychol Sci ; 19(7): 702-8, 2008 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18727786

RESUMEN

This study examined whether individual differences in error-related self-regulation predict emotion regulation in daily life, as suggested by a common-systems view of cognitive and emotional self-regulation. Participants (N= 47) completed a Stroop task, from which error-related brain potentials and behavioral measures of error correction were computed. Participants subsequently reported on daily stressors and anxiety over a 2-week period. As predicted by the common-systems view, a physiological marker of error monitoring and a behavioral measure of error correction predicted emotion regulation in daily life. Specifically, participants higher in cognitive control, as assessed neurally and behaviorally, were less reactive to stress in daily life. The results support the notion that cognitive control and emotion regulation depend on common or interacting systems.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Estrés Psicológico/prevención & control , Estrés Psicológico/psicología , Afecto , Ansiedad/psicología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
10.
Med Care Res Rev ; 75(4): 516-524, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29148334

RESUMEN

We compared new Medicaid enrollees with similar ongoing enrollees for evidence of pent-up demand using claims data following Minnesota's 2014 Medicaid expansion. We hypothesized that if new enrollees had pent-up demand, utilization would decline over time as testing and disease management plans are put in place. Consistent with pent-up demand among new enrollees, the probability of an office visit, a new patient office visit, and an emergency department visit declines over time for new enrollees relative to ongoing Medicaid enrollees. The pattern of utilization suggests that the newly insured are connecting with primary care after the 2014 Medicaid expansion and, unlike ongoing Medicaid enrollees; the newly insured have a declining reliance on the emergency department over time.


Asunto(s)
Atención a la Salud/organización & administración , Atención a la Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Necesidades y Demandas de Servicios de Salud/organización & administración , Necesidades y Demandas de Servicios de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Medicaid/organización & administración , Medicaid/estadística & datos numéricos , Aceptación de la Atención de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Minnesota , Estados Unidos
11.
Emotion ; 7(3): 579-91, 2007 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17683214

RESUMEN

The present hypotheses were guided by four premises, which were systematically examined in six studies involving 409 undergraduate participants. The first premise, established by prior work, is that trait neuroticism is closely associated with avoidance-related goals. The second premise, however, is that neuroticism may be uncorrelated with cognitive tendencies to recognize threats as they occur, and subsequently to down-regulate them. In support of this point, all six studies found that neuroticism was unrelated to post-error behavioral adjustments in choice reaction time. The third premise is that post-error reactivity would nonetheless predict individual differences in threat-recognition (Studies 1 and 2) and its apparent mitigation (Study 3), independently of trait neuroticism. These predictions were supported. The fourth premise is that individual differences in neuroticism and error-reactivity would interact with each other in predicting everyday experiences of distress. In support of such predictions, Studies 4-6 found that higher levels of error-reactivity were associated with less negative affect at high levels of neuroticism, but more negative affect at low levels of neuroticism. The findings are interpreted in terms of trait-cognition self-regulation principles.


Asunto(s)
Depresión/diagnóstico , Depresión/psicología , Trastornos Neuróticos/diagnóstico , Trastornos Neuróticos/psicología , Controles Informales de la Sociedad , Adulto , Afecto , Conflicto Psicológico , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Tiempo de Reacción
12.
Pers Individ Dif ; 43(8): 2137-2148, 2007 Dec 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19568327

RESUMEN

In the temperament literature, agreeableness has been theoretically linked to effortful control. Further, research in this area has suggested that effortful control may play a broad role in moderating temperament-based tendencies toward negative affect. The present three studies, involving a total of 300 undergraduate participants, sought to extend this perspective to the adult literature by examining potential interactions between agreeableness and neuroticism in predicting reported somatic symptoms. Although such symptoms have been linked to neuroticism, they are not characteristic of the interpersonal concerns linked to agreeableness. Nevertheless, all studies found that agreeableness and neuroticism interacted to predict somatic symptoms such that high levels of agreeableness decoupled the relationship between neuroticism and somatic distress. These findings indicate a broad role for agreeableness in the self-regulation of neuroticism-linked distress.

13.
Pers Individ Dif ; 42(7): 1221-1231, 2007 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18449325

RESUMEN

Neuroticism has been hypothesized to systematically relate to semantic memory networks favoring negative affect, but no studies using affective priming tasks have established this link. The present two studies, involving 145 undergraduate participants, sought to provide initial evidence along these lines. Study 1 used a task in which participants were asked to judge their emotions in the past, whereas Study 2 used a perceptual identification task in which participants merely had to identify the word in question. In both studies, neuroticism was positively correlated with negative affective priming, but not positive affective priming. The studies suggest that neuroticism systematically relates to the inter-connectivity of negative affect with semantic memory systems, whether involving the self-concept (Study 1) or not (Study 2). These results are novel and important in understanding individual differences in neuroticism and their affective processing correlates.

14.
Psychol Health ; 29(7): 781-95, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24460477

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The objective was to examine an executive control difficulty perspective on individual differences in cortisol reactivity using a daily protocol. DESIGN: Fifty participants competed a laboratory stressor task and individual differences in cortisol reactivity were quantified. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Daily attentional control, conflicting thoughts, error reactivity, worry and mindfulness were assessed. RESULTS: The findings support the idea that as cortisol responses to stress get larger (as an individual difference), attentional control ceases to function as it should in terms of variables that should predict (mindfulness) and follow from (e.g. worry) it. CONCLUSION: The findings support the idea that individual differences in cortisol reactivity can be conceptualised in terms of ineffectual attentional control.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Individualidad , Estrés Psicológico/metabolismo , Ansiedad/metabolismo , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Atención Plena , Saliva/metabolismo , Pensamiento/fisiología
15.
Motiv Emot ; 37(1): 33-38, 2013 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23869114

RESUMEN

The two hemispheres of the brain appear to play different roles in emotion and/or motivation. A great deal of previous research has examined the valence hypothesis (left hemisphere = positive; right = negative), but an increasing body of work has supported the motivational hypothesis (left hemisphere = approach; right = avoidance) as an alternative. The present investigation (N = 117) sought to provide novel support for the latter perspective. Left versus right hemispheres were briefly activated by neutral lateralized auditory primes. Subsequently, participants categorized approach versus avoidance words as quickly and accurately as possible. Performance in the task revealed that approach-related thoughts were more accessible following left-hemispheric activation, whereas avoidance-related thoughts were more accessible following right-hemispheric activation. The present results are the first to examine such lateralized differences in accessible motivational thoughts, which may underlie more "downstream" manifestations of approach and avoidance motivation such as judgments, decision making, and behavior.

16.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 104(5): 907-20, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23527850

RESUMEN

Several theories and self-reported sources of data link individual differences in negative affectivity to avoidance motivation. Chronic avoidance motivation, through repeated practice, may result in a relatively cognitive distance-enhancing dynamic whereby events and stimuli are perceived as further away from the self, even when they are not threatening. Such predictions are novel but follow from cybernetic theories of self-regulation. In 5 studies (total N = 463), relations of this type were investigated. Study 1 presented participants with phrases that were ambiguous and found that trait negative affect predicted phrase interpretation in a distance-enhancing temporal direction. Study 2 replicated this effect across a systematic manipulation of event valence. Study 3 asked individuals to estimate the size of words and found that individuals higher in neuroticism generally perceived words to be smaller than did individuals lower in neuroticism. In Study 4, people high (but not low) in neuroticism perceived words to be shrinking faster than they were growing. In Study 5, greater perceptual distancing, in a font size estimation task, predicted more adverse reactions to negative events in daily life. Although normative effects varied across studies, consistent support for a chronic distancing perspective of individual differences in negative affectivity was found.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Ansiedad/psicología , Afecto , Percepción de Distancia , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Motivación , Neuroticismo , Pruebas Psicológicas , Percepción del Tamaño , Percepción Social , Percepción del Tiempo , Adulto Joven
17.
Cognit Ther Res ; 37(2): 412-418, 2013 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23687398

RESUMEN

Several theories of psychopathy link it to an egocentric mode of perceiving the world. This explanatory perspective is quite plausible given that psychopaths are viewed as callous, uncaring, and narcissistic. This explanatory perspective, though, has been an insufficient focus of research, particularly in basic cognitive tasks. Building on the work of Wapner and Werner (1957), an implicit measure of cognitive egocentrism was developed. Continuous variations in primary and secondary psychopathy were assessed in a sample of college undergraduates (N = 80). Individuals high in primary psychopathy exhibited cognitive egocentrism, whereas individuals low in primary psychopathy did not. On the other hand, variations in secondary psychopathy were non-predictive of performance in the task. Results are discussed in terms of theories of psychopathy, distinctions between its primary and secondary components, and the utility of modeling egocentrism in basic cognitive terms.

18.
J Res Pers ; 41(7): 90-96, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23564985

RESUMEN

Warmth-coldness is a fundamental dimension of social behavior. Cold individuals are egocentric in their social relations, whereas warm individuals are not. Previous theorizing suggests that cognitive egocentrism underlies social egocentrism. It was hypothesized that higher levels of interpersonal coldness would predict greater cognitive egocentrism. Cognitive egocentrism was assessed in basic terms through tasks wherein priming a lateralized self-state biased subsequent visual perceptions in an assimilation-related manner. Such effects reflect a tendency to assume that the self's incidental state provides meaningful information concerning the external world. Cognitive egocentrism was evident at high, but not low, levels of interpersonal coldness. The findings reveal a basic difference between warm and cold people, encouraging future research linking cognitive egocentrism to variability in relationship functioning.

19.
Emotion ; 12(1): 91-101, 2012 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21875189

RESUMEN

Four experiments (total N = 391) examined predictions derived from a biologically based incentive salience theory of approach motivation. In all experiments, judgments indicative of enhanced perceptual salience were exaggerated in the context of positive, relative to neutral or negative, stimuli. In Experiments 1 and 2, positive words were judged to be of a larger size (Experiment 1) and led individuals to judge subsequently presented neutral objects as larger in size (Experiment 2). In Experiment 3, similar effects were observed in a mock subliminal presentation paradigm. In Experiment 4, positive word primes were perceived to have been presented for a longer duration of time, again relative to both neutral and negative word primes. Results are discussed in relation to theories of approach motivation, affective priming, and the motivation-perception interface.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Motivación/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Psicolingüística/métodos , Adulto Joven
20.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 38(7): 858-69, 2012 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22399360

RESUMEN

Self-reports of approach motivation are unlikely to be sufficient in understanding the extent to which the individual reacts to appetitive cues in an approach-related manner. A novel implicit probe of approach tendencies was thus developed, one that assessed the extent to which positive affective (versus neutral) stimuli primed larger size estimates, as larger perceptual sizes co-occur with locomotion toward objects in the environment. In two studies (total N = 150), self-reports of approach motivation interacted with this implicit probe of approach motivation to predict individual differences in arrogance, a broad interpersonal dimension previously linked to narcissism, antisocial personality tendencies, and aggression. The results of the two studies were highly parallel in that self-reported levels of approach motivation predicted interpersonal arrogance in the particular context of high, but not low, levels of implicit approach motivation. Implications for understanding approach motivation, implicit probes of it, and problematic approach-related outcomes are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Relaciones Interpersonales , Motivación , Narcisismo , Conducta Social , Adulto , Agresión , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Teoría Psicológica , Autoinforme , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA