Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Clin Psychol ; 79(2): 477-496, 2023 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36000930

RESUMEN

Our study aimed to develop and provide a preliminary psychometric validation of the Somatomorphic Matrix-Female (SM-F), a new bidimensional female figural rating scale which can be used to gauge actual and desired levels of both body fat and muscularity in a consolidated measure, as well as providing an index of actual-desired body discrepancy based on these measures. Across two studies undergraduate women (n Study 1 = 481; n Study 2 = 391) completed the Eating Disorder Examination-Questionnaire, the Body Shape Questionnaire-34, and the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (Study 1) and the Drive for Thinness Scale and the Drive for Muscularity Scale (Study 2), as well as the SM-F. Overall, the SM-F demonstrated sound content, concurrent, and convergent validity for actual and desired body fat, actual and desired muscularity, and their respective discrepancy scores.


Asunto(s)
Imagen Corporal , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos , Humanos , Femenino , Impulso (Psicología) , Delgadez , Estudiantes , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos/diagnóstico
2.
Eur J Contracept Reprod Health Care ; 28(2): 97-112, 2023 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36803281

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Abortion stigma is a barrier to accessing and delivering comprehensive, sustainable healthcare. This study aimed to systematically identify measures of abortion stigma, and assess their psychometric properties and uses. MATERIALS AND METHODS: The systematic review was preregistered with PROSPERO (ID#127339) and adhered to Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses guidelines. Eight databases were screened for articles measuring abortion stigma. Data were extracted by four researchers and checked for accuracy by two reviewers. Psychometric properties were assessed with COSMIN guidelines. RESULTS: Of 102 articles reviewed, 21 reported original measures of abortion stigma. Instruments assessed individual and community level stigma for people who have had an abortion (n = 8), healthcare professionals (n = 4), and the public (n = 9), and predominantly originated from the United States (U.S.). Measures varied in structure, use, and comprehensiveness of psychometric properties. On psychometric properties, the Individual Level Abortion Stigma scale and Abortion Provider Stigma Scale - Revised performed best for individual-level stigma and the Stigmatising Attitudes, Beliefs and Actions Scale for community-level stigma. CONCLUSION: Gaps in abortion stigma measurement include geography, conceptualisation, and structural-level stigma. Continued development and testing of tools and methods for measuring abortion stigma is needed.


A range of methods are available to measure abortion stigma, however with gaps in psychometrics, geography, and structural-level stigma. Continued development and testing of tools measuring abortion stigma is required for valid and reliable measurement, models, and intervention.


Asunto(s)
Aborto Inducido , Estigma Social , Embarazo , Femenino , Humanos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Psicometría , Personal de Salud
3.
Curr Psychol ; : 1-9, 2023 Mar 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37359582

RESUMEN

The emotional experiences you have with a romantic partner shape how satisfied you are in your relationship. Engaging in attempts to make a romantic partner feel better is linked with better relationship outcomes. However, it is not yet clear which specific processes people use to regulate their partners' emotions, nor which processes are most strongly linked with relationship satisfaction. In the current study of 277 individuals (55% female), we tested the extent to which eight extrinsic emotion regulation processes (expressive suppression, downward social comparison, humor, distraction, direct action, reappraisal, receptive listening, and valuing) predict relationship satisfaction. Six of the eight processes showed significant positive correlations with relationship satisfaction, with the strongest associations for valuing (r = .43), humor (r = .33), and receptive listening (r = .27). Relative weights were significant only for valuing, humor, and receptive listening, suggesting that these are the most important predictors of relationship satisfaction. Results are discussed in terms of the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic regulation processes and the potential importance of motives for regulation. Supplementary Information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s12144-023-04432-4.

4.
Int J Eat Disord ; 54(11): 1956-1966, 2021 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34661291

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Toned muscularity continues to emerge as a salient aspect of women's body image. However, there is a dearth of research investigating the potentially maladaptive eating practices and related cognitions that accompany the drive for muscularity in women. This may be attributable to the limited empirical and clinical attention previously given to muscularity-oriented disordered eating and, accordingly, the lack of validated measures assessing these concerns. To address this knowledge gap, our study aimed to provide a preliminary evaluation of the factor structure and core psychometric properties of a recently developed measure of muscularity-oriented disordered eating, the Muscularity-Oriented Eating Test (MOET), in university women in Australia. METHOD: Participants included 419 university women who completed the 15-item MOET and other self-report measures for validity evaluation as part of an online survey. Data from split-half samples were used to undertake an exploratory factor analysis and subsequent confirmatory factor analysis. RESULTS: Factor analytic results supported a briefer (12-item), one-factor scale in this sample of university women. The internal consistency reliability and validity (convergent and discriminant) of this 12-item unidimensional MOET was supported. DISCUSSION: Our study provides preliminary support for this modified MOET as a psychometrically sound self-report measure of muscularity-oriented disordered eating in university women in Australia, providing a useful tool for understanding maladaptive eating behaviors and cognitions concomitant to the pursuit of muscularity in this population.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos , Universidades , Imagen Corporal , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos/diagnóstico , Femenino , Humanos , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
5.
Emotion ; 24(1): 234-240, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37498726

RESUMEN

To regulate others' emotions effectively we must learn about the efficacy of our regulation attempts. Deciding whether we made someone else feel better involves a causal judgment about the effect of our intervention on their emotional state. The current study examined whether, like other causal judgments, beliefs about emotion regulation efficacy are disproportionately affected by base rates. In two experiments, we showed that participants' perceived efficacy at helping a target regulate their emotions was more influenced by the target's average emotion levels than the relative effect of regulating versus not regulating the target's emotion. This led participants to conclude that they were helpful both when they were not (Experiment 1) and even when they made the target feel worse (Experiment 2). These findings suggest that our beliefs about the effectiveness of other-directed emotion regulation are notably biased by the average level of emotion expressed by the regulation target. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Regulación Emocional , Humanos , Emociones/fisiología , Aprendizaje , Juicio , Manejo de Datos
6.
PLoS One ; 18(3): e0282501, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36920917

RESUMEN

There is considerable variability in how successful people are in losing weight via exercise programs. Experimental research suggests that greater food intake after exercise may be one factor underlying this variability, but no studies have assessed patterns of post-exercise eating behaviour over time in naturalistic settings. Thus, we aimed to assess how exercise and contextual factors (e.g., hunger, presence of others) influence the healthiness and amount of food eaten after exercise in two daily diary studies. In Study 1, participants (n = 48) reported their food intake and exercise daily for 28 days. For each meal, they provided a brief description of the food(s) eaten which were then categorised as healthy, unhealthy, or mixed (neither healthy nor unhealthy) by two independent coders. Study 2 used the same method, but participants (n = 55) also reported the portion size of each meal. Hierarchical linear modelling showed that in Study 1, contrary to expectations, post-exercise meals were less likely to be unhealthy (relative to mixed) than were random meals from non-exercise days (OR = 0.63, p = .011), and that participants ate proportionally fewer unhealthy meals on exercise days compared to non-exercise days (b = -4.27, p = .004). Study 2 replicated these findings, and also found that participants consumed larger meals after exercise in comparison to random meals from non-exercise days (b = 0.25, p < .001). Participants were not consistently engaging in compensatory eating by eating less healthily after exercise compared to on non-exercise days, but they did eat larger portions post-exercise. This work highlights the need for naturalistic methods of assessing compensatory eating, and has the potential to facilitate development of strategies to improve health behaviour regulation.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos , Conducta Alimentaria , Humanos , Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiología , Hambre , Comidas , Factores de Tiempo , Ingestión de Energía
7.
Emotion ; 22(2): 397-402, 2022 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35073110

RESUMEN

Emotion regulation strategies have been characterized as adaptive or maladaptive; however, the ability to switch strategies to best suit the situation (regulatory flexibility and adaptability) underlies effective emotion regulation. Emotional intelligence may be a key capacity that enables flexible emotion regulation. We use experience sampling data from 165 participants to test whether emotional intelligence abilities (emotion understanding and management) predict variability in four emotion regulation strategies. Results show that both the emotion understanding and emotion management branches of emotional intelligence significantly relate to between-strategy variability (with moderate effect sizes), but only emotion understanding significantly predicts within-strategy variability. These findings support the hypothesis that emotional intelligence is an important predictor of the ability to flexibly vary emotion regulation depending on the situation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Regulación Emocional , Emociones , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Inteligencia Emocional , Emociones/fisiología , Humanos , Cuidados Paliativos
8.
Body Image ; 42: 276-286, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35841701

RESUMEN

Today, many women are driven to subscribe to a more athletically oriented appearance ideal hallmarked by muscle tone (referred to as athletic- or fit-looking). However, the constellation of unhealthy eating and exercise behaviors that may accompany the pursuit of toned muscularity among women is not yet well characterized. To address this knowledge gap, our study evaluated the associations among the female drive for toned muscularity and both thinness-oriented disordered eating behaviors (e.g., dietary restriction) and muscularity-oriented disordered eating behaviors (e.g., excessive scrutiny of macro-nutrient values of food), as well as dimensions of compulsive exercise. Participants were 388 Australian undergraduate women who completed an online questionnaire including self-report measures of said constructs. Hierarchical regression models were used to determine whether the drive for toned muscularity accounted for unique variance in eating and exercise behaviors after adjusting for theoretically relevant covariates. Results indicated independent relationships between the drive for toned muscularity and several facets of thinness- and muscularity-oriented disordered eating, as well as compulsive exercise (e.g., exercise rigidity). Our findings further contribute to an emerging literature illustrating that women driven to attain a toned appearance may be vulnerable to engaging in a wide range of maladaptive eating and exercise practices.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos , Delgadez , Australia , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Ejercicio Compulsivo , Impulso (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Tono Muscular
9.
Body Image ; 38: 219-229, 2021 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33932884

RESUMEN

Appearance comparisons can negatively influence women's body image, but little is known about the potential impact of comparison targets. We conducted an ecological momentary assessment study in which female undergraduate students (N = 146) completed a brief online survey at five random times every day for five days. In this survey, participants were asked if they had made an appearance comparison. If so, they were asked who they compared themselves to (i.e., close peer, acquaintance, stranger, celebrity/model), how they rated compared to that person (i.e., more attractive, just as attractive, less attractive), and how attainable that person's appearance is to them. All participants then completed state measures of mood, appearance satisfaction, and intention to diet and exercise. Upward comparisons (i.e., to more attractive others) to all targets were associated with less appearance satisfaction, lower positive mood, and more thoughts of dieting and exercising than when no comparisons were made. There were indirect relationships between comparisons to celebrities/models versus all other targets and appearance satisfaction via perceived attainability of the target's appearance. These findings suggest that celebrities may be particularly harmful appearance comparison targets in women's everyday lives because their attractive appearance is perceived to be less personally attainable than other targets.


Asunto(s)
Imagen Corporal , Comparación Social , Afecto , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Femenino , Humanos , Satisfacción Personal
10.
Body Image ; 39: 232-236, 2021 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34628232

RESUMEN

Regular weighing is a routine component of public health interventions but concerns have been raised about possible negative psychological consequences. Blind weighing is an alternative form of weighing that is commonly used in clinical contexts, and that is thought to decrease weighing anxiety and engagement with disordered eating behaviours. In this study, normal weight female participants who indicated a high drive for thinness (N = 53) were randomly assigned to receive bi-weekly open or blind weighing for three weeks. Participants who were open weighed felt increasingly anxious over time, whereas participants who were blind weighed felt less anxious. Both groups experienced increased weight preoccupation and disordered eating behaviours on weighing days compared to non-weighing days. Analyses of participants' qualitative responses further indicated that open weighed participants experienced increased urges to engage in weight-controlling behaviours throughout the experiment whereas blind weighed participants reported reduced concern with weight. Findings suggest that blind weighing may be a safer approach to weight monitoring, even though weighing in both forms can have a (transient) negative effect. Future research should evaluate whether the current findings are generalisable for other subgroups of the population.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos , Delgadez , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Peso Corporal , Impulso (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Aumento de Peso
11.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 97(2): 256-78, 2009 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19634974

RESUMEN

A model of the commitment-insurance system is proposed to examine how low and high self-esteem people cope with the costs interdependence imposes on autonomous goal pursuits. In this system, autonomy costs automatically activate compensatory cognitive processes that attach greater value to the partner. Greater partner valuing compels greater responsiveness to the partner's needs. Two experiments and a daily diary study of newlyweds supported the model. Autonomy costs automatically activate more positive implicit evaluations of the partner. On explicit measures of positive illusions, high self-esteem people continue to compensate for costs. However, cost-primed low self-esteem people correct and override their positive implicit sentiments when they have the opportunity to do so. Such corrections put the marriages of low self-esteem people at risk: Failing to compensate for costs predicted declines in satisfaction over a 1-year period.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Codependencia Psicológica/fisiología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Matrimonio/psicología , Autonomía Personal , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Cognición/fisiología , Femenino , Objetivos , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , New York , Satisfacción Personal , Autoimagen , Estudiantes/psicología , Adulto Joven
12.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 95(5): 1180-201, 2008 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18954201

RESUMEN

The authors examined the frequency, direction, and impact of social comparisons between romantic partners. Comparisons were expected to occur on a daily basis, owing to regular interactions between partners. To the extent that one empathizes and shares outcomes with one's partner, one might respond more positively to upward than to downward comparisons. Study 1a was an experience-sampling study in which participants reported comparisons made to their spouse over 2 weeks. Study 1b examined reactions to the most significant comparisons made during the experience-sampling study. Participants reported making comparisons to their romantic partner more than once a day on average and experienced more positive responses to upward than to downward comparisons. Study 2 demonstrated that participants empathized and shared outcomes with their partner to a greater extent than with a friend. Study 3 confirmed that participants responded more positively to upward than to downward comparisons even for domains high in self-relevance and even when the comparison had negative self-evaluative implications. These results suggest that, owing to higher levels of empathy and shared fate with partners, comparisons function differently in romantic than in other relationships.


Asunto(s)
Dominación-Subordinación , Empatía , Relaciones Interpersonales , Matrimonio/psicología , Autoimagen , Deseabilidad Social , Adaptación Psicológica , Adulto , Computadoras de Mano , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
13.
Obesity (Silver Spring) ; 26(4): 755-761, 2018 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29427370

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Weight-based stigmatization is associated with negative psychological and behavioral consequences, but individuals respond to stigma in different ways. The present study aimed to understand some of the factors that predict how one will cope with weight stigma and how different coping responses predict psychological well-being. METHODS: Across four samples, 1,391 individuals who identified as having overweight or obesity completed surveys assessing the frequency of weight stigma experiences, internalized weight bias, coping responses to weight stigma, and psychological distress. RESULTS: Frequency of weight stigma predicted greater internalized weight bias, which predicted more frequent use of maladaptive coping responses ("disengagement coping") and less frequent use of adaptive coping responses ("reappraisal coping"), in turn predicting more depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms. CONCLUSIONS: The more that individuals with overweight or obesity experience weight stigma and internalize weight bias, the more they report using maladaptive coping and the less they report using adaptive coping when dealing with weight stigma. Maladaptive coping is strongly associated with poorer psychological well-being. Thus, those who experience more frequent weight stigma may be more vulnerable to psychological distress because they appear to be at greater risk of employing maladaptive coping strategies.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Peso Corporal/fisiología , Obesidad/psicología , Sobrepeso/psicología , Estigma Social , Adulto , Sesgo , Mecanismos de Defensa , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
14.
Body Image ; 20: 31-39, 2017 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27907812

RESUMEN

Appearance comparisons are an important sociocultural factor influencing women's body image. These comparisons can occur in different contexts (e.g., through magazines, social media, in person). However, little is known about the frequency and outcome of appearance comparisons made in different contexts in women's everyday lives. Using Ecological Momentary Assessment methods, female undergraduate students (n=146) completed a brief online survey at random times every day for 5 days. They reported the frequency, direction (upward, lateral, downward), and context of appearance comparisons, and also reported their appearance satisfaction, mood, and diet and exercise thoughts and behaviors. Upward appearance comparisons were the most common across all contexts. Upward comparisons through social media were associated with more negative outcomes on all measures (except diet and exercise behavior) than comparisons made in person, and with more negative mood than comparisons in any other context. These findings highlight the importance of the appearance comparison context.


Asunto(s)
Imagen Corporal/psicología , Medios de Comunicación de Masas , Medios de Comunicación Sociales , Percepción Social , Adulto , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Femenino , Humanos , Adulto Joven
15.
J Exp Soc Psychol ; 49(3): 522-533, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25013236

RESUMEN

A contextual model of self-protection is proposed to explain when adhering to cautious "if-then" rules in daily interaction erodes marital satisfaction. People can self-protect against partner non-responsiveness by distancing when a partner seems rejecting, promoting a partner's dependence when feeling unworthy, or by devaluing a partner in the face of costs. The model implies that being less trusting elicits self-protection, and that mismatches between self-protective practices and encountered risk accelerate declines in satisfaction. A longitudinal study of newlyweds revealed that the fit between self-protection practices and risk predicted declines in satisfaction over three years. When people self-protected more initially, satisfaction declined more in low-risk (i.e., low conflict, resilient partner) than high-risk relationships (i.e., high conflict, vulnerable partner). However, when people self-protected less initially, satisfaction declined more in high-risk than low-risk relationships. Process evidence was consistent with moderated mediation: In low-risk relationships only, being less trusting predicted higher levels of self-protective caution that forecast later declines in satisfaction.

16.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 37(9): 1259-73, 2011 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21617021

RESUMEN

The present research examined the impact of everyday romantic goal strivings on women's attitudes toward science, technology, engineering, and math (STEM). It was hypothesized that women may distance themselves from STEM when the goal to be romantically desirable is activated because pursuing intelligence goals in masculine domains (i.e., STEM) conflicts with pursuing romantic goals associated with traditional romantic scripts and gender norms. Consistent with hypotheses, women, but not men, who viewed images (Study 1) or overheard conversations (Studies 2a-2b) related to romantic goals reported less positive attitudes toward STEM and less preference for majoring in math/science compared to other disciplines. On days when women pursued romantic goals, the more romantic activities they engaged in and the more desirable they felt, but the fewer math activities they engaged in. Furthermore, women's previous day romantic goal strivings predicted feeling more desirable but being less invested in math on the following day (Study 3).


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Objetivos , Amor , Matemática , Ciencia , Conflicto Psicológico , Escolaridad , Femenino , Identidad de Género , Humanos , Inteligencia , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Adulto Joven
17.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 101(3): 485-502, 2011 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21443370

RESUMEN

A dual process model is proposed to explain how automatic evaluative associations to the partner (i.e., impulsive trust) and deliberative expectations of partner caring (i.e., reflective trust) interact to govern self-protection in romantic relationships. Experimental and correlational studies of dating and marital relationships supported the model. Subliminally conditioning more positive evaluative associations to the partner increased confidence in the partner's caring, suggesting that trust has an impulsive basis. Being high on impulsive trust (i.e., more positive evaluative associations to the partner on the Implicit Association Test; Zayas & Shoda, 2005) also reduced the automatic inclination to distance in response to doubts about the partner's trustworthiness. It similarly reduced self-protective behavioral reactions to these reflective trust concerns. The studies further revealed that the effects of impulsive trust depend on working memory capacity: Being high on impulsive trust inoculated against reflective trust concerns for people low on working memory capacity.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Conducta Impulsiva/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Confianza/psicología , Adulto , Cortejo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Matrimonio/psicología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estimulación Subliminal , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Pruebas de Asociación de Palabras/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto Joven
18.
J Exp Soc Psychol ; 46(4): 650-656, 2010 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20526450

RESUMEN

The paper examines potential origins of automatic (i.e., unconscious) attitudes toward one's marital partner. It tests the hypothesis that early experiences in conflict-of-interest situations predict one's later automatic inclination to approach (or avoid) the partner. A longitudinal study linked daily experiences in conflict-of-interest situations in the initial months of new marriages to automatic evaluations of the partner assessed four years later using the Implicit Associations Test. The results revealed that partners who were initially (1) treated less responsively and (2) evidenced more self-protective and less connectedness-promoting "if-then" contingencies in their thoughts and behavior later evidenced less positive automatic partner attitudes. However, these factors did not predict changes in love, satisfaction, or explicit beliefs about the partner. The findings hint at the existence of a "smart" relationship unconscious that captures behavioral realities conscious reflection can miss.

19.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 97(6): 1142-56, 2009 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19968424

RESUMEN

In light of consistently observed correlations among Big Five ratings, the authors developed and tested a model that combined E. L. Thorndike's (1920) general evaluative bias (halo) model and J. M. Digman's (1997) higher order personality factors (alpha and beta) model. With 4 multitrait-multimethod analyses, Study 1 revealed moderate convergent validity for alpha and beta across raters, whereas halo was mainly a unique factor for each rater. In Study 2, the authors showed that the halo factor was highly correlated with a validated measure of evaluative biases in self-ratings. Study 3 showed that halo is more strongly correlated with self-ratings of self-esteem than self-ratings of the Big Five, which suggests that halo is not a mere rating bias but actually reflects overly positive self-evaluations. Finally, Study 4 demonstrated that the halo bias in Big Five ratings is stable over short retest intervals. Taken together, the results suggest that the halo-alpha-beta model integrates the main findings in structural analyses of Big Five correlations. Accordingly, halo bias in self-ratings is a reliable and stable bias in individuals' perceptions of their own attributes. Implications of the present findings for the assessment of Big Five personality traits in monomethod studies are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Psicológicos , Determinación de la Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Personalidad/fisiología , Análisis Discriminante , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Teoría Psicológica , Psicometría/métodos , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Autoimagen , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Estudiantes/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
20.
J Exp Soc Psychol ; 45(6): 1180-1191, 2009 Nov 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20161401

RESUMEN

It is proposed that people are motivated to feel hard to replace in romantic relationships because feeling irreplaceable fosters trust in a partner's continued responsiveness. By contrast, feeling replaceable motivates compensatory behavior aimed at strengthening the partner's commitment to the relationship. A correlational study of dating couples and 2 experiments examined how satiating/thwarting the goal of feeling irreplaceable differentially affects relationship perception and behavior for low and high self-esteem people. The results revealed that satiating the goal of feeling irreplaceable increases trust for people low in self-esteem. In contrast, thwarting the goal of feeling irreplaceable increases compensatory behaviors meant to prove one's indispensability for people high in self-esteem.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA