Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 24
Filtrar
1.
PLoS Biol ; 19(12): e3001466, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34932558

RESUMEN

Gamma rhythms in many brain regions, including the primary visual cortex (V1), are thought to play a role in information processing. Here, we report a surprising finding of 3 narrowband gamma rhythms in V1 that processed distinct spatial frequency (SF) signals and had different neural origins. The low gamma (LG; 25 to 40 Hz) rhythm was generated at the V1 superficial layer and preferred a higher SF compared with spike activity, whereas both the medium gamma (MG; 40 to 65 Hz), generated at the cortical level, and the high gamma HG; (65 to 85 Hz), originated precortically, preferred lower SF information. Furthermore, compared with the rates of spike activity, the powers of the 3 gammas had better performance in discriminating the edge and surface of simple objects. These findings suggest that gamma rhythms reflect the neural dynamics of neural circuitries that process different SF information in the visual system, which may be crucial for multiplexing SF information and synchronizing different features of an object.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Gamma/fisiología , Corteza Visual Primaria/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Encéfalo/fisiología , Gatos , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Visual Primaria/patología , Corteza Visual/fisiología
2.
J Vis ; 23(14): 4, 2023 Dec 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38091030

RESUMEN

Gestalten in visual perception are defined by emergent properties of the whole, which cannot be predicted from the sum of its parts; rather, they arise by virtue of inherent principles, the Laws of Seeing. This review attempts to assign neurophysiological correlates to select emergent properties in motion and contour perception and proposes parallels to the processing of local versus global attributes by classical versus contextual receptive fields. The aim is to identify Gestalt neurons in the visual system to account for the Laws of Seeing in causal terms and to explain "Why do things look as they do" (Koffka, 1935, p. 76).


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología
3.
Neuromodulation ; 24(5): 863-869, 2021 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32270579

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: It would be a medically important advance if durable and focal neuromodulation of the brain could be delivered noninvasively and without ablation. This ongoing study seeks to elucidate the effects of precisely delivered ionizing radiation upon focal brain metabolism and the corresponding cellular integrity at that target. We hypothesize that focally delivered ionizing radiation to the brain can yield focal metabolic changes without lesioning the brain in the process. MATERIALS AND METHODS: We used stereotactic radiosurgery to deliver doses from 10 Gy to 120 Gy to the left primary motor cortex (M1) of Lee Sung miniature pigs (n = 8). One additional animal served as a nonirradiated control. We used positron emission tomography-computed tomography (PET-CT) to quantify radiation dose-dependent effects by calculating the ratio of standard uptake values (SUV) of 2-deoxy-2-[18 F]-fluoro-D-glucose (18 F-FDG) between the radiated (left) and irradiated (right) hemispheres across nine months. RESULTS: We found that the FDG-PET SUV ratio at the targeted M1 was significantly lowered from the pre-radiation baseline measurements for animals receiving 60 Gy or higher, with the effect persisting at nine months after radiosurgery. Only at 120 Gy was a lesion suggesting ablation visible at the M1 target. Animals treated at 60-100 Gy showed a reduced signal in the absence of an identifiable lesion, a result consistent with the occurrence of neuromodulation. CONCLUSION: Focal, noninvasive, and durable changes in brain activity can be induced without a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-visible lesion, a result that may be consistent with the occurrence of neuromodulation. This approach may provide new venues for the investigation of neuromodulatory treatments for disorders involving dysfunctional brain circuits. Postmortem pathological analysis is needed to elucidate whether there have been morphological changes not detected by MRI.


Asunto(s)
Glucosa , Tomografía Computarizada por Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Animales , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Fluorodesoxiglucosa F18 , Tomografía de Emisión de Positrones , Porcinos , Porcinos Enanos , Tomografía Computarizada por Rayos X
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(3): 1210-5, 2014 Jan 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24398523

RESUMEN

Darkness and brightness are very different perceptually. To understand the neural basis for the visual difference, we studied the dynamical states of populations of neurons in macaque primary visual cortex when a spatially uniform area (8° × 8°) of the visual field alternated between black and white. Darkness evoked sustained nerve-impulse spiking in primary visual cortex neurons, but bright stimuli evoked only a transient response. A peak in the local field potential (LFP) γ band (30-80 Hz) occurred during darkness; white-induced LFP fluctuations were of lower amplitude, peaking at 25 Hz. However, the sustained response to white in the evoked LFP was larger than for black. Together with the results on spiking, the LFP results imply that, throughout the stimulus period, bright fields evoked strong net sustained inhibition. Such cortical brightness adaptation can explain many perceptual phenomena: interocular speeding up of dark adaptation, tonic interocular suppression, and interocular masking.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Oscuridad , Electrofisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Macaca fascicularis , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Visión Ocular , Campos Visuales , Percepción Visual/fisiología
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(34): 13871-6, 2012 Aug 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22872866

RESUMEN

Studying the laminar pattern of neural activity is crucial for understanding the processing of neural signals in the cerebral cortex. We measured neural population activity [multiunit spike activity (MUA) and local field potential, LFP] in Macaque primary visual cortex (V1) in response to drifting grating stimuli. Sustained visually driven MUA was at an approximately constant level across cortical depth in V1. However, sustained, visually driven, local field potential power, which was concentrated in the γ-band (20-60 Hz), was greatest at the cortical depth corresponding to cortico-cortical output layers 2, 3, and 4B. γ-band power also tends to be more sustained in the output layers. Overall, cortico-cortical output layers accounted for 67% of total γ-band activity in V1, whereas 56% of total spikes evoked by drifting gratings were from layers 2, 3, and 4B. The high-resolution layer specificity of γ-band power, the laminar distribution of MUA and γ-band activity, and their dynamics imply that neural activity in V1 is generated by laminar-specific mechanisms. In particular, visual responses of MUA and γ-band activity in cortico-cortical output layers 2, 3, and 4B seem to be strongly influenced by laminar-specific recurrent circuitry and/or feedback.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Electrodos , Electrofisiología/métodos , Macaca , Modelos Biológicos , Neuronas/metabolismo , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Factores de Tiempo , Percepción Visual/fisiología
6.
J Neurosci ; 32(40): 13873-80a, 2012 Oct 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23035096

RESUMEN

Oscillatory neural activity within the gamma band (25-90 Hz) is generally thought to be able to provide a timing signal for harmonizing neural computations across different brain regions. Using time-frequency analyses of the dynamics of gamma-band activity in the local field potentials recorded from monkey primary visual cortex, we found identical temporal characteristics of gamma activity in both awake and anesthetized brain states, including large variability of peak frequency, brief oscillatory epochs (<100 ms on average), and stochastic statistics of the incidence and duration of oscillatory events. These findings indicate that gamma-band activity is temporally unstructured and is inherently a stochastic signal generated by neural networks. This idea was corroborated further by our neural-network simulations. Our results suggest that gamma-band activity is too random to serve as a clock signal for synchronizing neuronal responses in awake as in anesthetized monkeys. Instead, gamma-band activity is more likely to be filtered neuronal network noise. Its mean frequency changes with global state and is reduced under anesthesia.


Asunto(s)
Anestesia , Relojes Biológicos/fisiología , Ondas Encefálicas/fisiología , Sincronización de Fase en Electroencefalografía/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vigilia/fisiología , Anestesia/psicología , Animales , Macaca fascicularis , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Procesos Estocásticos , Factores de Tiempo , Percepción Visual/fisiología
7.
Nature ; 449(7158): 92-5, 2007 Sep 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17805296

RESUMEN

The timing of action potentials relative to sensory stimuli can be precise down to milliseconds in the visual system, even though the relevant timescales of natural vision are much slower. The existence of such precision contributes to a fundamental debate over the basis of the neural code and, specifically, what timescales are important for neural computation. Using recordings in the lateral geniculate nucleus, here we demonstrate that the relevant timescale of neuronal spike trains depends on the frequency content of the visual stimulus, and that 'relative', not absolute, precision is maintained both during spatially uniform white-noise visual stimuli and naturalistic movies. Using information-theoretic techniques, we demonstrate a clear role of relative precision, and show that the experimentally observed temporal structure in the neuronal response is necessary to represent accurately the more slowly changing visual world. By establishing a functional role of precision, we link visual neuron function on slow timescales to temporal structure in the response at faster timescales, and uncover a straightforward purpose of fine-timescale features of neuronal spike trains.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Cuerpos Geniculados/citología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Gatos , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Factores de Tiempo
8.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 106(34): 14652-7, 2009 Aug 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19706551

RESUMEN

The concept of receptive field is a linear, feed-forward view of visual signal processing. Frequently used models of V1 neurons, like the dynamic Linear filter--static nonlinearity--Poisson [corrected] spike encoder model, predict that receptive fields measured with different stimulus ensembles should be similar. Here, we tested this concept by comparing spatiotemporal maps of V1 neurons derived from two very different, but commonly used, stimulus ensembles: sparse noise and Hartley subspace stimuli. We found maps from the two methods agreed for neurons in input layer 4C but were very different for neurons in superficial layers of V1. Many layer 2/3 cells have receptive fields with multiple elongated subregions when mapped with Hartley stimuli, but their spatial maps collapse to only a single, less-elongated subregion when mapped with sparse noise. Moreover, for upper layer V1 neurons, the preferred orientation for Hartley maps is much closer to the preferred orientation measured with drifting gratings than is the orientation preference of sparse-noise maps. These results challenge the concept of a stimulus-invariant receptive field and imply that intracortical interactions shape fundamental properties of layer 2/3 neurons.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Algoritmos , Animales , Encéfalo/citología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Electrodos Implantados , Electrofisiología , Macaca fascicularis , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Visual/citología
9.
Nat Neurosci ; 11(1): 88-94, 2008 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18084287

RESUMEN

On- and off-center geniculate afferents form two major channels of visual processing that are thought to converge in the primary visual cortex. However, humans with severely reduced on responses can have normal visual acuity when tested in a white background, which indicates that off channels can function relatively independently from on channels under certain conditions. Consistent with this functional independence of channels, we demonstrate here that on- and off-center geniculate afferents segregate in different domains of the cat primary visual cortex and that off responses dominate the cortical representation of the area centralis. On average, 70% of the geniculate afferents converging at the same cortical domain had receptive fields of the same contrast polarity. Moreover, off-center afferents dominated the representation of the area centralis in the cortex, but not in the thalamus, indicating that on- and off-center afferents are balanced in number, but not in the amount of cortical territory that they cover.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Gatos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/efectos de la radiación , Agonistas del GABA , Muscimol/farmacología , Neuronas Aferentes/fisiología , Corteza Visual/citología , Corteza Visual/efectos de los fármacos , Vías Visuales
10.
PNAS Nexus ; 1(4): pgac188, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36714857

RESUMEN

An ongoing debate regarding the evolution of morality is whether other species show precursory moral behavior. The veil of ignorance (VOI) paradigm is often used to elicit human moral judgment but has never been tested in other primates. We study the division of resources behind the VOI in Formosan macaques. Monkeys choose the equal division more often when a conspecific is present than when it is absent, suggesting a degree of impartiality. To better understand this impartiality, we measure a monkey's reactions to two directions of inequity: one regarding inequity to its advantage and the other to its disadvantage. We find that disadvantageous inequity aversion correlates with the degree of impartiality behind the VOI. Therefore, seemingly impartial behavior could result from a primitive negative reaction to being disadvantaged. This suggests a mechanism to explain a tendency toward impartiality.

11.
Neuron ; 55(3): 479-91, 2007 Aug 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17678859

RESUMEN

In this study, we characterize the adaptation of neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus to changes in stimulus contrast and correlations. By comparing responses to high- and low-contrast natural scene movie and white noise stimuli, we show that an increase in contrast or correlations results in receptive fields with faster temporal dynamics and stronger antagonistic surrounds, as well as decreases in gain and selectivity. We also observe contrast- and correlation-induced changes in the reliability and sparseness of neural responses. We find that reliability is determined primarily by processing in the receptive field (the effective contrast of the stimulus), while sparseness is determined by the interactions between several functional properties. These results reveal a number of adaptive phenomena and suggest that adaptation to stimulus contrast and correlations may play an important role in visual coding in a dynamic natural environment.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Naturaleza , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Gatos , Cuerpos Geniculados/citología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Factores de Tiempo
12.
J Neurosci ; 30(40): 13504-12, 2010 Oct 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20926676

RESUMEN

Consistent with human perceptual data, we found many more black-dominant than white-dominant responses in layer 2/3 neurons of the macaque primary visual cortex (V1). Seeking the mechanism of this black dominance of layer 2/3 neurons, we measured the laminar pattern of population responses (multiunit activity and local field potential) and found that a small preference for black is observable in early responses in layer 4Cß, the parvocellular-input layer, but not in the magnocellular-input layer 4Cα. Surprisingly, further analysis of the dynamics of black-white responses in layers 4Cß and 2/3 suggested that black-dominant responses in layer 2/3 were not generated simply because of the weak black-dominant inputs from 4Cß. Instead, our results indicated the neural circuitry in V1 is wired with a preference to strengthen black responses. We hypothesize that this selective wiring could be due to (1) feedforward connectivity from black-dominant neurons in layer 4C to cells in layer 2/3 or (2) recurrent interactions between black-dominant neurons in layer 2/3, or a combination of both.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Visión de Colores/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Axones/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Electrofisiología/métodos , Macaca fascicularis , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Visual/citología
13.
Front Neural Circuits ; 15: 708459, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34566582

RESUMEN

Paired stimulation has been applied to modulate neuronal functions in the primary somatosensory cortex but its utility in the alternation of tuning function, such as direction tuning for whisker stimuli, remains unclear. In the present study, we attempted to manipulate feature preferences in barrel cortical neurons using repetitive paired whisker deflection combined with optogenetic stimulation and to obtain optimal parameters that can induce neuroplasticity. We found no significant response changes across stimulus parameters, such as onset asynchronies and paired directions. Only when paired stimulation was applied in the nonpreferred direction of the principal whisker of a neuron, were the neuron's responses enhanced in that direction. Importantly, this effect was only observed when the optogenetic stimulus preceded the mechanical stimulus. Our findings indicate that repetitive paired optogenetic-mechanical stimulation can induce in vivo neuroplasticity of feature selectivity in limited situations.


Asunto(s)
Optogenética , Vibrisas , Animales , Plasticidad Neuronal , Neuronas , Estimulación Física , Corteza Somatosensorial
14.
J Neurosci ; 29(37): 11540-9, 2009 Sep 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19759301

RESUMEN

We developed a new method to estimate the spatial extent of summation, the cortical spread, of the local field potential (LFP) throughout all layers of macaque primary visual cortex V1 by taking advantage of the V1 retinotopic map. We mapped multi-unit activity and LFP visual responses with sparse-noise at several cortical sites simultaneously. The cortical magnification factor near the recording sites was precisely estimated by track reconstruction. The new method combined experimental measurements together with a model of signal summation to obtain the cortical spread of the LFP. This new method could be extended to cortical areas that have topographic maps such as S1 or A1, and to cortical areas without functional columnar maps, such as rodent visual cortex. In macaque V1, the LFP was the sum of signals from a very local region, the radius of which was on average 250 microm. The LFP's cortical spread varied across cortical layers, reaching a minimum value of 120 microm in layer 4B. An important functional consequence of the small cortical spread of the LFP is that the visual field maps of LFP and MUA recorded at a single electrode site were very similar. The similar spatial scale of the visual responses, the restricted cortical spread, and their laminar variation led to new insights about the sources and possible applications of the LFP.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Macaca fascicularis , Modelos Neurológicos , Dinámicas no Lineales , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Visual/anatomía & histología , Vías Visuales/fisiología
15.
J Neurosci ; 29(38): 11753-60, 2009 Sep 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19776262

RESUMEN

Achromatic visual information is transferred from the retina to the brain through two parallel channels: ON-center cells carry "white" information and OFF-center cells "black" information (Nelson et al., 1978; Schiller, 1982; Schiller et al., 1986). Responses of ON and OFF retinal and thalamic neurons are approximately equal in magnitude (Krüger and Fischer, 1975; Kremers et al., 1993), but psychophysical studies have shown that humans detect light decrements (black) better and faster than increments (white) (Blackwell, 1946; Short, 1966; Krauskopf, 1980; Whittle, 1986; Bowen et al., 1989; Chan and Tyler, 1992; Kontsevich and Tyler, 1999; Chubb and Nam, 2000; Dannemiller and Stephens, 2001). From recordings of single-cell activity in the macaque monkey's primary visual cortex (V1), we found that black-dominant neurons substantially outnumbered white-dominant neurons in the corticocortical output layers 2/3, but the numbers of black- and white-dominant neurons were nearly equal in the thalamocortical input layer 4c. These results strongly suggest that the black-over-white preference is generated or greatly amplified in V1. The predominance of OFF neurons in layers 2/3 of V1, which provide visual input to higher cortical areas, may explain why human subjects detect black more easily than white. Furthermore, our results agree with human EEG and fMRI findings that V1 responses to decrements are stronger than to increments, though the OFF/ON imbalance we found in layers 2/3 of macaque V1 is much larger than in the whole V1 population in the human V1 experiments (Zemon et al., 1988, 1995; Olman et al., 2008).


Asunto(s)
Color , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción , Animales , Macaca fascicularis , Microelectrodos , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Tálamo/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Vías Visuales/fisiología
16.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 5494, 2020 03 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32218502

RESUMEN

The hand explores the environment for obtaining tactile information that can be fruitfully integrated with other functions, such as vision, audition, and movement. In theory, somatosensory signals gathered by the hand are accurately mapped in the world-centered (allocentric) reference frame such that the multi-modal information signals, whether visual-tactile or motor-tactile, are perfectly aligned. However, an accumulating body of evidence indicates that the perceived tactile orientation or direction is inaccurate; yielding a surprisingly large perceptual bias. To investigate such perceptual bias, this study presented tactile motion stimuli to healthy adult participants in a variety of finger and head postures, and requested the participants to report the perceived direction of motion mapped on a video screen placed on the frontoparallel plane in front of the eyes. Experimental results showed that the perceptual bias could be divided into systematic and nonsystematic biases. Systematic bias, defined as the mean difference between the perceived and veridical directions, correlated linearly with the relative posture between the finger and the head. By contrast, nonsystematic bias, defined as minor difference in bias for different stimulus directions, was highly individualized, phase-locked to stimulus orientation presented on the skin. Overall, the present findings on systematic bias indicate that the transformation bias among the reference frames is dominated by the finger-to-head posture. Moreover, the highly individualized nature of nonsystematic bias reflects how information is obtained by the orientation-selective units in the S1 cortex.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Postura/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Sesgo , Femenino , Dedos , Cabeza , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Estimulación Física
17.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 9354, 2020 Jun 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32493910

RESUMEN

An amendment to this paper has been published and can be accessed via a link at the top of the paper.

18.
PLoS Biol ; 4(7): e209, 2006 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16756389

RESUMEN

In the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus, visual stimulation produces two distinct types of responses known as tonic and burst. Due to the dynamics of the T-type Ca(2+) channels involved in burst generation, the type of response evoked by a particular stimulus depends on the resting membrane potential, which is controlled by a network of modulatory connections from other brain areas. In this study, we use simulated responses to natural scene movies to describe how modulatory and stimulus-driven changes in LGN membrane potential interact to determine the luminance sequences that trigger burst responses. We find that at low resting potentials, when the T channels are de-inactivated and bursts are relatively frequent, an excitatory stimulus transient alone is sufficient to evoke a burst. However, to evoke a burst at high resting potentials, when the T channels are inactivated and bursts are relatively rare, prolonged inhibitory stimulation followed by an excitatory transient is required. We also observe evidence of these effects in vivo, where analysis of experimental recordings demonstrates that the luminance sequences that trigger bursts can vary dramatically with the overall burst percentage of the response. To characterize the functional consequences of the effects of resting potential on burst generation, we simulate LGN responses to different luminance sequences at a range of resting potentials with and without a mechanism for generating bursts. Using analysis based on signal detection theory, we show that bursts enhance detection of specific luminance sequences, ranging from the onset of excitatory sequences at low resting potentials to the offset of inhibitory sequences at high resting potentials. These results suggest a dynamic role for burst responses during visual processing that may change according to behavioral state.


Asunto(s)
Canales de Calcio Tipo T/fisiología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Péptidos y Proteínas de Señalización Intracelular/metabolismo , Animales , Gatos , Potenciales de la Membrana , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tálamo/metabolismo , Vías Visuales/metabolismo , Percepción Visual/fisiología
19.
Front Neurosci ; 13: 605, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31258463

RESUMEN

Psychophysical visual experiments have shown illusory motion reversal (IMR), in which the perceived direction of motion is the opposite of its actual direction. The tactile form of this illusion has also been reported. However, it remains unclear which stimulus characteristics affect the magnitude of IMR. We closely examined the effect of stimulus characteristics on IMR by presenting moving sinusoid gratings and random-dot patterns to 10 participants' fingerpads at different spatial periods, speeds, and indentation depths. All participants perceived a motion direction opposite to the veridical direction some of the time. The illusion was more prevalent at spatial periods of 1 and 2 mm and at extreme speeds of 20 and 320 mm/s. We observed stronger IMR for gratings and much weaker IMR for a random-dot pattern, indicating that edge orientation might be a major contributor to this illusion. These results show that the optimal parameters for IMR are consistent with the characteristics of motion-selective neurons in the somatosensory cortex, as most of these neurons are also orientation-selective. We speculate that these neurons could be the neural substrate that accounts for tactile IMR.

20.
Behav Neurosci ; 119(6): 1656-61, 2005 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16420168

RESUMEN

Turtles (Chrysemys picta) were given the nitric oxide synthase inhibitor NW-nitro-L-arginine methyl ester (L-NAME) or its inactive isomer NW-nitro-D-arginine methyl ester (D-NAME) and were trained on a negative patterning task or a simple go/no-go discrimination task. L-NAME impaired the learning of negative patterning but did not affect retention of the task if it had already been learned. D-NAME had no effect. Go/no-go discrimination learning was not affected by L-NAME. These findings support the notion that nitric oxide plays a role in complex configural learning in a reptile closely related to the ancestors of mammals.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , NG-Nitroarginina Metil Éster/farmacología , Óxido Nítrico/antagonistas & inhibidores , Tortugas/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal , Conducta de Elección/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/efectos de los fármacos , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Inhibidores Enzimáticos/farmacología , Aprendizaje/efectos de los fármacos , NG-Nitroarginina Metil Éster/análogos & derivados , Probabilidad , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA