Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 26
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Hum Evol ; 196: 103591, 2024 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39366305

RESUMEN

The evolution of the human hand is a topic of great interest in paleoanthropology. As the hand can be involved in a vast array of activities, knowledge regarding how it was used by early hominins can yield crucial information on the factors driving biocultural evolution. Previous research on early hominin hands focused on the overall bone shape. However, while such approaches can inform on mechanical abilities and the evolved efficiency of manipulation, they cannot be used as a definite proxy for individual habitual activity. Accordingly, it is crucial to examine bone structures more responsive to lifetime biomechanical loading, such as muscle attachment sites or internal bone architecture. In this study, we investigate the manual entheseal patterns of Australopithecus afarensis, Australopithecus africanus, and Australopithecus sediba through the application of the validated entheses-based reconstruction of activity method. Using a comparative sample of later Homo and three great ape genera, we analyze the muscle attachment site proportions on the thumb, fifth ray, and third intermediate phalanx to gain insight into the habitual hand use of Australopithecus. We use a novel statistical procedure to account for the effects of interspecies variation in overall size and ray proportions. Our results highlight the importance of certain muscles of the first and fifth digits for humanlike hand use. In humans, these muscles are required for variable in-hand manipulation and are activated during stone-tool production. The entheses of A. sediba suggest muscle activation patterns consistent with a similar suite of habitual manual activities as in later Homo. In contrast, A. africanus and A. afarensis display a mosaic entheseal pattern that combines indications of both humanlike and apelike manipulation. Overall, these findings provide new evidence that some australopith species were already habitually engaging in humanlike manipulation, even if their manual dexterity was likely not as high as in later Homo.


Asunto(s)
Hominidae , Animales , Hominidae/anatomía & histología , Hominidae/fisiología , Mano/anatomía & histología , Mano/fisiología , Fósiles/anatomía & histología , Evolución Biológica , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Femenino , Masculino , Humanos
2.
J Neuroeng Rehabil ; 20(1): 93, 2023 07 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37464404

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To compare the efficacy of Dextrain Manipulandum™ training of dexterity components such as force control and independent finger movements, to dose-matched conventional therapy (CT) post-stroke. METHODS: A prospective, single-blind, pilot randomized clinical trial was conducted. Chronic-phase post-stroke patients with mild-to-moderate dexterity impairment (Box and Block Test (BBT) > 1) received 12 sessions of Dextrain or CT. Blinded measures were obtained before and after training and at 3-months follow-up. Primary outcome was BBT-change (after-before training). Secondary outcomes included changes in motor impairments, activity limitations and dexterity components. Corticospinal excitability and short intracortical inhibition (SICI) were measured using transcranial magnetic stimulation. RESULTS: BBT-change after training did not differ between the Dextrain (N = 21) vs CT group (N = 21) (median [IQR] = 5[2-7] vs 4[2-7], respectively; P = 0.36). Gains in BBT were maintained at the 3-month post-training follow-up, with a non-significant trend for enhanced BBT-change in the Dextrain group (median [IQR] = 3[- 1-7.0], P = 0.06). Several secondary outcomes showed significantly larger changes in the Dextrain group: finger tracking precision (mean ± SD = 0.3 ± 0.3N vs - 0.1 ± 0.33N; P < 0.0018), independent finger movements (34.7 ± 25.1 ms vs 7.7 ± 18.5 ms, P = 0.02) and maximal finger tapping speed (8.4 ± 7.1 vs 4.5 ± 4.9, P = 0.045). At follow-up, Dextrain group showed significantly greater improvement in Motor Activity Log (median/IQR = 0.7/0.2-0.8 vs 0.2/0.1-0.6, P = 0.05). Across both groups SICI increased in patients with greater BBT-change (Rho = 0.80, P = 0.006). Comparing Dextrain subgroups with maximal grip force higher/lower than median (61.2%), BBT-change was significantly larger in patients with low vs high grip force (7.5 ± 5.6 vs 2.9 ± 2.8; respectively, P = 0.015). CONCLUSIONS: Although immediate improvements in gross dexterity post-stroke did not significantly differ between Dextrain training and CT, our findings suggest that Dextrain enhances recovery of several dexterity components and reported hand-use, particularly when motor impairment is moderate (low initial grip force). Findings need to be confirmed in a larger trial. Trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03934073 (retrospectively registered).


Asunto(s)
Rehabilitación de Accidente Cerebrovascular , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Humanos , Método Simple Ciego , Estudios Prospectivos , Recuperación de la Función , Resultado del Tratamiento , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Extremidad Superior
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 239(6): 1895-1909, 2021 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33870438

RESUMEN

Hand use is a widespread act in many vertebrate lineages and subserves behaviors including locomotion, predation, feeding, nest construction, and grooming. In order to determine whether hand use is similarly used in social behavior, the present paper describes hand use in the social play of rats. In the course of rough and tumble play sessions, rats are found to make as many as twenty different movements a minute with each hand for the purposes of manipulating a partner into a subordinate position or defending against a partner's attack. The hand movements comprise signaling movements of touching, offensive manipulating of a partner to control a play engagement, and defensive hand movements directed toward blocking, pushing and pulling to parry an attack. For signaling, attack and defense, hand movements have a structure that is similar to the structure of hand movements used for other purposes including eating, but in their contact points on an opponent, they are tailored for partner control. Given the time devoted to play by rats, play likely features the social rat behavior with the most extensive use of hand movements. This extensive use of hand movements for social play is discussed in relation to the ubiquity of hand use in adaptive behavior, the evolution of hand use in the play of mammals, and in relation to extending the multifunctional theory of the purposes of play to include the education of skilled hand movements for various adult functions including as feeding.


Asunto(s)
Mano , Movimiento , Animales , Ratas , Conducta Social , Tacto
4.
J Intellect Disabil Res ; 62(3): 262-268, 2018 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29250853

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Children with developmental disabilities (DD) may experience limited participation in school activities. Little is known about whether school participation of children with DD who attend special schools is impacted. This study specifically focused on physical engagement in school-related activities that require hand use for the comparison between this group of children with DD and typically developing children. METHODS: The sample consisted of 97 children with DD who attended special schools (mean age 8.2 ± 2.9 years; 60 boys and 37 girls) and 105 typically developing children who attended mainstream schools/kindergartens (mean age 8.6 ± 2.4 years; 48 boys and 57 girls). Parents completed the Children's Assessment of Participation with Hands, one of the domains of which captures participation in eight school-related activities involving hand use. RESULTS: Parents of children with DD reported that their children participated less, in terms of the number (χ2  = 8.45-14.97, P ≤ 0.004) and frequency (t = 4.00-6.47, P < 0.001), in four activities than typically developing children. Parents of children with DD also reported that more assistance was needed for their children's participation in all activities (t = 6.93-11.92, P < 0.001), and they wanted their children to participate in most activities more often and more independently (χ2  = 18.46-59.34, P < 0.001). CONCLUSIONS: Differences in participation in school-related activities requiring hand use between children with DD and typically developing children were revealed generally across all participation dimensions (does participate, frequency, independence, and desired change). This study provides information on the areas in which greater efforts are needed to support children's school participation.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Discapacidades del Desarrollo/fisiopatología , Mano , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Instituciones Académicas , Niño , Educación Especial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Laterality ; 23(6): 705-721, 2018 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29607706

RESUMEN

It has been suggested that strepsirrhines (lemurs, lorises, and galagos) retain the more primitive left-hand preference, whilst monkeys and apes more regularly display a right-hand preference at the individual-level. We looked to address questions of laterality in the slow loris (Nycticebus spp.) using spontaneous observations of 7 wild individuals, unimanual tests in 6 captive individuals, and photos of 42 individuals in a bilateral posture assessing handedness at the individual- and group-level. During the unimanual reach task, we found at the individual-level, only 4 slow lorises showed a hand use bias (R: 3, L: 1), Handedness index (HI) ranged from -0.57 to 1.00. In the wild unimanual grasp task, we found at the individual-level two individual showed a right-hand bias, the HI ranged from -0.19 to 0.70. The bilateral venom pose showed a trend toward a right-hand dominant grip in those photographed in captivity, but an ambiguous difference in wild individuals. There are many environmental constraints in captivity that wild animals do not face, thus data collected in wild settings are more representative of their natural state. The presence of right-handedness in these species suggests that there is a need to re-evaluate the evolution of handedness in primates.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Mano , Lorisidae , Animales , Animales Salvajes , Femenino , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Postura
6.
Behav Res Methods ; 50(3): 1074-1087, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28707213

RESUMEN

The Trail Making Test (TMT) is used in neuropsychological clinical practice to assess aspects of attention and executive function. The test consists of two parts (A and B) and requires drawing a trail between elements. Many patients are assessed with their non-dominant hand because of motor dysfunction that prevents them from using their dominant hand. Since drawing with the non-dominant hand is not an automatic task for many people, we explored the effect of hand use on TMT performance. The TMT was administered digitally in order to analyze new outcome measures in addition to total completion time. In a sample of 82 healthy participants, we found that non-dominant hand use increased completion times on the TMT B but not on the TMT A. The average completion time increased by almost 5 seconds, which may be clinically relevant. A substantial number of participants who performed the TMT with their non-dominant hand had a B/A ratio score of 2.5 or higher. In clinical practice, an abnormally high B/A ratio score may be falsely attributed to cognitive dysfunction. With our digitized pen data, we further explored the causes of the reduced TMT B performance by using new outcome measures, including individual element completion times and interelement variability. These measures indicated selective interference between non-dominant hand use and executive functions. Both non-dominant hand use and performance of the TMT B seem to draw on the same, limited higher-order cognitive resources.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Lateralidad Funcional , Prueba de Secuencia Alfanumérica , Adulto , Anciano , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
7.
Ergonomics ; 58(2): 195-207, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25434777

RESUMEN

In this study the effects of horizontal bin locations in an order picking workstation on upper arm elevation, trunk inclination and hand use were investigated. Eight subjects moved (self-paced) light or heavy products (0.2 and 3.0 kg) from a central product bin to an inner or outer order bin (at 60 or 150 cm) on the left or right side of the workstation, while movements were recorded. The outer compared to inner bin location resulted in more upper arm elevation and trunk inclination per work cycle, both in terms of number of peak values and in terms of time integrals of angles (which is a dose measure over time). Considering the peak values and time integrals per minute (instead of per work cycle), these effects are reduced, due to the higher cycle times for outer bins. Hand use (left, right or both) was not affected by order bin locations.


Asunto(s)
Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Hombro/fisiología , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Trabajo/fisiología , Adulto , Brazo/fisiología , Femenino , Mano/fisiología , Voluntarios Sanos , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Torso/fisiología
8.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 153(1): 61-7, 2014 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24318942

RESUMEN

The dominant use of one hand is a striking feature of humans, but manual lateralization can be found in a variety of other species as well. In primates, the lateralization in hand use varies among species and several theories such as the "postural origin," "task complexity," or "development theory" have been suggested to explain this variation. In order to contribute comparative data on this phenomenon from a basal primate, we studied manual lateralization in wild redfronted lemurs (Eulemur rufifrons). Data were collected on four groups at Kirindy Forest, western Madagascar, during spontaneous actions and by confronting them with artificial feeding boxes. The lemurs did not exhibit manual lateralization on a group level in either condition. More individuals showed a hand preference in the experimental task, and the preferences were stronger compared to spontaneous actions. The direction of individual hand preferences was not consistent across the two conditions. The results of this study show that measuring manual laterality in different contexts can yield different results. Manual lateralization in wild redfronted lemurs therefore seems to be flexible and situation dependent and probably not ecologically relevant in their natural habitat.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Lemur/fisiología , Animales , Antropología Física , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Femenino , Fuerza de la Mano , Madagascar , Masculino
9.
Am J Biol Anthropol ; 185(3): e25019, 2024 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39222398

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The appearance of early lithic industries has been associated with the gradual development of unique biomechanical and cognitive abilities in hominins, including human-like precision grasping and basic learning and/or communicating capacities. These include tools used for activities exclusively associated with hominin contexts (cutting flakes) and hammerstones utilized for behaviors shared with non-human primates (e.g., nut-cracking). However, no previous experimental research has focused on comparing the factors affecting efficiency between these two key behavioral patterns and their evolutionary implications. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Here, we address this gap with an experimental design involving participants with varying tool-related experience levels (i.e., no experience, theoretical-only experience, and extensive practical knapping expertise) to monitor their success rates, biometrics, and surface electromyography (sEMG) recordings from eight important hand and forearm muscles. RESULTS: Our results showed that practical experience had a substantial impact on flake-cutting efficiency, allowing participants to achieve greater success rates with substantially less muscle effort. This relationship between success rates and muscle effort was not observed for the nut-cracking task. Moreover, even though practical experience did not significantly benefit nut-cracking success, experts exhibited increased rates of self-improvement in that task. DISCUSSION: Altogether, these experimental findings suggest that the ability to practice and retain tool-using knowledge played a fundamental role in the subsistence strategies and adaptability of early hominins, potentially providing the cognitive basis for conceptualizing the first intentional tool production strategies.


Asunto(s)
Hominidae , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Animales , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Mano/fisiología , Electromiografía , Adulto , Antropología Física , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología
10.
Appl Neuropsychol Child ; : 1-8, 2024 Apr 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38636449

RESUMEN

The aim was to assess validation and reliability of Arabic version of Children's Hand-use Experience Questionnaire (CHEQ) for children with hemiparetic cerebral palsy (HCP). Ninety-nine children aged 6-18 years diagnosed with HCP participated in the study. The CHEQ was used to evaluate the hand-use experiences of children with cerebral palsy (CP). In the expert opinion questionnaire, the average percentage score of agreement on grasp efficacy was 91.5%. The highest percentage (93.3%) was for time taken. Agreement on CHEQ ranged from 91.5 to 93.3% indicating that the content validity of the prototype is supported by the expert ratings. Fitting indices showed that the one-factor structure of the CHEQ has a good and acceptable fit in children with HCP aged 6-18 years. All factor loads of the CHEQ items were greater than 0.7 and significant. Cronbach's alpha coefficient in this study was 0.921, thus showing that the questionnaire had high internal consistency.

11.
Dev Psychobiol ; 55(8): 902-11, 2013 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23129422

RESUMEN

The variation in hand use as a function of task and developmental age poses a problem for understanding how and when "handedness," preferred use of one hand, develops. The present cross-section study is the first to contrast hand preference use for the natural and frequently used reach-to-eat movement with a constructional task that requires a very similar reach-to-grasp movement. Thirty children between the ages of 1 and 3 years completed an eating task, in which they grasped small food items (Cheerios™ or Froot Loops™) that they brought to the mouth for eating. Thirty children between the ages of 3 and 5 years completed the construction task, in which they grasped LEGO® pieces to construct 3D models. Hand use preference for grasping in the eating and construction tasks was calculated by comparing the percentage of grasps made by the right hand and by the left hand. There were two main findings: First, right hand preference for grasping in the eating task is present as early as 1 year of age, whereas right hand preference for grasping in the construction task does not develop until 4 years of age. Second, right hand preference for grasping is greater in the eating than in the construction task. The results are discussed in relation to the idea that a consideration for task constraints (e.g., unimanual vs. bimanual; eating vs. construction; natural vs. praxic) should be incorporated into the experimental design when measuring hand use in children.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino
12.
Disabil Rehabil ; 45(14): 2301-2306, 2023 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35722769

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To understand the role of affected upper extremity (UE) self-efficacy for explaining daily-hand-use in individuals post-stroke. Specifically, to describe UE self-efficacy and to assess the associations between UE self-efficacy to UE motor and functional capacity and daily hand-use. MATERIALS AND METHODS: This cross-sectional study included individuals post-stroke receiving rehabilitation with high UE functional capacity [Action Research Arm Test (ARAT) > 50]. The Rating of Everyday Arm-Use in the Community and Home (REACH) assessed perceived UE daily use and the Confidence in Arm and Hand Movement scale (CAHM) assessed UE self-efficacy. Functional capacity was assessed by the ARAT and the Fugl-Meyer-motor-assessment assessed motor impairment. Correlations between measures were assessed. RESULTS: Twenty-two individuals, aged 19-80, with high UE functional capacity [median (IQR) ARAT-56.5 (54-57)] and varying UE self-efficacy [median (IQR) CAHM-76.7 (58-84.4)], were included. UE self-efficacy was significantly correlated with the ARAT (rs = 0.53, p < 0.01) and REACH (rs = 0.51, p < 0.01) but ARAT was not significantly correlated with REACH. CONCLUSIONS: UE self-efficacy is correlated with perceived daily hand-use in individuals with high functional capacity. Further research and a deeper understanding of the clinical implications of UE self-efficacy are warranted. UE self-efficacy should perhaps be assessed during rehabilitation. IMPLICATIONS FOR REHABILITATIONIndividuals with stroke with high affected upper extremity functional capacity do not necessarily use this hand for daily living.Upper extremity self-efficacy is correlated with perceived daily hand-use in individuals with high functional capacity; participants with higher upper extremity self-efficacy also reported more daily hand-use.Upper extremity self-efficacy seems to be upper extremity task or situation-specific.Upper extremity self-efficacy should be assessed during rehabilitation and the clinical implications of (low) upper extremity self-efficacy should be further researched.


Asunto(s)
Rehabilitación de Accidente Cerebrovascular , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Humanos , Estudios Transversales , Autoeficacia , Recuperación de la Función , Extremidad Superior
13.
Am J Biol Anthropol ; 178(4): 667-677, 2022 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36790685

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: How a species uses its anatomical manipulators is determined by its anatomy, physiology, and ecology. While ecology explains interspecific variation in gripping, grasping, and manipulating objects, its role in intraspecific variation in mouth- and hand-use by animals is less explored. Primates are distinguished by their prehensile capabilities and manual dexterity. In context to the adaptive pressures of urbanization on primates, we examined if mouth and hand use differed across the forest-urban gradient in food retrieval and processing under experimental and naturalistic conditions in cercopithecids, a family comprising several urbanizing primates. MATERIALS AND METHODS: We recorded the acquisition and processing of peanuts under experimental conditions in three groups of bonnet macaques (BM, Macaca radiata) differing in their dietary dependence on packaged food items along a rural-urban gradient. To affirm the pattern obtained in the experiment, we coded food acquisition of three cercopithecid species in similar habitats from video sources. RESULTS: Urban macaques had a disproportionately higher hand use to acquire and process peanuts while rural macaques had higher mouth use. Based on analyses of videos, urban populations of BM, Japanese macaque (M. fuscata) and vervet monkey (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) showed a bias toward hand use during food acquisition. DISCUSSION: The adaptive pressures of urbanization, like the manual constraints of extracting packaged foods and perhaps, the need for visual-haptic exploration of novel objects seem to accentuate hand use in synanthropic groups of primates. Additional research should ascertain similar patterns in other primates and determine specific aspects of urbanization that modulate the observed trend.


Asunto(s)
Primates , Urbanización , Animales , Chlorocebus aethiops , Mano , Macaca , Macaca radiata/fisiología , Macaca fuscata
14.
Front Psychol ; 13: 712252, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35726268

RESUMEN

Background: Infants at risk for developmental disabilities often show signs of motor delay. Reaching is a skill that can help us identify atypical motor trajectories in early infancy. Researchers have studied performance after onset of reaching, but none have followed infants at risk from pre-reaching to skilled reaching. Aims: We assessed differences in reaching outcomes and hand use as reaching skill emerged in infants at risk for developmental disabilities and with typical development. Methods and Procedures: We followed infants at risk for developmental disabilities (n = 11) and infants with typical development (n = 21) longitudinally as they developed reaching skill. Infants reached for a toy at midline while sitting in the caregiver's lap. Video data were coded for reach outcome (miss, touch, partial grasp, and whole-hand grasp) and hand use (right, left, and bilateral). Outcomes and Results: Infants at risk had a larger proportion of missed reaches across visits compared to infants with typical development. Infants at risk also showed less variability in hand use when grasping over the study period. Conclusion and Implications: Our results provide information to support early differences in reaching performance to inform identification of typical and atypical developmental trajectories. Future studies should assess how the missed reaches are different and consider other quantitative measures of movement variability in infants at risk.

15.
J Clin Med ; 10(6)2021 Mar 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33807014

RESUMEN

Use of the paretic arm and hand is a key indicator of recovery and reintegration after stroke. A sound methodology is essential to comprehensively identify the possible factors impacting daily arm/hand use behavior. We combined ecological momentary assessment (EMA), a prompt methodology capturing real-time psycho-contextual factors, with accelerometry to investigate arm/hand behavior in the natural environment. Our aims were to determine (1) feasibility and (2) measurement validity of the combined methodology. We monitored 30 right-dominant, mild-moderately motor impaired chronic stroke survivors over 5 days (6 EMA prompts/day with accelerometers on each wrist). We observed high adherence for accelerometer wearing time (80.3%), EMA prompt response (84.6%), and generally positive user feedback upon exit interview. The customized prompt schedule and the self-triggered prompt option may have improved adherence. There was no evidence of EMA response bias nor immediate measurement reactivity. An unexpected small but significant increase in paretic arm/hand use was observed over days (12-14 min), which may be the accumulated effect of prompting that provided a reminder to choose the paretic limb. Further research that uses this combined methodology is needed to develop targeted interventions that effectively change behavior and enable reintegration post-stroke.

16.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 15: 667509, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34366809

RESUMEN

The ultimate goal of any upper-limb neurorehabilitation procedure is to improve upper-limb functioning in daily life. While clinic-based assessments provide an assessment of what a patient can do, they do not completely reflect what a patient does in his/her daily life. The use of compensatory strategies such as the use of the less affected upper-limb or excessive use of trunk in daily life is a common behavioral pattern seen in patients with hemiparesis. To this end, there has been an increasing interest in the use of wearable sensors to objectively assess upper-limb functioning. This paper presents a framework for assessing upper-limb functioning using sensors by providing: (a) a set of definitions of important constructs associated with upper-limb functioning; (b) different visualization methods for evaluating upper-limb functioning; and (c) two new measures for quantifying how much an upper-limb is used and the relative bias in their use. The demonstration of some of these components is presented using data collected from inertial measurement units from a previous study. The proposed framework can help guide the future technical and clinical work in this area to realize valid, objective, and robust tools for assessing upper-limb functioning. This will in turn drive the refinement and standardization of the assessment of upper-limb functioning.

17.
Behav Brain Res ; 412: 113404, 2021 08 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34139203

RESUMEN

Although the mouse (Mus musculus) is preyed upon by many other species of animals, it is also a predator and will hunt and consume crickets. There has been no previous description of how mice learn to hunt and no report on the extent to which they use their hands and mouth to assist prey capture and these were the objectives of the present study. Mice given one cricket each day displayed decreasing hunt times over 25-days for three phases of a hunt: investigate, in which a mouse explored and periodically encounter a cricket and often bit at it; pursue, in which a mouse's approach remained focused on the cricket until it was captured; and consume, in which the cricket was handled, decapitated, its core eaten, with its shell discarded. Although visual and auditory cues may contribute to locating a cricket, the vibrissae appeared to provide guidance in pursuit and capture when the cricket and mouse were proximate. Cricket capture involved extensive collaborative use of the mouth and the hands and mice could initiate capture with either the mouth or hands. Handling to eat involved manipulating the cricket into a head-up, ventrum-in position for decapitation and selective eating of the core of the cricket. The results are discussed in relation to mouse learning of a complex natural behavior, the use of tactile cues in the species-specific behavior of predation, and the contributions of the hands and mouth to predation.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Pie/fisiología , Miembro Anterior/fisiología , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Boca/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Vibrisas/fisiología
18.
Scand J Occup Ther ; 27(5): 385-393, 2020 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30734615

RESUMEN

Background: Many bimanual activities are challenging for children with unilateral spastic cerebral palsy (USCP).Aims: To investigate hand use by children with USCP in daily activities of their interest.Material e methods: Sample included twenty children with USCP, aged 8 to 14 years old. Participants identified bimanual activities of their interest and no interest. Children's performance on these activities were videotaped. Videos were coded according to the affected extremity's forms and effectiveness of use, caregiver provision and types of assistance, and child's satisfaction. The relative proportions of each parameter were compared using the signed-rank test, considering the time spent in activities of interest and no interest.Results: We analyzed 116 activities. We found effects of interest in children's satisfaction (p = 0.003) and on the type of assistance (p = 0.03). Specifically, children demonstrated longer periods of satisfaction performing activities of their interest and caregiver physical assistance was provided for longer periods of time in activities of no interest. There were no effects on the other parameters.Conclusions: A model that illustrates the impact of a child's interest on the performance of daily bimanual activities is presented, grounded in our results and in the literature.


Asunto(s)
Actividades Cotidianas/psicología , Cuidadores/psicología , Parálisis Cerebral/psicología , Parálisis Cerebral/rehabilitación , Niños con Discapacidad/psicología , Niños con Discapacidad/rehabilitación , Manipulaciones Musculoesqueléticas/métodos , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Behav Brain Res ; 381: 112241, 2020 03 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31655097

RESUMEN

There is debate over whether single-handed eating movements, reaching for food and withdrawing the hand to place the food in the mouth, originated in the primate lineage or whether they originated in phylogenetically-earlier Euarchontoglires. Most spontaneous hand use in eating by the laboratory mouse (Mus domestica) involves both hands, and a central question is the extent to which the movements are symmetric. Here we describe an asymmetry of spontaneous single hand use by the head-fixed mouse in making the yo-yo hand movement of removing and replacing a piece of pasta (spaghetti) in the mouth for eating. We also describe the problem/solution of placing into the mouth the end of a held item that protrudes at some distance from the hand. Pasta-eating proceeds in bouts, and a bout starts with raising the hands, which are holding a piece of pasta, to place one end of the pasta in the mouth for biting. A bout ends with lowering the hands, still holding the pasta stem, while the pasta morsel that has been bitten off is chewed. Hand-lowering after the pasta is removed from the mouth is slow, concurrent and symmetric, both when the pasta is held by both hands and when it is held in one hand. Hand-raising to place the pasta in the mouth is fast, consecutive and asymmetric, both when the pasta is held in both hands and when it is held in one hand. Frame-by-frame analyses of the video record combined with kinematic analyses show that a preferred single hand not only directs one end of the pasta to the mouth but also readjusts the trajectory of the pasta if it misses the mouth. The specialized use of a single hand by the mouse, even when the hands are bilaterally engaged, and the corrective asymmetric movements with which one hand adjusts the pasta's trajectory with the other hand playing a supporting role, is discussed in relation to the idea that hand preference, specialization, and dexterity have somatosensory and preprimate origins.


Asunto(s)
Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Pie/fisiología , Miembro Anterior/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Ratones , Actividad Motora , Factores de Tiempo
20.
Behav Brain Res ; 393: 112732, 2020 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32505659

RESUMEN

Grooming in the mouse features hand licking and symmetric and asymmetric arm and hand "strokes" over the face and body to maintain pelage. Grooming is syntactically organized but the structure of individualized movements of the arm, hand, and tongue have not been examined. Here spontaneous and water-induced grooming was video recorded in free-moving and head-fixed mice and subject to frame-by-frame video inspection and kinematic analysis using Physics Tracker. All groom arm and hand movements had a structure similar to that described for reach-to-eat movements. The movement included the hand lifting from the floor to supinate with the digits flexing and closed to a collect position, an aim position directed to a groom target, an advance to the target during which the fingers extend and open and the hand pronates, a grasp of a target on the snout, nose, or vibrissae, and a withdraw to the mouth where licking occurs, or a return to the starting position. This structure was present in individual unilateral forelimb groom strokes, in bilateral symmetric, or asymmetric groom strokes, and comprised the individuated components of a sequence of groom movements. Reach-to-groom movements could feature an ulnar adduction that positions the ulnar portion of the hand including and the thumb across the eye and nose, a movement that aids Hardarian fluid spreading. It is proposed that the mouse thumb nail is an anatomical feature that minimizes damage to the eye or nose that might be incurred by a claw. This analysis of the reach-to-groom movement provides insights into the flexibility of hand use in adaptive behavior, the evolution of skilled reaching movements, the neural control of reaching movements and the presence of the thumb nail in the mouse.


Asunto(s)
Miembro Anterior/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Aseo Animal/fisiología , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Ratones
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA