Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 90
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
N Z Vet J ; 68(6): 324-330, 2020 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32495724

RESUMEN

Aims: To evaluate the efficacy of insulating the limbs and thorax of cats with a combination of bubble wrap and an absorbent, plastic-lined pad in reducing heat loss during ovariohysterectomy. Methods: A preliminary study was performed to compare heat loss of 1 L bags of Hartmann's solution heated to 38°C which were either wrapped in two layers of bubble wrap and an absorbent pad (n = 6) or were unwrapped (n = 6). Bags were allowed to cool in a temperature-controlled room and the temperature of the bags was measured every 10 minutes for 60 minutes. The clinical study, included 16 intact female cats undergoing ovariohysterectomy. The cats were premedicated with I/M morphine and either medetomidine or dexmedetomidine, and anaesthesia was induced with I/V propofol and maintained with isoflurane in 100% oxygen. Cats were randomly assigned to either the treatment group (n = 8) whose limbs and thorax wrapped with two layers of bubble wrap and an absorbent pad immediately after induction, or the control group (n = 8) which were unwrapped. Body temperature (measured with an oesophageal temperature probe), heart rate, respiratory rate, mean arterial pressure and partial pressure of end-tidal CO2 were recorded immediately after induction (T start), before surgery started (T surgery), and at the end of isoflurane administration (T end). The times from T end to extubation, from T end to when the cat could maintain sternal recumbency and from T end to when the cat was able to stand, were also recorded. Results: In the preliminary study of heat loss by fluid bags, the mean temperature at 60 minutes was higher in wrapped bags (35.4 (SD 0.2)°C) compared to unwrapped bags (33.0 (SD 0.3)°C; p < 0.01). For cats undergoing ovariohysterectomy, mean body temperature of wrapped cats was higher than that of unwrapped cats both at T surgery (36.0 (SE 0.3) vs. 34.5 (SE 0.3)°C; p = 0.001) and at T end (37.2 (SE 0.5) vs. 36.0 (SE 0.5)°C; p = 0.01). Wrapped cats regained the ability to stand more rapidly that unwrapped cats (26.4 (SE 5.8) vs. 47.0 (SE 5.8) minutes p = 0.01). Conclusions: Wrapping the limbs and thorax of cats undergoing ovariohysterectomy in a combination of bubble wrap and absorbent pads reduced heat loss, which in turn improved recovery time from general anaesthesia. Clinical relevance: This inexpensive and practical method may reduce perioperative hypothermia, in cats undergoing abdominal surgery.


Asunto(s)
Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal , Gatos/fisiología , Hipotermia/veterinaria , Histerectomía/veterinaria , Ovariectomía/veterinaria , Almohadillas Absorbentes , Animales , Temperatura Corporal , Femenino , Hipotermia/prevención & control , Histerectomía/métodos , Ovariectomía/métodos
2.
N Z Vet J ; 66(2): 85-92, 2018 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29207250

RESUMEN

AIMS: To survey current anaesthesia practices for dogs and cats in small and mixed animal practices in New Zealand in order to improve anaesthesia education. METHODS: A questionnaire was sent to 440 small and mixed animal practices, including questions regarding the type of practice, preanaesthetic examination, anaesthetic drugs and management, anaesthetic machines, monitoring and topics of interest for continuing professional development. RESULTS: Responses were obtained from 113/440 (26%) practices, with 78 (69%) respondents from small and 35 (31%) from mixed animal practices. A preanaesthetic physical examination was carried out by >95% of respondents and premedication was usually given to dogs (112/113; 99%) and cats (95/113; 85%). Acepromazine was the preferred sedative for dogs and cats, with morphine or buprenorphine. Propofol and alfaxalone were the preferred induction agents, and isoflurane was preferred for maintenance in both dogs and cats. A venous catheter was usually placed for anaesthesia in dogs (59/113; 52%), but less so in cats (39/113; 35%). Perioperative fluid was administered at 10 mL/kg/hour by 62/110 (56%) respondents. Intubation was usually used for anaesthesia in dogs (111/112; 99%), and cats (87/112; 78%). Almost 40% of respondents usually administered supplementary oxygen if patients were not intubated. Local analgesia was used by 69/111 (88%) respondents sometimes or always if applicable. Morphine or buprenorphine, and meloxicam were common choices for post-operative analgesia after neuter surgery in dogs and cats. A semiclosed (non-rebreathing) system was used in animals weighing <10 kg, and a Mapleson E or F non-rebreathing circuit was used by 66/109 (61%) practices. Only 15/111 (14%) practices had a ventilator in their practice. A dedicated anaesthetist was usually used by 104/113 (92%) practices, and apnoea alarms, pulse oximeters, thermometers and oesophageal stethoscopes were the main monitoring devices available in practices. Loco-regional block, pain management, and anaesthetic drugs were the main topics of interest for continuing education. CONCLUSIONS AND CLINICAL RELEVANCE: Responses by the veterinarians taking part in this survey indicated that they had a reasonably good standard of anaesthetic practice. A physical examination was carried out preanaesthesia, and premedication including analgesia was routinely administered to most patients. A dedicated anaesthetist usually monitored patients and most respondents reported they had access to basic anaesthetic monitoring equipment. Areas where changes could lead to improved anaesthetic practice were increased use of I/V catheterisation, endotracheal intubation, and supplementary oxygen, and reduced I/V fluid rates.


Asunto(s)
Anestesia/veterinaria , Anestésicos/uso terapéutico , Enfermedades de los Gatos/tratamiento farmacológico , Enfermedades de los Perros/tratamiento farmacológico , Utilización de Medicamentos , Anestesia/métodos , Anestésicos/administración & dosificación , Animales , Gatos , Perros , Utilización de Medicamentos/estadística & datos numéricos , Humanos , Monitoreo Fisiológico/veterinaria , Nueva Zelanda , Examen Físico/veterinaria , Cuidados Posoperatorios/veterinaria , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Veterinarios
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 282(1805)2015 Apr 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25808892

RESUMEN

Space and time are intimately coupled dimensions in the human brain. Several lines of evidence suggest that space and time are processed by a shared analogue magnitude system. It has been proposed that actions are instrumental in establishing this shared magnitude system. Here we provide evidence in support of this hypothesis, by showing that the interaction between space and time is enhanced when magnitude information is acquired through action. Participants observed increases or decreases in the height of a visual bar (spatial magnitude) while judging whether a simultaneously presented sequence of acoustic tones had accelerated or decelerated (temporal magnitude). In one condition (Action), participants directly controlled the changes in bar height with a hand grip device, whereas in the other (No Action), changes in bar height were externally controlled but matched the spatial/temporal profile of the Action condition. The sign of changes in bar height biased the perceived rate of the tone sequences, where increases in bar height produced apparent increases in tone rate. This effect was amplified when the visual bar was actively controlled in the Action condition, and the strength of the interaction was scaled by the magnitude of the action. Subsequent experiments ruled out that this was simply explained by attentional factors, and additionally showed that a monotonic mapping is also required between grip force and bar height in order to bias the perception of the tones. These data provide support for an instrumental role of action in interfacing spatial and temporal quantities in the brain.


Asunto(s)
Fuerza de la Mano , Percepción Espacial , Percepción del Tiempo , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
4.
J Neurophysiol ; 105(6): 2891-6, 2011 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21490286

RESUMEN

Imaging, lesion, and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) studies have implicated a number of regions of the brain in searching for a target defined by a combination of attributes. The necessity of both frontal eye fields (FEF) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) in task performance has been shown by the application of TMS over these regions. The effects of stimulation over these two areas have, thus far, proved to be remarkably similar and the only dissociation reported being in the timing of their involvement. We tested the hypotheses that 1) FEF contributes to performance in terms of visual target detection (possibly by modulation of activity in extrastriate areas with respect to the target), and 2) PPC is involved in translation of visual information for action. We used a task where the presence (and location) of the target was indicated by an eye movement. Task disruption was seen with FEF TMS (with reduced accuracy on the task) but not with PPC stimulation. When a search task requiring a manual response was presented, disruption with PPC TMS was seen. These results show dissociation of FEF and PPC contributions to visual search performance and that PPC involvement seems to be dependent on the response required by the task, whereas this is not the case for FEF. This supports the idea of FEF involvement in visual processes in a manner that might not depend on the required response, whereas PPC seems to be involved when a manual motor response to a stimulus is required.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Ojo/inervación , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/métodos , Adulto Joven
5.
Science ; 292(5516): 510-2, 2001 Apr 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11313497

RESUMEN

Much is known about the pathways from photoreceptors to higher visual areas in the brain. However, how we become aware of what we see or of having seen at all is a problem that has eluded neuroscience. Recordings from macaque V1 during deactivation of MT+/V5 and psychophysical studies of perceptual integration suggest that feedback from secondary visual areas to V1 is necessary for visual awareness. We used transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe the timing and function of feedback from human area MT+/V5 to V1 and found its action to be early and critical for awareness of visual motion.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación , Percepción de Movimiento , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Retroalimentación , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Magnetismo , Masculino , Fosfenos , Vías Visuales
6.
Cereb Cortex ; 18(10): 2410-5, 2008 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18326522

RESUMEN

The premotor theory of attention suggests that target processing and generation of a saccade to the target are interdependent. Temporally precise transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was delivered over the human frontal eye fields, the area most frequently associated with the premotor theory in association with eye movements, while subjects performed a visually instructed pro-/antisaccade task. Visual analysis and saccade preparation were clearly separated in time, as indicated by 2 distinct time points of TMS delivery that resulted in elevated saccade latencies. These results show that visual analysis and saccade preparation, although frequently enacted together, are dissociable processes.


Asunto(s)
Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
7.
Nat Neurosci ; 4(6): 656-61, 2001 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11369949

RESUMEN

It is widely agreed that the right posterior parietal cortex has a preeminent role in visuospatial and orienting attention. A number of lines of evidence suggest that although orienting and the preparation of oculomotor responses are dissociable from each other, the two are intimately related. If this is true, then it should be possible to identify other attentional mechanisms tied to other response modalities. We used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to demonstrate the existence of a distinct anterior parietal mechanism of motor attention. The critical area for motor attention is anterior to the one concerned with orienting, and it is lateralized to the left hemisphere in humans.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Mano/inervación , Humanos , Magnetismo
8.
Curr Biol ; 8(1): R8-11, 1998 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9427619

RESUMEN

Electromagnetic induction of focal currents in the brain - 'transcranial magnetic stimulation' - can be used to study cortical development and plasticity, as well as the organization of sensory and cognitive functions. It may also prove to be useful tool in the treatment of depression.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Magnetoencefalografía , Animales , Cognición , Humanos
9.
Curr Biol ; 10(12): R460-2, 2000 Jun 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10873796

RESUMEN

The two cerebral hemispheres are specialised for different cognitive functions, and which hemisphere's strategy is superior depends on the nature of the task. A new study of split-brain patients has provided another unexpected insight: the two hemispheres use different strategies when performing a guessing task.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional , Animales , Cognición/fisiología , Humanos
10.
Curr Biol ; 8(16): R572-4, 1998.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9707394

RESUMEN

The brain areas involved in a task may change their contribution as one acquires expertise. An understanding of how this occurs relies on a good psychological theory of the processing requirements of a task and knowledge of which regions of the brain can perform the necessary component computations.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Lectura
11.
Curr Biol ; 5(7): 703-5, 1995 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7583108

RESUMEN

The colours we see reflect not only the light wavelengths presently being detected, but also those already received. To understand colour constancy therefore requires an understanding of adaptation in the visual system.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica , Animales , Humanos , Modelos Biológicos
12.
Curr Biol ; 6(9): 1079-81, 1996 Sep 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8805349

RESUMEN

About half of the readers of this article would not be able to tell whether a picture of the Mona Lisa is correct or mirror-reversed. Neuropsychological studies may help explain this striking dissociation between the ability to identify images and memory for their handedness.


Asunto(s)
Neuropsicología , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas , Percepción Visual , Animales , Humanos , Corteza Visual/fisiopatología
13.
Curr Biol ; 6(4): 389-91, 1996 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8723340

RESUMEN

Some people perceive colours when they hear words--a phenomenon known as synaesthesia. Brain imaging techniques have revealed the areas of the cortex involved in this rare mingling of the senses.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Oído , Humanos
14.
Curr Biol ; 5(11): 1216-7, 1995 Nov 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8574571

RESUMEN

Reading is one of the most complex tasks in which vision is involved. Our understanding of the visual system is contributing to the analysis of dyslexia, a reading disorder which may reflect abnormal visual processes.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/fisiopatología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Lectura , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Humanos , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología
15.
Curr Biol ; 10(1): R34-5, 2000 Jan 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10660292

RESUMEN

Some visual attributes, such as colour, are purely visual, but others, such as orientation and movement, can be perceived by touch or audition. A magnetic stimulation study has now shown that the perception of tactile orientation may be influenced by visual Information.


Asunto(s)
Tacto/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos , Magnetismo , Plasticidad Neuronal , Tomografía Computarizada de Emisión , Corteza Visual/fisiología
16.
Curr Biol ; 7(4): R249-51, 1997 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9162503

RESUMEN

The Hebbian synapse and Hebb learning rule are familiar to those working on biological and machine learning. But Hebb's insights from over fifty years ago carry many other lessons in learning and may contribute to a more parsimonious taxonomy of the mechanisms involved.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Sinapsis/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Animales , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Visión Ocular
17.
Curr Biol ; 11(4): R125-7, 2001 Feb 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11250165

RESUMEN

Music may be the food of love but it is also good fodder for cognitive scientists. Here we highlight a recent study of a neuropsychological patient who has lost her ability to read music, but not text, in the absence of any other musical deficit.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Música , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Humanos , Leucoencefalitis Hemorrágica Aguda/fisiopatología , Modelos Neurológicos , Neuropsicología , Lóbulo Temporal/lesiones
18.
Curr Biol ; 11(7): R261-3, 2001 Apr 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11413014

RESUMEN

Every neuroscientist knows that emotions are as much to do with the head as the heart, but as a number of new studies show, the heart - or rather the body - and the brain are by no means independent purveyors of feeling and emotion.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Corazón/fisiología , Enfermedades del Sistema Nervioso Autónomo , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Humanos , Neurobiología , Estrés Fisiológico
19.
Curr Biol ; 11(15): R613-5, 2001 Aug 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11516969

RESUMEN

Learning leads to neural changes often considered to be driven by 'smart' areas of the brain. A recent study of the cellular changes that underlie perceptual learning has found that plasticity in the primary visual cortex V1 is necessary for learning and the changes that correlate with learning are more complex than one might expect.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Plasticidad Neuronal
20.
Curr Biol ; 9(5): R184-5, 1999 Mar 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10074447

RESUMEN

Magnetic stimulation as used in studies of the human brain may not merely disrupt cognitive functions, but also enhance them. The direction of the effect may depend on the frequency of stimulation as much as the area of the brain that is stimulated.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Animales , Estimulación Eléctrica , Humanos , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA