Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
1.
Child Dev ; 91(1): 307-326, 2020 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30273981

RESUMEN

This study investigated the association between perceived material deprivation, children's behavior problems, and parents' disciplinary practices. The sample included 1,418 8- to 12-year-old children and their parents in China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States. Multilevel mixed- and fixed-effects regression models found that, even when income remained stable, perceived material deprivation was associated with children's externalizing behavior problems and parents' psychological aggression. Parents' disciplinary practices mediated a small share of the association between perceived material deprivation and children's behavior problems. There were no differences in these associations between mothers and fathers or between high- and low- and middle-income countries. These results suggest that material deprivation likely influences children's outcomes at any income level.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/etnología , Comparación Transcultural , Estatus Económico , Responsabilidad Parental/etnología , Problema de Conducta , Niño , China/etnología , Colombia/etnología , Femenino , Humanos , Italia/etnología , Jordania/etnología , Kenia/etnología , Masculino , Filipinas/etnología , Tailandia/etnología , Estados Unidos/etnología
2.
Law Hum Behav ; 43(1): 69-85, 2019 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30762417

RESUMEN

All countries distinguish between minors and adults for various legal purposes. Recent U.S. Supreme Court cases concerning the legal status of juveniles have consulted psychological science to decide where to draw these boundaries. However, little is known about the robustness of the relevant research, because it has been conducted largely in the U.S. and other Western countries. To the extent that lawmakers look to research to guide their decisions, it is important to know how generalizable the scientific conclusions are. The present study examines 2 psychological phenomena relevant to legal questions about adolescent maturity: cognitive capacity, which undergirds logical thinking, and psychosocial maturity, which comprises individuals' ability to restrain themselves in the face of emotional, exciting, or risky stimuli. Age patterns of these constructs were assessed in 5,227 individuals (50.7% female), ages 10-30 (M = 17.05, SD = 5.91) from 11 countries. Importantly, whereas cognitive capacity reached adult levels around age 16, psychosocial maturity reached adult levels beyond age 18, creating a "maturity gap" between cognitive and psychosocial development. Juveniles may be capable of deliberative decision making by age 16, but even young adults may demonstrate "immature" decision making in arousing situations. We argue it is therefore reasonable to have different age boundaries for different legal purposes: 1 for matters in which cognitive capacity predominates, and a later 1 for matters in which psychosocial maturity plays a substantial role. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Desarrollo del Adolescente/fisiología , Cognición , Menores/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Distribución por Edad , Niño , China , Cognición/fisiología , Colombia , Estudios Transversales , Chipre , Toma de Decisiones , Femenino , Humanos , India , Italia , Jordania , Kenia , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Menores/legislación & jurisprudencia , Filipinas , Psicología del Adolescente , Análisis de Regresión , Decisiones de la Corte Suprema , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Suecia , Tailandia , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
3.
Child Dev ; 88(4): 1100-1114, 2017 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28643844

RESUMEN

Bidirectional relations among adolescents' positivity, perceived positive school climate, and prosocial behavior were examined in Colombian youth. Also, the role of a positive school climate in mediating the relation of positivity to prosocial behaviors was tested. Adolescents (N = 151; Mage of child in Wave 1 = 12.68, SD = 1.06; 58.9% male) and their parents (N = 127) provided data in two waves (9 months apart). A model of bidirectional relations between positivity and perceived positive school climate emerged. In addition, adolescents with higher levels of perceived positive school climate at age 12 showed higher levels of prosocial behaviors in the following year. Positive school climate related positivity to adolescents' prosocial behavior over time.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/etnología , Conducta Infantil/etnología , Instituciones Académicas , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Niño , Colombia/etnología , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción Social
4.
Child Dev ; 88(5): 1598-1614, 2017 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28869665

RESUMEN

According to the dual systems model of adolescent risk taking, sensation seeking and impulse control follow different developmental trajectories across adolescence and are governed by two different brain systems. The authors tested whether different underlying processes also drive age differences in reward approach and cost avoidance. Using a modified Iowa Gambling Task in a multinational, cross-sectional sample of 3,234 adolescents (ages 9-17; M = 12.87, SD = 2.36), pubertal maturation, but not age, predicted reward approach, mediated through higher sensation seeking. In contrast, age, but not pubertal maturation, predicted increased cost avoidance, mediated through greater impulse control. These findings add to evidence that adolescent behavior is best understood as the product of two interacting, but independently developing, brain systems.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/fisiología , Conducta Infantil/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Conducta Impulsiva/fisiología , Pubertad/fisiología , Recompensa , Adolescente , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Int J Behav Dev ; 41(4): 491-502, 2017 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28729751

RESUMEN

There is strong evidence of a positive association between corporal punishment and negative child outcomes, but previous studies have suggested that the manner in which parents implement corporal punishment moderates the effects of its use. This study investigated whether severity and justness in the use of corporal punishment moderate the associations between frequency of corporal punishment and child externalizing and internalizing behaviors. This question was examined using a multicultural sample from eight countries and two waves of data collected one year apart. Interviews were conducted with 998 children aged 7-10 years, and their mothers and fathers, from China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines, Thailand, and the United States. Mothers and fathers responded to questions on the frequency, severity, and justness of their use of corporal punishment; they also reported on the externalizing and internalizing behavior of their child. Children reported on their aggression. Multigroup path models revealed that across cultural groups, and as reported by mothers and fathers, there is a positive relation between the frequency of corporal punishment and externalizing child behaviors. Mother-reported severity and father-reported justness were associated with child-reported aggression. Neither severity nor justness moderated the relation between frequency of corporal punishment and child problem behavior. The null result suggests that more use of corporal punishment is harmful to children regardless of how it is implemented, but requires further substantiation as the study is unable to definitively conclude that there is no true interaction effect.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA