Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 37
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Int J Psychol ; 2024 Feb 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38418410

RESUMEN

This study examined associations of mothers' and fathers' individualism, collectivism and conformity values with parenting (warmth, rules/limit-setting, knowledge solicitation and expectations regarding children's family obligations) and child internalising and externalising behaviours in Colombia. Mothers, fathers and children (N = 100) from Medellín, Colombia were interviewed when children were, on average, 10 years old. Higher maternal collectivism and conformity values were associated with higher maternal warmth and fewer child externalising problems, whereas higher paternal collectivism was associated with lower maternal warmth and more child externalising problems. Fathers' cultural values also were related to their expectations regarding children's family obligations. The findings suggest differences in how mothers' and fathers' cultural values are related to parenting and child adjustment in Colombia, as well as the need to examine cultural values beyond individualism, collectivism and conformity values.

2.
Int J Psychol ; 2024 Apr 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38622493

RESUMEN

This study investigated how individualism, collectivism and conformity are associated with parenting and child adjustment in 1297 families with 10-year-old children from 13 cultural groups in nine countries. With multilevel models disaggregating between- and within-culture effects, we examined between- and within-culture associations between maternal and paternal cultural values, parenting dimensions and children's adjustment. Mothers from cultures endorsing higher collectivism and fathers from cultures endorsing lower individualism engage more frequently in warm parenting behaviours. Mothers and fathers with higher-than-average collectivism in their culture reported higher parent warmth and expectations for children's family obligations. Mothers with higher-than-average collectivism in their cultures more frequently reported warm parenting and fewer externalising problems in children, whereas mothers with higher-than-average individualism in their culture reported more child adjustment problems. Mothers with higher-than-average conformity values in their culture reported more father-displays of warmth and greater mother-reported expectations for children's family obligations. Fathers with higher-than-average individualism in their culture reported setting more rules and soliciting more knowledge about their children's whereabouts. Fathers who endorsed higher-than-average conformity in their culture displayed more warmth and expectations for children's family obligations and granted them more autonomy. Being connected to an interdependent, cohesive group appears to relate to parenting and children's adjustment.

3.
Dev Psychopathol ; 35(3): 1203-1218, 2023 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34895387

RESUMEN

Prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, adolescents (N = 1,330; Mages = 15 and 16; 50% female), mothers, and fathers from nine countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, United States) reported on adolescents' internalizing and externalizing problems, adolescents completed a lab-based task to assess tendency for risk-taking, and adolescents reported on their well-being. During the pandemic, participants (Mage = 20) reported on changes in their internalizing, externalizing, and substance use compared to before the pandemic. Across countries, adolescents' internalizing problems pre-pandemic predicted increased internalizing during the pandemic, and poorer well-being pre-pandemic predicted increased externalizing and substance use during the pandemic. Other relations varied across countries, and some were moderated by confidence in the government's handling of the pandemic, gender, and parents' education.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19 , Ajuste Emocional , Control Interno-Externo , Internacionalidad , COVID-19/epidemiología , Humanos , Análisis de Mediación , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/psicología , Masculino , Femenino , Adolescente , Adulto Joven
4.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 31(6): 947-957, 2022 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33547952

RESUMEN

This longitudinal study examined the unique and joint effects of early adolescent temperament and parenting in predicting the development of adolescent internalizing symptoms in a cross-cultural sample. Participants were 544 early adolescents (T1: Mage = 12.58; 49.5% female) and their mothers (n = 530) from Medellín, Colombia (n = 88), Naples, Italy (n = 90), Rome, Italy (n = 100) and Durham, North Carolina, United States (African Americans n = 92, European Americans n = 97, and Latinx n = 77). Early adolescent negative emotionality (i.e., anger and sadness experience), self-regulation (i.e., effortful control), and parent monitoring and psychological control were measured at T1. Adolescent internalizing symptoms were measured at three time points. Latent Growth Curve Modeling (LGCM) without covariates or predictors indicated a slight linear increase in internalizing symptoms from ages 13-16 years across nearly all cultural groups. Multi-group LGCMs demonstrated several paths were consistently invariant across groups when examining how well temperament and parenting predicted intercept and slope factors. Higher initial levels of internalizing symptoms were significantly predicted by higher adolescent negative emotionality and parental psychological control as well as lower adolescent effortful control and parental monitoring measured one year earlier. Overall, adolescent effortful control appeared to protect against the emergence of internalizing symptoms in all cultures, but this effect faded over time. This study advances knowledge of the normative development of internalizing symptoms during adolescence across cultures while highlighting the predictive value of early adolescent temperament and parenting.


Asunto(s)
Responsabilidad Parental , Temperamento , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Madres/psicología , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Estados Unidos
5.
Prev Sci ; 2022 Jul 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35857257

RESUMEN

Longitudinal data from the Parenting Across Cultures study of children, mothers, and fathers in 12 cultural groups in nine countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the USA; N = 1331 families) were used to understand predictors of compliance with COVID-19 mitigation strategies and vaccine hesitancy. Confidence in government responses to the COVID pandemic was also examined as a potential moderator of links between pre-COVID risk factors and compliance with COVID mitigation strategies and vaccine hesitancy. Greater confidence in government responses to the COVID pandemic was associated with greater compliance with COVID mitigation strategies and less vaccine hesitancy across cultures and reporters. Pre-COVID financial strain and family stress were less consistent predictors of compliance with COVID mitigation strategies and vaccine hesitancy than confidence in government responses to the pandemic. Findings suggest the importance of bolstering confidence in government responses to future human ecosystem disruptions, perhaps through consistent, clear, non-partisan messaging and transparency in acknowledging limitations and admitting mistakes to inspire compliance with government and public health recommendations.

6.
Child Dev ; 92(4): e493-e512, 2021 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33521940

RESUMEN

Children, mothers, and fathers in 12 ethnic and regional groups in nine countries (N = 1,338 families) were interviewed annually for 8 years (Mage child = 8-16 years) to model four domains of parenting as a function of child age, puberty, or both. Latent growth curve models revealed that for boys and girls, parents decrease their warmth, behavioral control, rules/limit-setting, and knowledge solicitation in conjunction with children's age and pubertal status as children develop from ages 8 to 16 across a range of diverse contexts, with steeper declines after age 11 or 12 in three of the four parenting domains. National, ethnic, and regional differences and similarities in the trajectories as a function of age and puberty are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Madres , Responsabilidad Parental , Adolescente , Niño , Padre , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pubertad
7.
Child Dev ; 92(6): e1138-e1153, 2021 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34291830

RESUMEN

Families from nine countries (N = 1,338) were interviewed annually seven times (Mage child = 7-15) to test specificity and commonality in parenting behaviors associated with child flourishing and moderation of associations by normativeness of parenting. Participants included 1,338 children (M = 8.59 years, SD = 0.68, range = 7-11 years; 50% girls), their mothers (N = 1,283, M = 37.04 years, SD = 6.51, range = 19-70 years), and their fathers (N = 1,170, M = 40.19 years, SD = 6.75, range = 22-76 years) at Wave 1 of 7 annual waves collected between 2008 and 2017. Families were recruited from 12 ethnocultural groups in nine countries including: Shanghai, China (n = 123); Medellín, Colombia (n = 108); Naples (n = 102) and Rome (n = 111), Italy; Zarqa, Jordan (n = 114); Kisumu, Kenya (n = 100); Manila, Philippines (n = 120); Trollhättan & Vänersborg, Sweden (n = 129); Chiang Mai, Thailand (n = 120); and Durham, NC, United States (n = 110 White, n = 102 Black, n = 99 Latinx). Intergenerational parenting (parenting passed from Generation 1 to Generation 2) demonstrated specificity. Children from cultures with above-average G2 parent warmth experienced the most benefit from the intergenerational transmission of warmth, whereas children from cultures with below-average G2 hostility, neglect, and rejection were best protected from deleterious intergenerational effects of parenting behaviors on flourishing. Single-generation parenting (Generation 2 parenting directly associated with Generation 3 flourishing) demonstrated commonality. Parent warmth promoted, and parent hostility, neglect, and rejection impeded the development of child flourishing largely regardless of parenting norms.


Asunto(s)
Comparación Transcultural , Responsabilidad Parental , Niño , China , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Filipinas , Estados Unidos
8.
J Res Adolesc ; 30(4): 835-855, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32609411

RESUMEN

We investigated the effects of parental warmth and behavioral control on externalizing and internalizing symptom trajectories from ages 8 to 14 in 1,298 adolescents from 12 cultural groups. We did not find that single universal trajectories characterized adolescent externalizing and internalizing symptoms across cultures, but instead found significant heterogeneity in starting points and rates of change in both externalizing and internalizing symptoms across cultures. Some similarities did emerge. Across many cultural groups, internalizing symptoms decreased from ages 8 to 10, and externalizing symptoms increased from ages 10 to 14. Parental warmth appears to function similarly in many cultures as a protective factor that prevents the onset and growth of adolescent externalizing and internalizing symptoms, whereas the effects of behavioral control vary from culture to culture.


Asunto(s)
Control de la Conducta , Padres , Adolescente , Niño , Humanos , Factores Protectores
9.
J Youth Adolesc ; 49(6): 1225-1244, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32166654

RESUMEN

Internalizing and externalizing problems increase during adolescence. However, these problems may be mitigated by adequate parenting, including effective parent-adolescent communication. The ways in which parent-driven (i.e., parent behavior control and solicitation) and adolescent-driven (i.e., disclosure and secrecy) communication efforts are linked to adolescent psychological problems universally and cross-culturally is a question that needs more empirical investigation. The current study used a sample of 1087 adolescents (M = 13.19 years, SD = 0.90, 50% girls) from 12 cultural groups in nine countries including China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States to test the cultural moderation of links between parent solicitation, parent behavior control, adolescent disclosure, and adolescent secrecy with adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. The results indicate that adolescent-driven communication, and secrecy in particular, is intertwined with adolescents' externalizing problems across all cultures, and intertwined with internalizing problems in specific cultural contexts. Moreover, parent-driven communication efforts were predicted by adolescent disclosure in all cultures. Overall, the findings suggest that adolescent-driven communication efforts, and adolescent secrecy in particular, are important predictors of adolescent psychological problems as well as facilitators of parent-adolescent communication.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Mecanismos de Defensa , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Psicología del Adolescente , Adolescente , China , Colombia , Comunicación , Comparación Transcultural , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Jordania , Kenia , Masculino , Padres/psicología , Filipinas , Ajuste Social , Suecia , Tailandia , Estados Unidos
10.
New Dir Child Adolesc Dev ; 2020(172): 73-88, 2020 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32964604

RESUMEN

This study tested culture-general and culture-specific aspects of adolescent developmental processes by focusing on opportunities and peer support for aggressive and delinquent behavior, which could help account for cultural similarities and differences in problem behavior during adolescence. Adolescents from 12 cultural groups in 9 countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States) provided data at ages 12, 14, and 15. Variance in opportunities and peer support for aggression and delinquency, as well as aggressive and delinquent behavior, was greater within than between cultures. Across cultural groups, opportunities and peer support for aggression and delinquency increased from early to mid-adolescence. Consistently across diverse cultural groups, opportunities and peer support for aggression and delinquency predicted subsequent aggressive and delinquent behavior, even after controlling for prior aggressive and delinquent behavior. The findings illustrate ways that international collaborative research can contribute to developmental science by embedding the study of development within cultural contexts.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/etnología , Desarrollo del Adolescente , Agresión , Delincuencia Juvenil/etnología , Grupo Paritario , Apoyo Social , Adolescente , Niño , China/etnología , Colombia/etnología , Comparación Transcultural , Femenino , Humanos , Italia/etnología , Jordania/etnología , Kenia/etnología , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Filipinas/etnología , Suecia/etnología , Tailandia/etnología , Estados Unidos/etnología
11.
Proc Biol Sci ; 286(1917): 20192097, 2019 12 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31847773

RESUMEN

The external environment has traditionally been considered as the primary driver of animal life history (LH). Recent research suggests that animals' internal state is also involved, especially in forming LH behavioural phenotypes. The present study investigated how these two factors interact in formulating LH in humans. Based on a longitudinal sample of 1223 adolescents in nine countries, the results show that harsh and unpredictable environments and adverse internal states in childhood are each uniquely associated with fast LH behavioural profiles consisting of aggression, impulsivity, and risk-taking in adolescence. The external environment and internal state each strengthened the LH association of the other, but overall the external environment was more predictive of LH than was the internal state. These findings suggest that individuals rely on a multitude and consistency of sensory information in more decisively calibrating LH and behavioural strategies.


Asunto(s)
Factores de Edad , Conducta Animal , Ambiente , Rasgos de la Historia de Vida , Animales , Femenino , Masculino
12.
Dev Psychopathol ; 30(5): 1937-1958, 2018 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30132425

RESUMEN

Using multilevel models, we examined mother-, father-, and child-reported (N = 1,336 families) externalizing behavior problem trajectories from age 7 to 14 in nine countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States). The intercept and slope of children's externalizing behavior trajectories varied both across individuals within culture and across cultures, and the variance was larger at the individual level than at the culture level. Mothers' and children's endorsement of aggression as well as mothers' authoritarian attitudes predicted higher age 8 intercepts of child externalizing behaviors. Furthermore, prediction from individual-level endorsement of aggression and authoritarian attitudes to more child externalizing behaviors was augmented by prediction from cultural-level endorsement of aggression and authoritarian attitudes, respectively. Cultures in which father-reported endorsement of aggression was higher and both mother- and father-reported authoritarian attitudes were higher also reported more child externalizing behavior problems at age 8. Among fathers, greater attributions regarding uncontrollable success in caregiving situations were associated with steeper declines in externalizing over time. Understanding cultural-level as well as individual-level correlates of children's externalizing behavior offers potential insights into prevention and intervention efforts that can be more effectively targeted at individual children and parents as well as targeted at changing cultural norms that increase the risk of children's and adolescents' externalizing behavior.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/etnología , Agresión , Conducta Infantil/etnología , Comparación Transcultural , Padre , Madres , Responsabilidad Parental/etnología , Problema de Conducta , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , China/etnología , Colombia/etnología , Femenino , Humanos , Italia/etnología , Jordania/etnología , Kenia/etnología , Masculino , Filipinas/etnología , Suecia/etnología , Tailandia/etnología , Estados Unidos/etnología
13.
J Res Adolesc ; 28(3): 571-590, 2018 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30515947

RESUMEN

This study used data from 12 cultural groups in nine countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States; N = 1,298) to understand the cross-cultural generalizability of how parental warmth and control are bidirectionally related to externalizing and internalizing behaviors from childhood to early adolescence. Mothers, fathers, and children completed measures when children were ages 8-13. Multiple-group autoregressive, cross-lagged structural equation models revealed that child effects rather than parent effects may better characterize how warmth and control are related to child externalizing and internalizing behaviors over time, and that parent effects may be more characteristic of relations between parental warmth and control and child externalizing and internalizing behavior during childhood than early adolescence.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Problema de Conducta/psicología , Adolescente , Niño , China , Colombia , Comparación Transcultural , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Jordania , Kenia , Masculino , Filipinas , Suecia , Tailandia , Estados Unidos
14.
J Adolesc ; 64: 124-135, 2018 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29454294

RESUMEN

The present study examines whether early adolescents' self-efficacy beliefs about anger regulation mediate the relation between parents' self-efficacy beliefs about anger regulation and early adolescents' internalizing and externalizing problems. Participants were 534 early adolescents (T1: M age = 10.89, SD = .70; 50% female), their mothers (n = 534), and their fathers (n = 431). Families were drawn from Colombia, Italy, and the USA. Follow-up data were obtained two (T2) and three (T3) years later. At T1 and T3, parents' self-efficacy beliefs were self-reported and internalizing and externalizing problems were assessed via mothers', fathers', and early adolescents' reports. At T2, early adolescents' self-efficacy beliefs were self-reported Within the overall sample, mothers with higher self-efficacy beliefs about anger regulation had children with similar beliefs. Early adolescents' low self-efficacy beliefs were associated with higher internalizing and externalizing problems.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Ira , Mecanismos de Defensa , Autoeficacia , Autocontrol/psicología , Adolescente , Niño , Colombia , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Padres/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Estados Unidos
15.
J Adolesc ; 62: 207-217, 2018 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28662856

RESUMEN

This study grapples with what it means to be part of a cultural group, from a statistical modeling perspective. The method we present compares within- and between-cultural group variability, in behaviors in families. We demonstrate the method using a cross-cultural study of adolescent development and parenting, involving three biennial waves of longitudinal data from 1296 eight-year-olds and their parents (multiple cultures in nine countries). Family members completed surveys about parental negativity and positivity, child academic and social-emotional adjustment, and attitudes about parenting and adolescent behavior. Variance estimates were computed at the cultural group, person, and within-person level using multilevel models. Of the longitudinally consistent variance, most was within and not between cultural groups-although there was a wide range of between-group differences. This approach to quantifying cultural group variability may prove valuable when applied to quantitative studies of acculturation.


Asunto(s)
Aculturación , Conducta del Adolescente/fisiología , Comparación Transcultural , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Adolescente , Niño , Cultura , Familia/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Padres/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
16.
J Youth Adolesc ; 47(5): 1052-1072, 2018 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29047004

RESUMEN

Epidemiological data indicate that risk behaviors are among the leading causes of adolescent morbidity and mortality worldwide. Consistent with this, laboratory-based studies of age differences in risk behavior allude to a peak in adolescence, suggesting that adolescents demonstrate a heightened propensity, or inherent inclination, to take risks. Unlike epidemiological reports, studies of risk taking propensity have been limited to Western samples, leaving questions about the extent to which heightened risk taking propensity is an inherent or culturally constructed aspect of adolescence. In the present study, age patterns in risk-taking propensity (using two laboratory tasks: the Stoplight and the BART) and real-world risk taking (using self-reports of health and antisocial risk taking) were examined in a sample of 5227 individuals (50.7% female) ages 10-30 (M = 17.05 years, SD = 5.91) from 11 Western and non-Western countries (China, Colombia, Cyprus, India, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the US). Two hypotheses were tested: (1) risk taking follows an inverted-U pattern across age groups, peaking earlier on measures of risk taking propensity than on measures of real-world risk taking, and (2) age patterns in risk taking propensity are more consistent across countries than age patterns in real-world risk taking. Overall, risk taking followed the hypothesized inverted-U pattern across age groups, with health risk taking evincing the latest peak. Age patterns in risk taking propensity were more consistent across countries than age patterns in real-world risk taking. Results suggest that although the association between age and risk taking is sensitive to measurement and culture, around the world, risk taking is generally highest among late adolescents.


Asunto(s)
Conducta del Adolescente/psicología , Comparación Transcultural , Asunción de Riesgos , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Salud Global , Humanos , Masculino , Autoinforme , Adulto Joven
17.
Dev Psychopathol ; 29(5): 1675-1688, 2017 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29162175

RESUMEN

Using data from 1,177 families in eight countries (Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States), we tested a conceptual model of direct effects of childhood family adversity on subsequent externalizing behaviors as well as indirect effects through psychological mediators. When children were 9 years old, mothers and fathers reported on financial difficulties and their use of corporal punishment, and children reported perceptions of their parents' rejection. When children were 10 years old, they completed a computerized battery of tasks assessing reward sensitivity and impulse control and responded to questions about hypothetical social provocations to assess their hostile attributions and proclivity for aggressive responding. When children were 12 years old, they reported on their externalizing behavior. Multigroup structural equation models revealed that across all eight countries, childhood family adversity had direct effects on externalizing behaviors 3 years later, and childhood family adversity had indirect effects on externalizing behavior through psychological mediators. The findings suggest ways in which family-level adversity poses risk for children's subsequent development of problems at psychological and behavioral levels, situated within diverse cultural contexts.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Familiares/psicología , Modelos Psicológicos , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Pobreza/psicología , Castigo/psicología , Recompensa , Adolescente , Adulto , Agresión/psicología , Niño , Colombia , Comparación Transcultural , Femenino , Humanos , Italia , Jordania , Kenia , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Filipinas , Conducta Social , Percepción Social , Factores Socioeconómicos , Suecia , Tailandia , Estados Unidos
18.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 57(7): 824-34, 2016 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26511201

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Research supports the beneficial role of prosocial behaviors on children's adjustment and successful youth development. Empirical studies point to reciprocal relations between negative parenting and children's maladjustment, but reciprocal relations between positive parenting and children's prosocial behavior are understudied. In this study reciprocal relations between two different dimensions of positive parenting (quality of the mother-child relationship and the use of balanced positive discipline) and children's prosocial behavior were examined in Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States. METHODS: Mother-child dyads (N = 1105) provided data over 2 years in two waves (Mage of child in wave 1 = 9.31 years, SD = 0.73; 50% female). RESULTS: A model of reciprocal relations between parenting dimensions, but not among parenting and children's prosocial behavior, emerged. In particular, children with higher levels of prosocial behavior at age 9 elicited higher levels of mother-child relationship quality in the following year. CONCLUSIONS: Findings yielded similar relations across countries, evidencing that being prosocial in late childhood contributes to some degree to the enhancement of a nurturing and involved mother-child relationship in countries that vary widely on sociodemographic profiles and psychological characteristics. Policy and intervention implications of this study are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/etnología , Relaciones Madre-Hijo/etnología , Responsabilidad Parental/etnología , Conducta Social , Niño , Colombia/etnología , Femenino , Humanos , Italia/etnología , Jordania/etnología , Kenia/etnología , Masculino , Filipinas/etnología , Suecia/etnología , Tailandia/etnología , Estados Unidos/etnología
19.
Int J Psychol ; 51(5): 366-74, 2016 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26104262

RESUMEN

Children's family obligations involve assistance and respect that children are expected to provide to immediate and extended family members and reflect beliefs related to family life that may differ across cultural groups. Mothers, fathers and children (N = 1432 families) in 13 cultural groups in 9 countries (China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, Philippines, Sweden, Thailand and United States) reported on their expectations regarding children's family obligations and parenting attitudes and behaviours. Within families, mothers and fathers had more concordant expectations regarding children's family obligations than did parents and children. Parenting behaviours that were warmer, less neglectful and more controlling as well as parenting attitudes that were more authoritarian were related to higher expectations regarding children's family obligations between families within cultures as well as between cultures. These international findings advance understanding of children's family obligations by contextualising them both within families and across a number of diverse cultural groups in 9 countries.


Asunto(s)
Comparación Transcultural , Padre , Madres , Relaciones Padres-Hijo/etnología , Percepción , Responsabilidad Social , Adulto , Niño , China/etnología , Relaciones Familiares/etnología , Relaciones Familiares/psicología , Padre/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Italia/etnología , Jordania/etnología , Kenia/etnología , Masculino , Madres/psicología , Responsabilidad Parental/etnología , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Filipinas/etnología , Suecia/etnología , Tailandia/etnología , Estados Unidos/etnología
20.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 56(8): 923-32, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25492267

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: It is generally believed that parental rejection of children leads to child maladaptation. However, the specific effects of perceived parental acceptance-rejection on diverse domains of child adjustment and development have been incompletely documented, and whether these effects hold across diverse populations and for mothers and fathers are still open questions. METHODS: This study assessed children's perceptions of mother and father acceptance-rejection in 1,247 families from China, Colombia, Italy, Jordan, Kenya, the Philippines, Sweden, Thailand, and the United States as antecedent predictors of later internalizing and externalizing behavior problems, school performance, prosocial behavior, and social competence. RESULTS: Higher perceived parental rejection predicted increases in internalizing and externalizing behavior problems and decreases in school performance and prosocial behavior across 3 years controlling for within-wave relations, stability across waves, and parental age, education, and social desirability bias. Patterns of relations were similar across mothers and fathers and, with a few exceptions, all nine countries. CONCLUSIONS: Children's perceptions of maternal and paternal acceptance-rejection have small but nearly universal effects on multiple aspects of their adjustment and development regardless of the family's country of origin.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/epidemiología , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Padres/psicología , Distancia Psicológica , Rechazo en Psicología , Ajuste Social , Adaptación Psicológica , Adulto , Niño , Trastornos de la Conducta Infantil/psicología , China/epidemiología , Colombia/epidemiología , Comparación Transcultural , Ajuste Emocional , Padre/psicología , Padre/estadística & datos numéricos , Femenino , Humanos , Italia/epidemiología , Jordania/epidemiología , Kenia/epidemiología , Masculino , Madres/psicología , Madres/estadística & datos numéricos , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Filipinas/epidemiología , Suecia/epidemiología , Tailandia/epidemiología , Estados Unidos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA