Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 127
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
N Engl J Med ; 391(7): 598-608, 2024 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39141852

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Patients with brain injury who are unresponsive to commands may perform cognitive tasks that are detected on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG). This phenomenon, known as cognitive motor dissociation, has not been systematically studied in a large cohort of persons with disorders of consciousness. METHODS: In this prospective cohort study conducted at six international centers, we collected clinical, behavioral, and task-based fMRI and EEG data from a convenience sample of 353 adults with disorders of consciousness. We assessed the response to commands on task-based fMRI or EEG in participants without an observable response to verbal commands (i.e., those with a behavioral diagnosis of coma, vegetative state, or minimally conscious state-minus) and in participants with an observable response to verbal commands. The presence or absence of an observable response to commands was assessed with the use of the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R). RESULTS: Data from fMRI only or EEG only were available for 65% of the participants, and data from both fMRI and EEG were available for 35%. The median age of the participants was 37.9 years, the median time between brain injury and assessment with the CRS-R was 7.9 months (25% of the participants were assessed with the CRS-R within 28 days after injury), and brain trauma was an etiologic factor in 50%. We detected cognitive motor dissociation in 60 of the 241 participants (25%) without an observable response to commands, of whom 11 had been assessed with the use of fMRI only, 13 with the use of EEG only, and 36 with the use of both techniques. Cognitive motor dissociation was associated with younger age, longer time since injury, and brain trauma as an etiologic factor. In contrast, responses on task-based fMRI or EEG occurred in 43 of 112 participants (38%) with an observable response to verbal commands. CONCLUSIONS: Approximately one in four participants without an observable response to commands performed a cognitive task on fMRI or EEG as compared with one in three participants with an observable response to commands. (Funded by the James S. McDonnell Foundation and others.).


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas , Trastornos de la Conciencia , Trastornos Disociativos , Estado Vegetativo Persistente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Lesiones Encefálicas/fisiopatología , Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Lesiones Encefálicas/diagnóstico por imagen , Cognición/fisiología , Trastornos de la Conciencia/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos de la Conciencia/etiología , Trastornos de la Conciencia/fisiopatología , Electroencefalografía , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/diagnóstico por imagen , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/etiología , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/fisiopatología , Estudios Prospectivos , Trastornos Disociativos/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos Disociativos/etiología , Trastornos Disociativos/fisiopatología
2.
J Neurosci ; 44(4)2024 01 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38267235

RESUMEN

Low-level features are typically continuous (e.g., the gamut between two colors), but semantic information is often categorical (there is no corresponding gradient between dog and turtle) and hierarchical (animals live in land, water, or air). To determine the impact of these differences on cognitive representations, we characterized the geometry of perceptual spaces of five domains: a domain dominated by semantic information (animal names presented as words), a domain dominated by low-level features (colored textures), and three intermediate domains (animal images, lightly texturized animal images that were easy to recognize, and heavily texturized animal images that were difficult to recognize). Each domain had 37 stimuli derived from the same animal names. From 13 participants (9F), we gathered similarity judgments in each domain via an efficient psychophysical ranking paradigm. We then built geometric models of each domain for each participant, in which distances between stimuli accounted for participants' similarity judgments and intrinsic uncertainty. Remarkably, the five domains had similar global properties: each required 5-7 dimensions, and a modest amount of spherical curvature provided the best fit. However, the arrangement of the stimuli within these embeddings depended on the level of semantic information: dendrograms derived from semantic domains (word, image, and lightly texturized images) were more "tree-like" than those from feature-dominated domains (heavily texturized images and textures). Thus, the perceptual spaces of domains along this feature-dominated to semantic-dominated gradient shift to a tree-like organization when semantic information dominates, while retaining a similar global geometry.


Asunto(s)
Juicio , Tortugas , Humanos , Animales , Perros , Semántica , Incertidumbre , Agua
3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 40(2): 237-258, 2023 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36821194

RESUMEN

Analysis of visual texture is important for many key steps in early vision. We study visual sensitivity to image statistics in three families of textures that include multiple gray levels and correlations in two spatial dimensions. Sensitivities to positive and negative correlations are approximately independent of correlation sign, and signals from different kinds of correlations combine quadratically. We build a computational model, fully constrained by prior studies of sensitivity to uncorrelated textures and black-and-white textures with spatial correlations. The model accounts for many features of the new data, including sign-independence, quadratic combination, and the dependence on gray-level distribution.

4.
J Vis ; 23(11): 42, 2023 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37733536

RESUMEN

Studies of emmetropization have traditionally focused on the spatial characteristics of visual input signals. Yet the input to the retina is not a two-dimensional pattern but a temporally-varying luminance flow. The temporal structure of this flow is predominately determined by eye movements, as the human eyes move incessantly. Even when fixating on a single point, a persistent motion known as ocular drift reformats the luminance flow in a way that counterbalances the spectra of natural scenes. It is established that emmetropes are highly sensitive to these luminance modulations. However, their visual consequences in myopia and hyperopia are unknown. Here, we first review how the temporal-frequency distribution of retinal input signals varies with the amount of ocular drift. We then use a detailed optical/geometrical model of the eye to study how the eye movements jointly shape retinal input as a function of refraction. We show that, within the temporal range of sensitivity of the retina, the spatial frequency distribution of the input signals conveys signed information about defocus. Specifically, for a given degree of defocus, myopic retinas experience more power from low spatial frequency stimuli than hyperopic retinas. These redistribution of input power may have a consequence during eye growth supporting the proposal that eye movements should be taken into consideration in the process of emmetropization.


Asunto(s)
Hiperopía , Miopía , Humanos , Movimientos Oculares , Retina , Cara
5.
J Neurosci ; 41(3): 489-501, 2021 01 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33234608

RESUMEN

Recent work has shown that most cells in the rostral, gustatory portion of the nucleus tractus solitarius (rNTS) in awake, freely licking rats show lick-related firing. However, the relationship between taste-related and lick-related activity in rNTS remains unclear. Here, we tested whether GABA-derived inhibitory activity regulates the balance of lick- and taste-driven neuronal activity. Combinatorial viral tools were used to restrict the expression of channelrhodopsin 2-enhanced yellow fluorescent protein to GAD1+ GABAergic neurons. Viral infusions were bilateral in rNTS. A fiber-optic fiber attached to a bundle of drivable microwires was later implanted into the rNTS. After recovery, water-deprived rats were presented with taste stimuli in an experimental chamber. Trials were five consecutive taste licks [NaCl, KCl, NH4Cl, sucrose, monosodium glutamate/inosine-5'-monophosphate, citric acid, quinine, or artificial saliva (AS)] separated by five AS rinse licks on a variable ratio 5 schedule. Each taste lick triggered a 1 s train of laser light (25 Hz; 473 nm; 8-10 mW) in a random half of the trials. In all, 113 cells were recorded in the rNTS, 50 cells responded to one or more taste stimuli without GABA enhancement. Selective changes in response magnitude (spike count) within cells shifted across-unit patterns but preserved interstimulus relationships. Cells where enhanced GABAergic tone increased lick coherence conveyed more information distinguishing basic taste qualities and different salts than other cells. In addition, GABA activation significantly amplified the amount of information that discriminated palatable versus unpalatable tastants. By dynamically regulating lick coherence and remodeling the across-unit response patterns to taste, enhancing GABAergic tone in rNTS reconfigures the neural activity reflecting sensation and movement.


Asunto(s)
Actividad Motora/fisiología , Sensación/fisiología , Núcleo Solitario/fisiología , Ácido gamma-Aminobutírico/fisiología , Animales , Electrodos Implantados , Fenómenos Electrofisiológicos , Femenino , Colorantes Fluorescentes , Glutamato Descarboxilasa/fisiología , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Gusto/fisiología , Percepción del Gusto/fisiología
6.
J Neurosci ; 39(19): 3713-3727, 2019 05 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30846614

RESUMEN

The demands on a sensory system depend not only on the statistics of its inputs but also on the task. In olfactory navigation, for example, the task is to find the plume source; allocation of sensory resources may therefore be driven by aspects of the plume that are informative about source location, rather than concentration per se. Here we explore the implications of this idea for encoding odor concentration. To formalize the notion that sensory resources are limited, we considered coding strategies that partitioned the odor concentration range into a set of discriminable intervals. We developed a dynamic programming algorithm that, given the distribution of odor concentrations at several locations, determines the partitioning that conveys the most information about location. We applied this analysis to planar laser-induced fluorescence measurements of spatiotemporal odor fields with realistic advection speeds (5-20 cm/s), with or without a nearby boundary or obstacle. Across all environments, the optimal coding strategy allocated more resources (i.e., more and finer discriminable intervals) to the upper end of the concentration range than would be expected from histogram equalization, the optimal strategy if the goal were to reconstruct the plume, rather than to navigate. Finally, we show that ligand binding, as captured by the Hill equation, transforms odorant concentration into response levels in a way that approximates information maximization for navigation. This behavior occurs when the Hill dissociation constant is near the mean odor concentration, an adaptive set-point that has been observed in the olfactory system of flies.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The first step of olfactory processing is receptor binding, and the resulting relationship between odorant concentration and the bound receptor fraction is a saturating one. While this Hill nonlinearity can be viewed as a distortion that is imposed by the biophysics of receptor binding, here we show that it also plays an important information-processing role in olfactory navigation. Specifically, by combining a novel dynamic-programming algorithm with physical measurements of turbulent plumes, we determine the optimal strategy for encoding odor concentration when the goal is to determine location. This strategy is distinct from histogram equalization, the strategy that maximizes information about plume concentration, and is closely approximated by the Hill nonlinearity when the binding constant is near the ambient mean.


Asunto(s)
Algoritmos , Dinámicas no Lineales , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Acetona/administración & dosificación , Animales , Olfato/efectos de los fármacos , Navegación Espacial/efectos de los fármacos
7.
Neuroimage ; 221: 117173, 2020 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32682991

RESUMEN

Functional neuroimaging experiments that employ naturalistic stimuli (natural scenes, films, spoken narratives) provide insights into cognitive function "in the wild". Natural stimuli typically possess crowded, spectrally dense, dynamic, and multimodal properties within a rich multiscale structure. However, when using natural stimuli, various challenges exist for creating parametric manipulations with tight experimental control. Here, we revisit the typical spectral composition and statistical dependences of natural scenes, which distinguish them from abstract stimuli. We then demonstrate how to selectively degrade subtle statistical dependences within specific spatial scales using the wavelet transform. Such manipulations leave basic features of the stimuli, such as luminance and contrast, intact. Using functional neuroimaging of human participants viewing degraded natural images, we demonstrate that cortical responses at different levels of the visual hierarchy are differentially sensitive to subtle statistical dependences in natural images. This demonstration supports the notion that perceptual systems in the brain are optimally tuned to the complex statistical properties of the natural world. The code to undertake these stimulus manipulations, and their natural extension to dynamic natural scenes (films), is freely available.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
8.
Brain ; 142(7): 1887-1893, 2019 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31505542

RESUMEN

Dopaminergic stimulation has been proposed as a treatment strategy for post-traumatic brain injured patients in minimally conscious state based on a clinical trial using amantadine, a weak dopamine transporter blocker. However, a specific contribution of dopaminergic neuromodulation in minimally conscious state is undemonstrated. In a phase 0 clinical trial, we evaluated 13 normal volunteers and seven post-traumatic minimally conscious state patients using 11C-raclopride PET to estimate dopamine 2-like receptors occupancy in the striatum and central thalamus before and after dopamine transporter blockade with dextroamphetamine. If a presynaptic deficit was observed, a third and a fourth 11C-raclopride PET were acquired to evaluate changes in dopamine release induced by l-DOPA and l-DOPA+dextroamphetamine. Permutation analysis showed a significant reduction of dopamine release in patients, demonstrating a presynaptic deficit in the striatum and central thalamus that could not be reversed by blocking the dopamine transporter. However, administration of the dopamine precursor l-DOPA reversed the presynaptic deficit by restoring the biosynthesis of dopamine from both ventral tegmentum and substantia nigra. The advantages of alternative pharmacodynamic approaches in post-traumatic minimally conscious state patients should be tested in clinical trials, as patients currently refractory to amantadine might benefit from them.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/metabolismo , Dopamina/deficiencia , Dopamina/metabolismo , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/metabolismo , Terminales Presinápticos/metabolismo , Adulto , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/complicaciones , Cuerpo Estriado/metabolismo , Dextroanfetamina/farmacología , Proteínas de Transporte de Dopamina a través de la Membrana Plasmática/antagonistas & inhibidores , Femenino , Humanos , Levodopa/farmacología , Masculino , Estado Vegetativo Persistente/complicaciones , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Terminales Presinápticos/efectos de los fármacos , Racloprida/metabolismo , Receptores de Dopamina D2/metabolismo , Sustancia Negra/metabolismo , Tegmento Mesencefálico/metabolismo , Tálamo/metabolismo , Adulto Joven
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 121(2): 634-645, 2019 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30565959

RESUMEN

Theories of neural coding in the taste system typically rely exclusively on data gleaned from taste-responsive cells. However, even in the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), the first stage of central processing, neurons with taste selectivity coexist with neurons whose activity is linked to motor behavior related to ingestion. We recorded from a large ( n = 324) sample of NTS neurons recorded in awake rats, examining both their taste selectivity and the association of their activity with licking. All subjects were implanted with a bundle of microelectrodes aimed at the NTS and allowed to recover. Following moderate water deprivation, rats were placed in an experimental chamber where tastants or artificial saliva (AS) were delivered from a lick spout. Electrophysiological responses were recorded, and waveforms from single cells were isolated offline. Results showed that only a minority of NTS cells responded to taste stimuli as determined by conventional firing-rate measures. In contrast, most cells, including taste-responsive cells, tracked the lick pattern, as evidenced by significant lick coherence in the 5- to 7-Hz range. Several quantitative measures of taste selectivity and lick relatedness showed that the population formed a continuum, ranging from cells dominated by taste responses to those dominated by lick relatedness. Moreover, even neurons whose responses were highly correlated with lick activity could convey substantial information about taste quality. In all, data point to a blurred boundary between taste-dominated and lick-related cells in NTS, suggesting that information from the taste of food and from the movements it evokes are seamlessly integrated. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Neurons in the rostral nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) are known to encode information about taste. However, recordings from awake rats reveal that only a minority of NTS cells respond exclusively to taste stimuli. The majority of neurons track behaviors associated with food consumption, and even strongly lick-related neurons could convey information about taste quality. These findings suggest that the NTS integrates information from both taste and behavior to identify food.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas/fisiología , Núcleo Solitario/fisiología , Percepción del Gusto , Animales , Masculino , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Núcleo Solitario/citología , Gusto
10.
Chem Senses ; 44(4): 237-247, 2019 04 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30788507

RESUMEN

The gustatory system encodes information about chemical identity, nutritional value, and concentration of sensory stimuli before transmitting the signal from taste buds to central neurons that process and transform the signal. Deciphering the coding logic for taste quality requires examining responses at each level along the neural axis-from peripheral sensory organs to gustatory cortex. From the earliest single-fiber recordings, it was clear that some afferent neurons respond uniquely and others to stimuli of multiple qualities. There is frequently a "best stimulus" for a given neuron, leading to the suggestion that taste exhibits "labeled line coding." In the extreme, a strict "labeled line" requires neurons and pathways dedicated to single qualities (e.g., sweet, bitter, etc.). At the other end of the spectrum, "across-fiber," "combinatorial," or "ensemble" coding requires minimal specific information to be imparted by a single neuron. Instead, taste quality information is encoded by simultaneous activity in ensembles of afferent fibers. Further, "temporal coding" models have proposed that certain features of taste quality may be embedded in the cadence of impulse activity. Taste receptor proteins are often expressed in nonoverlapping sets of cells in taste buds apparently supporting "labeled lines." Yet, taste buds include both narrowly and broadly tuned cells. As gustatory signals proceed to the hindbrain and on to higher centers, coding becomes more distributed and temporal patterns of activity become important. Here, we present the conundrum of taste coding in the light of current electrophysiological and imaging techniques at several levels of the gustatory processing pathway.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Papilas Gustativas/fisiología , Gusto/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Estimulación Química
11.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 14(7): e1006275, 2018 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29990365

RESUMEN

Many species rely on olfaction to navigate towards food sources or mates. Olfactory navigation is a challenging task since odor environments are typically turbulent. While time-averaged odor concentration varies smoothly with the distance to the source, instaneous concentrations are intermittent and obtaining stable averages takes longer than the typical intervals between animals' navigation decisions. How to effectively sample from the odor distribution to determine sampling location is the focus in this article. To investigate which sampling strategies are most informative about the location of an odor source, we recorded three naturalistic stimuli with planar lased-induced fluorescence and used an information-theoretic approach to quantify the information that different sampling strategies provide about sampling location. Specifically, we compared multiple sampling strategies based on a fixed number of coding bits for encoding the olfactory stimulus. When the coding bits were all allocated to representing odor concentration at a single sensor, information rapidly saturated. Using the same number of coding bits in two sensors provides more information, as does coding multiple samples at different times. When accumulating multiple samples at a fixed location, the temporal sequence does not yield a large amount of information and can be averaged with minimal loss. Furthermore, we show that histogram-equalization is not the most efficient way to use coding bits when using the olfactory sample to determine location.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Teoría de la Información , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Navegación Espacial/fisiología , Algoritmos , Animales , Fluorescencia , Neuronas Receptoras Olfatorias/fisiología
12.
J Vis ; 18(7): 10, 2018 07 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30029274

RESUMEN

During development, the eye tunes its size to its optics so that distant objects are in focus, a state known as emmetropia. Although multiple factors contribute to this process, a strong influence appears to be exerted by the visual input signals entering the eye. Much research has been dedicated to the possible roles of specific features of the retinal image, such as the magnitude of blur. However, in humans and other species, the input to the retina is not an image, but a spatiotemporal flow of luminance. Small eye movements occur incessantly during natural fixation, continually transforming the spatial scene into temporal modulations on the retina. An emerging body of evidence suggests that this space-time reformatting is crucial to many aspects of visual processing, including sensitivity to fine spatial detail. The resulting temporal modulations depend not only on ocular dynamics, but also on the optics and shape of the eye, and the spatial statistics of the visual scene. Here we examine the characteristics of these signals and suggest that they may play a role in emmetropization. A direct consequence of this viewpoint is that abnormal oculomotor behavior may contribute to the development of myopia and hyperopia.


Asunto(s)
Emetropía/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Ojo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Humanos , Hiperopía/fisiopatología , Miopía/fisiopatología , Retina/fisiopatología , Visión Ocular/fisiología
15.
J Neurosci ; 35(16): 6284-97, 2015 Apr 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25904782

RESUMEN

Flavor is produced by the integration of taste, olfaction, texture, and temperature, currently thought to occur in the cortex. However, previous work has shown that brainstem taste-related nuclei also respond to multisensory inputs. Here, we test the hypothesis that taste and olfaction interact in the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS; the first neural relay in the central gustatory pathway) in awake, freely licking rats. Electrophysiological recordings of taste and taste + odor responses were conducted in an experimental chamber following surgical electrode implantation and recovery. Tastants (0.1 m NaCl, 0.1 m sucrose, 0.01 m citric acid, and 0.0001 m quinine) were delivered for five consecutive licks interspersed with five licks of artificial saliva rinse delivered on a VR5 schedule. Odorants were n-amyl acetate (banana), acetic acid (vinegar), octanoic acid (rancid), and phenylethyl alcohol (floral). For each cell, metric space analyses were used to quantify the information conveyed by spike count, by the rate envelope, and by individual spike timing. Results revealed diverse effects of odorants on taste-response magnitude and latency across cells. Importantly, NTS cells were more competent at discriminating taste + odor stimuli versus tastants presented alone for all taste qualities using both rate and temporal coding. The strong interaction of odorants and tastants at the NTS underscores its role as the initial node in the neural circuit that controls food identification and ingestion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Núcleo Solitario/fisiología , Percepción del Gusto/fisiología , Vigilia , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Masculino , Neuronas/fisiología , Ratas , Núcleo Solitario/citología
16.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(1): 171-82, 2016 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27121585

RESUMEN

Several studies have shown that taste-responsive cells in the brainstem taste nuclei of rodents respond to sensory qualities other than gustation. Such data suggest that cells in the classical gustatory brainstem may be better tuned to respond to stimuli that engage multiple sensory modalities than to stimuli that are purely gustatory. Here, we test this idea by recording the electrophysiological responses to complex, naturalistic stimuli in single neurons in the parabrachial pons (PbN, the second neural relay in the central gustatory pathway) in awake, freely licking rats. Following electrode implantation and recovery, we presented both prototypical and naturalistic taste stimuli and recorded the responses in the PbN. Prototypical taste stimuli (NaCl, sucrose, citric acid, and caffeine) and naturalistic stimuli (clam juice, grape juice, lemon juice, and coffee) were matched for taste quality and intensity (concentration). Umami (monosodium glutamate + inosine monophosphate) and fat (diluted heavy cream) were also tested. PbN neurons responded to naturalistic stimuli as much or more than to prototypical taste stimuli. Furthermore, they convey more information about naturalistic stimuli than about prototypical ones. Moreover, multidimensional scaling analyses showed that across unit responses to naturalistic stimuli were more widely separated than responses to prototypical taste stimuli. Interestingly, cream evoked a robust and widespread response in PbN cells. Collectively, these data suggest that natural foods are more potent stimulators of PbN cells than purely gustatory stimuli. Probing PbN cells with pure taste stimuli may underestimate the response repertoire of these cells.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas/fisiología , Puente/fisiología , Percepción del Gusto/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción , Animales , Electrodos Implantados , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Masculino , Estimulación Física , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Gusto , Vigilia/fisiología
17.
J Comput Neurosci ; 49(2): 69, 2021 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33646475
18.
Nature ; 466(7306): 617-21, 2010 Jul 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20601940

RESUMEN

Connectivity in the cortex is organized at multiple scales, suggesting that scale-dependent correlated activity is particularly important for understanding the behaviour of sensory cortices and their function in stimulus encoding. We analysed the scale-dependent structure of cortical interactions by using maximum entropy models to characterize multiple-tetrode recordings from primary visual cortex of anaesthetized macaque monkeys (Macaca mulatta). We compared the properties of firing patterns among local clusters of neurons (<300 microm apart) with those of neurons separated by larger distances (600-2,500 microm). Here we report that local firing patterns are distinctive: whereas multi-neuronal firing patterns at larger distances can be predicted by pairwise interactions, patterns within local clusters often show evidence of high-order correlations. Surprisingly, these local correlations are flexible and rapidly reorganized by visual input. Although they modestly reduce the amount of information that a cluster conveys, they also modify the format of this information, creating sparser codes by increasing the periods of total quiescence, and concentrating information into briefer periods of common activity. These results imply a hierarchical organization of neuronal correlations: simple pairwise correlations link neurons over scales of tens to hundreds of minicolumns, but on the scale of a few minicolumns, ensembles of neurons form complex subnetworks whose moment-to-moment effective connectivity is dynamically reorganized by the stimulus.


Asunto(s)
Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/citología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Visual/citología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Entropía , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología
19.
J Vis ; 16(14): 22, 2016 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27902829

RESUMEN

Motion signals are a rich source of information used in many everyday tasks, such as segregation of objects from background and navigation. Motion analysis by biological systems is generally considered to consist of two stages: extraction of local motion signals followed by spatial integration. Studies using synthetic stimuli show that there are many kinds and subtypes of local motion signals. When presented in isolation, these stimuli elicit behavioral and neurophysiological responses in a wide range of species, from insects to mammals. However, these mathematically-distinct varieties of local motion signals typically co-exist in natural scenes. This study focuses on interactions between two kinds of local motion signals: Fourier and glider. Fourier signals are typically associated with translation, while glider signals occur when an object approaches or recedes. Here, using a novel class of synthetic stimuli, we ask how distinct kinds of local motion signals interact and whether context influences sensitivity to Fourier motion. We report that local motion signals of different types interact at the perceptual level, and that this interaction can include subthreshold summation and, in some subjects, subtle context-dependent changes in sensitivity. We discuss the implications of these observations, and the factors that may underlie them.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven
20.
J Neurosci ; 34(10): 3559-78, 2014 Mar 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24599456

RESUMEN

Segmenting the visual image into objects is a crucial stage of visual processing. Object boundaries are typically associated with differences in luminance, but discontinuities in texture also play an important role. We showed previously that a subpopulation of neurons in V2 in anesthetized macaques responds to orientation discontinuities parallel to their receptive field orientation. Such single-cell responses could be a neurophysiological correlate of texture boundary detection. Neurons in V1, on the other hand, are known to have contextual response modulations such as iso-orientation surround suppression, which also produce responses to orientation discontinuities. Here, we use pseudorandom multiregion grating stimuli of two frame durations (20 and 40 ms) to probe and compare texture boundary responses in V1 and V2 in anesthetized macaque monkeys. In V1, responses to texture boundaries were observed for only the 40 ms frame duration and were independent of the orientation of the texture boundary. However, in transient V2 neurons, responses to such texture boundaries were robust for both frame durations and were stronger for boundaries parallel to the neuron's preferred orientation. The dependence of these processes on stimulus duration and orientation indicates that responses to texture boundaries in V2 arise independently of contextual modulations in V1. In addition, because the responses in transient V2 neurons are sensitive to the orientation of the texture boundary but those of V1 neurons are not, we suggest that V2 responses are the correlate of texture boundary detection, whereas contextual modulation in V1 serves other purposes, possibly related to orientation "pop-out."


Asunto(s)
Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Corteza Visual/citología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Macaca fascicularis , Masculino , Campos Visuales/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA