Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 47
Filtrar
1.
J Neurosci ; 43(44): 7393-7428, 2023 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37734947

RESUMEN

Larvae of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster are a powerful study case for understanding the neural circuits underlying behavior. Indeed, the numerical simplicity of the larval brain has permitted the reconstruction of its synaptic connectome, and genetic tools for manipulating single, identified neurons allow neural circuit function to be investigated with relative ease and precision. We focus on one of the most complex neurons in the brain of the larva (of either sex), the GABAergic anterior paired lateral neuron (APL). Using behavioral and connectomic analyses, optogenetics, Ca2+ imaging, and pharmacology, we study how APL affects associative olfactory memory. We first provide a detailed account of the structure, regional polarity, connectivity, and metamorphic development of APL, and further confirm that optogenetic activation of APL has an inhibiting effect on its main targets, the mushroom body Kenyon cells. All these findings are consistent with the previously identified function of APL in the sparsening of sensory representations. To our surprise, however, we found that optogenetically activating APL can also have a strong rewarding effect. Specifically, APL activation together with odor presentation establishes an odor-specific, appetitive, associative short-term memory, whereas naive olfactory behavior remains unaffected. An acute, systemic inhibition of dopamine synthesis as well as an ablation of the dopaminergic pPAM neurons impair reward learning through APL activation. Our findings provide a study case of complex circuit function in a numerically simple brain, and suggest a previously unrecognized capacity of central-brain GABAergic neurons to engage in dopaminergic reinforcement.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The single, identified giant anterior paired lateral (APL) neuron is one of the most complex neurons in the insect brain. It is GABAergic and contributes to the sparsening of neuronal activity in the mushroom body, the memory center of insects. We provide the most detailed account yet of the structure of APL in larval Drosophila as a neurogenetically accessible study case. We further reveal that, contrary to expectations, the experimental activation of APL can exert a rewarding effect, likely via dopaminergic reward pathways. The present study both provides an example of unexpected circuit complexity in a numerically simple brain, and reports an unexpected effect of activity in central-brain GABAergic circuits.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila melanogaster , Drosophila , Animales , Drosophila/fisiología , Larva/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología , Neuronas GABAérgicas/fisiología , Interneuronas , Dopamina , Recompensa , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología
2.
Nature ; 548(7666): 175-182, 2017 08 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28796202

RESUMEN

Associating stimuli with positive or negative reinforcement is essential for survival, but a complete wiring diagram of a higher-order circuit supporting associative memory has not been previously available. Here we reconstruct one such circuit at synaptic resolution, the Drosophila larval mushroom body. We find that most Kenyon cells integrate random combinations of inputs but that a subset receives stereotyped inputs from single projection neurons. This organization maximizes performance of a model output neuron on a stimulus discrimination task. We also report a novel canonical circuit in each mushroom body compartment with previously unidentified connections: reciprocal Kenyon cell to modulatory neuron connections, modulatory neuron to output neuron connections, and a surprisingly high number of recurrent connections between Kenyon cells. Stereotyped connections found between output neurons could enhance the selection of learned behaviours. The complete circuit map of the mushroom body should guide future functional studies of this learning and memory centre.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/citología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Conectoma , Drosophila melanogaster/citología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Animales , Retroalimentación Fisiológica , Femenino , Larva/citología , Larva/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/citología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas , Sinapsis/metabolismo
3.
PLoS Biol ; 17(1): e2006012, 2019 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30629594

RESUMEN

Oviparous animals across many taxa have evolved diverse strategies that deter egg predation, providing valuable tests of how natural selection mitigates direct fitness loss. Communal egg laying in nonsocial species minimizes egg predation. However, in cannibalistic species, this very behavior facilitates egg predation by conspecifics (cannibalism). Similarly, toxins and aposematic signaling that deter egg predators are often inefficient against resistant conspecifics. Egg cannibalism can be adaptive, wherein cannibals may benefit through reduced competition and added nutrition, but since it reduces Darwinian fitness, the evolution of anticannibalistic strategies is rife. However, such strategies are likely to be nontoxic because deploying toxins against related individuals would reduce inclusive fitness. Here, we report how D. melanogaster use specific hydrocarbons to chemically mask their eggs from cannibal larvae. Using an integrative approach combining behavioral, sensory, and mass spectrometry methods, we demonstrate that maternally provisioned pheromone 7,11-heptacosadiene (7,11-HD) in the eggshell's wax layer deters egg cannibalism. Furthermore, we show that 7,11-HD is nontoxic, can mask underlying substrates (for example, yeast) when coated upon them, and its detection requires pickpocket 23 (ppk23) gene function. Finally, using light and electron microscopy, we demonstrate how maternal pheromones leak-proof the egg, consequently concealing it from conspecific larvae. Our data suggest that semiochemicals possibly subserve in deceptive functions across taxa, especially when predators rely on chemical cues to forage, and stimulate further research on deceptive strategies mediated through nonvisual sensory modules. This study thus highlights how integrative approaches can illuminate our understanding on the adaptive significance of deceptive defenses and the mechanisms through which they operate.


Asunto(s)
Alcadienos/metabolismo , Óvulo/fisiología , Feromonas/metabolismo , Animales , Canibalismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Femenino , Larva , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Conducta Sexual Animal/fisiología
4.
J Neurosci ; 40(31): 5990-6006, 2020 07 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32586949

RESUMEN

An adaptive transition from exploring the environment in search of vital resources to exploiting these resources once the search was successful is important to all animals. Here we study the neuronal circuitry that allows larval Drosophila melanogaster of either sex to negotiate this exploration-exploitation transition. We do so by combining Pavlovian conditioning with high-resolution behavioral tracking, optogenetic manipulation of individually identified neurons, and EM data-based analyses of synaptic organization. We find that optogenetic activation of the dopaminergic neuron DAN-i1 can both establish memory during training and acutely terminate learned search behavior in a subsequent recall test. Its activation leaves innate behavior unaffected, however. Specifically, DAN-i1 activation can establish associative memories of opposite valence after paired and unpaired training with odor, and its activation during the recall test can terminate the search behavior resulting from either of these memories. Our results further suggest that in its behavioral significance DAN-i1 activation resembles, but does not equal, sugar reward. Dendrogram analyses of all the synaptic connections between DAN-i1 and its two main targets, the Kenyon cells and the mushroom body output neuron MBON-i1, further suggest that the DAN-i1 signals during training and during the recall test could be delivered to the Kenyon cells and to MBON-i1, respectively, within previously unrecognized, locally confined branching structures. This would provide an elegant circuit motif to terminate search on its successful completion.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In the struggle for survival, animals have to explore their environment in search of food. Once food is found, however, it is adaptive to prioritize exploiting it over continuing a search that would now be as pointless as searching for the glasses you are wearing. This exploration-exploitation trade-off is important for animals and humans, as well as for technical search devices. We investigate which of the only 10,000 neurons of a fruit fly larva can tip the balance in this trade-off, and identify a single dopamine neuron called DAN-i1 that can do so. Given the similarities in dopamine neuron function across the animal kingdom, this may reflect a general principle of how search is terminated once it is successful.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Neuronas Dopaminérgicas/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Animales , Condicionamiento Clásico , Drosophila melanogaster , Femenino , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Optogenética , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología , Sinapsis/fisiología
5.
J Neurogenet ; 34(1): 123-132, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31975653

RESUMEN

In many animals, the establishment and expression of food-related memory is limited by the presence of food and promoted by its absence, implying that this behavior is driven by motivation. In the past, this has already been demonstrated in various insects including honeybees and adult Drosophila. For Drosophila larvae, which are characterized by an immense growth and the resulting need for constant food intake, however, knowledge is rather limited. Accordingly, we have analyzed whether starvation modulates larval memory formation or expression after appetitive classical olfactory conditioning, in which an odor is associated with a sugar reward. We show that odor-sugar memory of starved larvae lasts longer than in fed larvae, although the initial performance is comparable. 80 minutes after odor fructose conditioning, only starved but not fed larvae show a reliable odor-fructose memory. This is likely due to a specific increase in the stability of anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM). Furthermore, we observe that starved larvae, in contrast to fed ones, prefer sugars that offer a nutritional benefit in addition to their sweetness. Taken together our work shows that Drosophila larvae adjust the expression of learned and naïve choice behaviors in the absence of food. These effects are only short-lasting probably due to their lifestyle and their higher internal motivation to feed. In the future, the extensive use of established genetic tools will allow us to identify development-specific differences arising at the neuronal and molecular level.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Apetitiva/fisiología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Animales , Drosophila melanogaster , Larva/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología
6.
Learn Mem ; 26(11): 424-435, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31615854

RESUMEN

Adjusting behavior to changed environmental contingencies is critical for survival, and reversal learning provides an experimental handle on such cognitive flexibility. Here, we investigate reversal learning in larval Drosophila Using odor-taste associations, we establish olfactory reversal learning in the appetitive and the aversive domain, using either fructose as a reward or high-concentration sodium chloride as a punishment, respectively. Reversal learning is demonstrated both in differential and in absolute conditioning, in either valence domain. In differential conditioning, the animals are first trained such that an odor A is paired, for example, with the reward whereas odor B is not (A+/B); this is followed by a second training phase with reversed contingencies (A/B+). In absolute conditioning, odor B is omitted, such that the animals are first trained with paired presentations of A and reward, followed by unpaired training in the second training phase. Our results reveal "true" reversal learning in that the opposite associative effects of both the first and the second training phase are detectable after reversed-contingency training. In what is a surprisingly quick, one-trial contingency adjustment in the Drosophila larva, the present study establishes a simple and genetically easy accessible study case of cognitive flexibility.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Condicionamiento Psicológico/fisiología , Drosophila/fisiología , Larva/fisiología , Aprendizaje Inverso/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Apetitiva/fisiología , Reacción de Prevención/fisiología , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Recompensa , Percepción del Gusto/fisiología
7.
PLoS Genet ; 12(10): e1006378, 2016 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27768692

RESUMEN

Memory formation is a highly complex and dynamic process. It consists of different phases, which depend on various neuronal and molecular mechanisms. In adult Drosophila it was shown that memory formation after aversive Pavlovian conditioning includes-besides other forms-a labile short-term component that consolidates within hours to a longer-lasting memory. Accordingly, memory formation requires the timely controlled action of different neuronal circuits, neurotransmitters, neuromodulators and molecules that were initially identified by classical forward genetic approaches. Compared to adult Drosophila, memory formation was only sporadically analyzed at its larval stage. Here we deconstruct the larval mnemonic organization after aversive olfactory conditioning. We show that after odor-high salt conditioning larvae form two parallel memory phases; a short lasting component that depends on cyclic adenosine 3'5'-monophosphate (cAMP) signaling and synapsin gene function. In addition, we show for the first time for Drosophila larvae an anesthesia resistant component, which relies on radish and bruchpilot gene function, protein kinase C activity, requires presynaptic output of mushroom body Kenyon cells and dopamine function. Given the numerical simplicity of the larval nervous system this work offers a unique prospect for studying memory formation of defined specifications, at full-brain scope with single-cell, and single-synapse resolution.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Olfato/genética , Sinapsis/genética , Sinapsinas/genética , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , AMP Cíclico , Dopamina/genética , Dopamina/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/genética , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Larva/genética , Larva/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/crecimiento & desarrollo , Cuerpos Pedunculados/metabolismo , Neuronas/metabolismo , Odorantes , Biosíntesis de Proteínas/genética , Proteína Quinasa C/biosíntesis , Proteína Quinasa C/genética , Olfato/fisiología , Sinapsis/enzimología , Sinapsis/metabolismo , Sinapsinas/biosíntesis
8.
J Exp Biol ; 220(Pt 13): 2452-2475, 2017 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28679796

RESUMEN

Mapping brain function to brain structure is a fundamental task for neuroscience. For such an endeavour, the Drosophila larva is simple enough to be tractable, yet complex enough to be interesting. It features about 10,000 neurons and is capable of various taxes, kineses and Pavlovian conditioning. All its neurons are currently being mapped into a light-microscopical atlas, and Gal4 strains are being generated to experimentally access neurons one at a time. In addition, an electron microscopic reconstruction of its nervous system seems within reach. Notably, this electron microscope-based connectome is being drafted for a stage 1 larva - because stage 1 larvae are much smaller than stage 3 larvae. However, most behaviour analyses have been performed for stage 3 larvae because their larger size makes them easier to handle and observe. It is therefore warranted to either redo the electron microscopic reconstruction for a stage 3 larva or to survey the behavioural faculties of stage 1 larvae. We provide the latter. In a community-based approach we called the Ol1mpiad, we probed stage 1 Drosophila larvae for free locomotion, feeding, responsiveness to substrate vibration, gentle and nociceptive touch, burrowing, olfactory preference and thermotaxis, light avoidance, gustatory choice of various tastants plus odour-taste associative learning, as well as light/dark-electric shock associative learning. Quantitatively, stage 1 larvae show lower scores in most tasks, arguably because of their smaller size and lower speed. Qualitatively, however, stage 1 larvae perform strikingly similar to stage 3 larvae in almost all cases. These results bolster confidence in mapping brain structure and behaviour across developmental stages.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Animales , Encéfalo/citología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/crecimiento & desarrollo , Larva/crecimiento & desarrollo , Larva/fisiología
9.
Cell Rep ; 43(1): 113640, 2024 01 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38180839

RESUMEN

Adhesion G-protein-coupled receptors (aGPCRs) form a large family of cell surface molecules with versatile tasks in organ development. Many aGPCRs still await their functional and pharmacological deorphanization. Here, we characterized the orphan aGPCR CG11318/mayo of Drosophila melanogaster and found it expressed in specific regions of the gastrointestinal canal and anal plates, epithelial specializations that control ion homeostasis. Genetic removal of mayo results in tachycardia, which is caused by hyperkalemia of the larval hemolymph. The hyperkalemic effect can be mimicked by a raise in ambient potassium concentration, while normal potassium levels in mayoKO mutants can be restored by pharmacological inhibition of potassium channels. Intriguingly, hyperkalemia and tachycardia are caused non-cell autonomously through mayo-dependent control of enterocyte proliferation in the larval midgut, which is the primary function of this aGPCR. These findings characterize the ancestral aGPCR Mayo as a homeostatic regulator of gut development.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila , Hiperpotasemia , Animales , Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster/metabolismo , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/metabolismo , Larva/metabolismo , Potasio/metabolismo , Taquicardia , Adhesión Celular
10.
J Neurosci ; 32(48): 17163-71, 2012 Nov 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23197709

RESUMEN

Memories are classified as consolidated (stable) or labile according to whether they withstand amnestic treatment, or not. In contrast to the general prevalence of this classification, its neuronal and molecular basis is poorly understood. Here, we focused on consolidated and labile memories induced after a single cycle training in the Drosophila aversive olfactory conditioning paradigm and we used mutants to define the impact of cAMP signals. At the biochemical level we report that cAMP signals misrelated in either rutabaga (rut) or dunce (dnc) mutants separate between consolidated anesthesia-resistant memory (ARM) and labile anesthesia-sensitive memory (ASM). Those functionally distinct cAMP signals act within different neuronal populations: while rut-dependent cAMP signals act within Kenyon cells (KCs) of the mushroom bodies to support ASM, dnc-sensitive cAMP signals support ARM within antennal lobe local neurons (LNs) and KCs. Collectively, different key positions along the olfactory circuitry seem to get modified during storage of ARM or ASM independently. A precise separation between those functionally distinct cAMP signals seems mandatory to allocate how they support appropriate memories.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Drosophila/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Adenilil Ciclasas/genética , Adenilil Ciclasas/metabolismo , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Reacción de Prevención/fisiología , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , AMP Cíclico/metabolismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Sinapsis/fisiología
11.
Elife ; 122023 03 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36867155

RESUMEN

The way neurons in the brain rewire in larvae as they turn to adult fruit flies sheds light on how complete metamorphosis was 'invented' over the course of evolution.


Asunto(s)
Artrópodos , Encéfalo , Animales , Frutas , Larva , Metamorfosis Biológica
12.
Cold Spring Harb Protoc ; 2023(3): 107863-pdb.top, 2023 03 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36180213

RESUMEN

The Drosophila larva has become an attractive model system for studying fundamental questions in neuroscience. Although the focus was initially on topics such as the structure of genes, mechanisms of inheritance, genetic regulation of development, and the function and physiology of ion channels, today it is often on the cellular and molecular principles of naive and learned behavior. Drosophila larvae have developed different mechanisms, often widespread in similar manifestations in the animal kingdom, to orient themselves toward olfactory, gustatory, mechanosensory, thermal, and visual stimuli to coordinate their locomotion appropriately. To adapt to changes in the environment, larvae are able to learn to categorize some of these sensory impressions as "good" or "bad." Depending on their relevance and reliability, the larva learns them and constantly updates these memories. Laboratory experiments allow us to parametrically study and describe many of these processes (e.g., olfactory appetitive and aversive memory or visual appetitive and aversive memory). Combining behavioral tests with various neurogenetic techniques allows us to thermally or optogenetically activate or inhibit individual cells during learning, memory consolidation, and memory retrieval. The molecular and genetic bases of larval learning can be analyzed by using specific mutants. The CRISPR-Cas method has established extensive new directions in this area, in addition to the already wide-ranging traditional approaches, like the GAL4/UAS system. The combination of these genetic methods with the simplicity and cost-effectiveness of the introduced behavioral assay provides a platform for discovering the fundamental mechanisms underlying learning and memory formation in the rather simple larval brain.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila , Memoria , Animales , Larva/fisiología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Memoria/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster
13.
Cold Spring Harb Protoc ; 2023(3): 108050-pdb.prot, 2023 03 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36180215

RESUMEN

Drosophila larvae are able to associate an odor stimulus with a temporally overlapping teaching signal encoding reward or punishment. Here, we describe a standardized experimental setup that allows the analysis of larval aversive-odor-taste learning and memory. This is a Pavlovian learning experiment with a single training trial in which larvae are presented with two specific odors in succession, one odor together with salt at a high concentration that is harmful to the larva. In the subsequent test, the trained larvae then show avoidance of the salt-paired odor and spend more time near the unpaired odor. To rule out nonassociative effects (such as naive preferences for odors, exposure, or handling effects), two independent groups of larvae are reciprocally trained. Subsequently, the average of the two individual preference values is determined and quantified as a Performance Index (PI), which assigns a numerical value to the larvae's shown behavioral change.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila , Gusto , Animales , Larva , Olfato , Odorantes , Drosophila melanogaster
14.
Sci Adv ; 9(36): eadh2301, 2023 09 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37683005

RESUMEN

In adulthood, sleep-wake rhythms are one of the most prominent behaviors under circadian control. However, during early life, sleep is spread across the 24-hour day. The mechanism through which sleep rhythms emerge, and consequent advantage conferred to a juvenile animal, is unknown. In the second-instar Drosophila larvae (L2), like in human infants, sleep is not under circadian control. We identify the precise developmental time point when the clock begins to regulate sleep in Drosophila, leading to emergence of sleep rhythms in early third-instars (L3). At this stage, a cellular connection forms between DN1a clock neurons and arousal-promoting Dh44 neurons, bringing arousal under clock control to drive emergence of circadian sleep. Last, we demonstrate that L3 but not L2 larvae exhibit long-term memory (LTM) of aversive cues and that this LTM depends upon deep sleep generated once sleep rhythms begin. We propose that the developmental emergence of circadian sleep enables more complex cognitive processes, including the onset of enduring memories.


Asunto(s)
Drosophila , Memoria a Largo Plazo , Animales , Lactante , Humanos , Afecto , Nivel de Alerta , Larva , Sueño
15.
Chem Senses ; 37(8): 711-21, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22695795

RESUMEN

Gustatory stimuli allow an organism not only to orient in its environment toward energy-rich food sources to maintain nutrition but also to avoid unpleasant or even poisonous substrates. For both mammals and insects, sugars-perceived as "sweet"-potentially predict nutritional benefit. Interestingly, even Drosophila adult flies are attracted to most high-potency sweeteners preferred by humans. However, the gustatory information of a sugar may be misleading as some sugars, although perceived as "sweet," cannot be metabolized. Accordingly, in adult Drosophila, a postingestive system that additionally evaluates the nutritional benefit of an ingested sugar has been shown to exist. By using a set of seven different sugars, which either offer (fructose, sucrose, glucose, maltodextrin, and sorbitol) or lack (xylose and arabinose) nutritional benefit, we show that Drosophila, at the larval stage, can perceive and evaluate sugars based on both nutrition-dependent and -independent qualities. In detail, we find that larval survival and feeding mainly depend on the nutritional value of a particular sugar. In contrast, larval choice behavior and learning are regulated in a more complex way by nutrition value-dependent and nutrition value-independent information. The simplicity of the larval neuronal circuits and their accessibility to genetic manipulation may ultimately allow one to identify the neuronal and molecular basis of the larval sugar perception systems described here behaviorally.


Asunto(s)
Carbohidratos/administración & dosificación , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Valor Nutritivo , Gusto/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Larva/fisiología , Neuronas/metabolismo , Odorantes/análisis , Percepción del Gusto
16.
J Neurosci ; 30(32): 10655-66, 2010 Aug 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20702697

RESUMEN

Insect mushroom bodies are required for diverse behavioral functions, including odor learning and memory. Using the numerically simple olfactory pathway of the Drosophila melanogaster larva, we provide evidence that the formation of appetitive olfactory associations relies on embryonic-born intrinsic mushroom body neurons (Kenyon cells). The participation of larval-born Kenyon cells, i.e., neurons that become gradually integrated in the developing mushroom body during larval life, in this task is unlikely. These data provide important insights into how a small set of identified Kenyon cells can store and integrate olfactory information in a developing brain. To investigate possible functional subdivisions of the larval mushroom body, we anatomically disentangle its input and output neurons at the single-cell level. Based on this approach, we define 10 subdomains of the larval mushroom body that may be implicated in mediating specific interactions between the olfactory pathway, modulatory neurons, and neuronal output.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Apetitiva/fisiología , Larva/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Cuerpos Pedunculados/citología , Neuronas/fisiología , Vías Olfatorias/fisiología , Animales , Animales Modificados Genéticamente , Conducta Apetitiva/efectos de los fármacos , Antígenos CD8/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Drosophila melanogaster , Embrión no Mamífero , Inhibidores Enzimáticos/farmacología , Regulación del Desarrollo de la Expresión Génica/efectos de los fármacos , Regulación del Desarrollo de la Expresión Génica/fisiología , Hidroxiurea/farmacología , Modelos Biológicos , Cuerpos Pedunculados/embriología , Neuronas/efectos de los fármacos , Vías Olfatorias/efectos de los fármacos , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos , Olfato/genética , Olfato/fisiología , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Sinapsis/metabolismo , Factores de Transcripción/genética
17.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 12307, 2021 06 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34112872

RESUMEN

Chemosensory signals allow vertebrates and invertebrates not only to orient in its environment toward energy-rich food sources to maintain nutrition but also to avoid unpleasant or even poisonous substrates. Ethanol is a substance found in the natural environment of Drosophila melanogaster. Accordingly, D. melanogaster has evolved specific sensory systems, physiological adaptations, and associated behaviors at its larval and adult stage to perceive and process ethanol. To systematically analyze how D. melanogaster larvae respond to naturally occurring ethanol, we examined ethanol-induced behavior in great detail by reevaluating existing approaches and comparing them with new experiments. Using behavioral assays, we confirm that larvae are attracted to different concentrations of ethanol in their environment. This behavior is controlled by olfactory and other environmental cues. It is independent of previous exposure to ethanol in their food. Moreover, moderate, naturally occurring ethanol concentration of 4% results in increased larval fitness. On the contrary, higher concentrations of 10% and 20% ethanol, which rarely or never appear in nature, increase larval mortality. Finally, ethanol also serves as a positive teaching signal in learning and memory and updates valence associated with simultaneously processed odor information. Since information on how larvae perceive and process ethanol at the genetic and neuronal level is limited, the establishment of standardized assays described here is an important step towards their discovery.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/efectos de los fármacos , Drosophila melanogaster/efectos de los fármacos , Etanol/farmacología , Larva/efectos de los fármacos , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Larva/fisiología , Aprendizaje/efectos de los fármacos , Neuronas/efectos de los fármacos , Odorantes/análisis , Olfato/fisiología
18.
Sci Adv ; 7(35)2021 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34452914

RESUMEN

Body temperature homeostasis is essential and reliant upon the integration of outputs from multiple classes of cooling- and warming-responsive cells. The computations that integrate these outputs are not understood. Here, we discover a set of warming cells (WCs) and show that the outputs of these WCs combine with previously described cooling cells (CCs) in a cross-inhibition computation to drive thermal homeostasis in larval Drosophila WCs and CCs detect temperature changes using overlapping combinations of ionotropic receptors: Ir68a, Ir93a, and Ir25a for WCs and Ir21a, Ir93a, and Ir25a for CCs. WCs mediate avoidance to warming while cross-inhibiting avoidance to cooling, and CCs mediate avoidance to cooling while cross-inhibiting avoidance to warming. Ambient temperature-dependent regulation of the strength of WC- and CC-mediated cross-inhibition keeps larvae near their homeostatic set point. Using neurophysiology, quantitative behavioral analysis, and connectomics, we demonstrate how flexible integration between warming and cooling pathways can orchestrate homeostatic thermoregulation.

19.
Chem Senses ; 35(4): 335-46, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20212010

RESUMEN

Associative plasticity is a basic essential attribute of nervous systems. As shown by numerous reports, Drosophila is able to establish simple forms of appetitive and aversive olfactory associations at both larval and adult stages. Whereas most adult studies on aversive learning employed electric shock as a negative reinforcer, larval paradigms essentially utilized gustatory stimuli to create negative associations, a discrepancy that limits the comparison of data. To overcome this drawback, we critically revisited larval odor-electric shock conditioning. First, we show that lithium chloride (LiCl), which was used in all previous larval electric shock paradigms, is not required per se in larval odor-electric shock learning. This is of considerable practical advantage because beside its peculiar effects LiCl is attractive to larvae at low concentration that renders comparative learning studies on genetically manipulated larvae complicated. Second, we confirm that in both a 2-odor reciprocal and a 1-odor nonreciprocal conditioning regimen, larvae are able to associate an odor with electric shock. In the latter experiments, initial learning scores reach an asymptote after 5 training trials, and aversive memory is still detectable after 60 min. Our experiments provide a comprehensive basis for future comparisons of larval olfactory conditioning reinforced by different modalities, for studies aimed at analyzing odor-electric shock learning in the larva and the adult, and for investigations of the cellular and molecular substrate of aversive olfactory learning in the simple Drosophila model.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Drosophila/fisiología , Electrochoque , Animales , Conducta Animal , Condicionamiento Psicológico/fisiología , Drosophila/crecimiento & desarrollo , Larva/fisiología , Cloruro de Litio/química , Cloruro de Litio/farmacología , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología
20.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 17614, 2020 10 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33077824

RESUMEN

Invertebrates such as Drosophila melanogaster have proven to be a valuable model organism for studies of the nervous system. In order to control neuronal activity, optogenetics has evolved as a powerful technique enabling non-invasive stimulation using light. This requires light sources that can deliver patterns of light with high temporal and spatial precision. Currently employed light sources for stimulation of small invertebrates, however, are either limited in spatial resolution or require sophisticated and bulky equipment. In this work, we used smartphone displays for optogenetic control of Drosophila melanogaster. We developed an open-source smartphone app that allows time-dependent display of light patterns and used this to activate and inhibit different neuronal populations in both larvae and adult flies. Characteristic behavioural responses were observed depending on the displayed colour and brightness and in agreement with the activation spectra and light sensitivity of the used channelrhodopsins. By displaying patterns of light, we constrained larval movement and were able to guide larvae on the display. Our method serves as a low-cost high-resolution testbench for optogenetic experiments using small invertebrate species and is particularly appealing to application in neuroscience teaching labs.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Drosophila melanogaster/fisiología , Optogenética/métodos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Teléfono Inteligente , Animales , Channelrhodopsins/genética , Neuronas/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA