Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 3.244
Filtrar
Más filtros

Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Trends Genet ; 40(8): 633-635, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38880722

RESUMEN

Speciation is familiar in radiations, but personality is not. In a recent article, Sommer-Trembo et al. linked exploratory behavior in African cichlids to a SNP in the promoter of a gene, the homolog of which is associated with human personality disorders, offering clues about the first fish of this radiation, with implications for vertebrate evolution.


Asunto(s)
Cíclidos , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Animales , Cíclidos/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple/genética , Personalidad/genética , Conducta Animal , Humanos , Genética Conductual , África
2.
Cell ; 171(6): 1241-1245, 2017 11 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29195068
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(30): e2122154119, 2022 07 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35858398

RESUMEN

The question of the heritability of behavior has been of long fascination to scientists and the broader public. It is now widely accepted that most behavioral variation has a genetic component, although the degree of genetic influence differs widely across behaviors. Starting with Mendel's remarkable discovery of "inheritance factors," it has become increasingly clear that specific genetic variants that influence behavior can be identified. This goal is not without its challenges: Unlike pea morphology, most natural behavioral variation has a complex genetic architecture. However, we can now apply powerful genome-wide approaches to connect variation in DNA to variation in behavior as well as analyses of behaviorally related variation in brain gene expression, which together have provided insights into both the genetic mechanisms underlying behavior and the dynamic relationship between genes and behavior, respectively, in a wide range of species and for a diversity of behaviors. Here, we focus on two systems to illustrate both of these approaches: the genetic basis of burrowing in deer mice and transcriptomic analyses of division of labor in honey bees. Finally, we discuss the troubled relationship between the field of behavioral genetics and eugenics, which reminds us that we must be cautious about how we discuss and contextualize the connections between genes and behavior, especially in humans.


Asunto(s)
Abejas , Genética Conductual , Pisum sativum , Animales , Abejas/genética , Genómica , Herencia , Humanos , Patrón de Herencia , Ratones , Pisum sativum/genética
4.
Neurogenetics ; 24(1): 29-41, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36449109

RESUMEN

Feather pecking (FP) is a damaging nonaggressive behavior in laying hens with a heritable component. Its occurrence has been linked to the immune system, the circadian clock, and foraging behavior. Furthermore, dysregulation of miRNA biogenesis, disturbance of the gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABAergic) system, as well as neurodevelopmental deficiencies are currently under debate as factors influencing the propensity for FP behavior. Past studies, which focused on the dissection of the genetic factors involved in FP, relied on single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and short insertions and deletions < 50 bp (InDels). These variant classes only represent a certain fraction of the genetic variation of an organism. Hence, we reanalyzed whole-genome sequencing data from two experimental populations, which have been divergently selected for FP behavior for over more than 15 generations, performed variant calling for structural variants (SVs) as well as tandem repeats (TRs), and jointly analyzed the data with SNPs and InDels. Genotype imputation and subsequent genome-wide association studies, in combination with expression quantitative trait loci analysis, led to the discovery of multiple variants influencing the GABAergic system. These include a significantly associated TR downstream of the GABA receptor subunit beta-3 (GABRB3) gene, two microRNAs targeting several GABA receptor genes, and dystrophin (DMD), a direct regulator of GABA receptor clustering. Furthermore, we found the transcription factor ETV1 to be associated with the differential expression of 23 genes, which points toward a role of ETV1, together with SMAD4 and KLF14, in the disturbed neurodevelopment of high-feather pecking chickens.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Pollos , Animales , Femenino , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Pollos/genética , Plumas , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Genotipo , Genética Conductual/métodos
5.
Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet ; 21: 437-463, 2020 08 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32283949

RESUMEN

Nervous systems allow animals to acutely respond and behaviorally adapt to changes and recurring patterns in their environment at multiple timescales-from milliseconds to years. Behavior is further shaped at intergenerational timescales by genetic variation, drift, and selection. This sophistication and flexibility of behavior makes it challenging to measure behavior consistently in individual subjects and to compare it across individuals. In spite of these challenges, careful behavioral observations in nature and controlled measurements in the laboratory, combined with modern technologies and powerful genetic approaches, have led to important discoveries about the way genetic variation shapes behavior. A critical mass of genes whose variation is known to modulate behavior in nature is finally accumulating, allowing us to recognize emerging patterns. In this review, we first discuss genetic mapping approaches useful for studying behavior. We then survey how variation acts at different levels-in environmental sensation, in internal neuronal circuits, and outside the nervous system altogether-and then discuss the sources and types of molecular variation linked to behavior and the mechanisms that shape such variation. We end by discussing remaining questions in the field.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Molecular , Genética Conductual , Genética de Población , Polimorfismo Genético , Selección Genética , Humanos
6.
Behav Genet ; 53(2): 75-84, 2023 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36662387

RESUMEN

A century after the first twin and adoption studies of behavior in the 1920s, this review looks back on the journey and celebrates milestones in behavioral genetic research. After a whistle-stop tour of early quantitative genetic research and the parallel journey of molecular genetics, the travelogue focuses on the last fifty years. Just as quantitative genetic discoveries were beginning to slow down in the 1990s, molecular genetics made it possible to assess DNA variation directly. From a rocky start with candidate gene association research, by 2005 the technological advance of DNA microarrays enabled genome-wide association studies, which have successfully identified some of the DNA variants that contribute to the ubiquitous heritability of behavioral traits. The ability to aggregate the effects of thousands of DNA variants in polygenic scores has created a DNA revolution in the behavioral sciences by making it possible to use DNA to predict individual differences in behavior from early in life.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Fenotipo , Análisis de Secuencia por Matrices de Oligonucleótidos , Herencia Multifactorial/genética
7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e197, 2023 09 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37694914

RESUMEN

When building causal knowledge in behavioural genetics, the natural randomisation of genotypes at conception (approximately analogous to the artificial randomisation occurring in randomised controlled trials) facilitates the discovery of genetic causes. More importantly, the randomisation of genetic material within families also enables a better identification of (environmental) risk factors and aetiological pathways to diseases and behaviours.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Genotipo , Humanos , Distribución Aleatoria
8.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e190, 2023 09 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37694926

RESUMEN

Behavioral genetics typically finds that the so-called shared environment contributes little or nothing to explaining within-population variation on most traits. If true, this has important implications for where not to look for good targets of interventions: Namely all things that are within the normal range of variation from one rearing environment to the next in that population.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Medio Social , Humanos
9.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e208, 2023 09 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37694995

RESUMEN

In her target article, Burt revives a by now ancient debate on nature and nurture, and the ways to measure, disentangle, and ultimately trust one or the other of these forces. Unfortunately, she largely dismisses recent advances in behavior genetics and its huge potential in contributing to a better prediction and understanding of complex traits in social sciences.


Asunto(s)
Frutas , Genética Conductual , Humanos , Femenino , Ciencias Sociales
10.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e202, 2023 09 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37694905

RESUMEN

Behavior genetics often emphasizes methods over the underlying quality of the psychological information to which the methods are applied. A core aspect of this quality is the demographic diversity of the samples. Building causal genetic models based only on European-ancestry samples compromises their generalizability. Behavior genetics researchers must spend additional time and resources diversifying their samples before emphasizing causation.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Humanos , Genética Conductual/tendencias , Demografía , Grupos de Población
11.
Behav Brain Sci ; 46: e205, 2023 09 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37694933

RESUMEN

The methodological shift from twin studies to genome-wide association studies (GWASs) diminished estimates of true genetic causation underlying statistical heritability of behavioral differences. The sum total of causal genetic influence on behavior is not zero, but, (a) no one cited in the target article ever thought this was the case, and (b) there is still little known about concrete instances of genetic causation.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos , Causalidad
12.
Annu Rev Psychol ; 72: 37-60, 2021 01 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32898465

RESUMEN

Behavior genetics studies how genetic differences among people contribute to differences in their psychology and behavior. Here, I describe how the conclusions and methods of behavior genetics have evolved in the postgenomic era in which the human genome can be directly measured. First, I revisit the first law of behavioral genetics stating that everything is heritable, and I describe results from large-scale meta-analyses of twin data and new methods for estimating heritability using measured DNA. Second, I describe new methods in statistical genetics, including genome-wide association studies and polygenic score analyses. Third, I describe the next generation of work on gene × environment interaction, with a particular focus on how genetic influences vary across sociopolitical contexts and exogenous environments. Genomic technology has ushered in a golden age of new tools to address enduring questions about how genes and environments combine to create unique human lives.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Interacción Gen-Ambiente , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos , Herencia Multifactorial , Estudios en Gemelos como Asunto
13.
Twin Res Hum Genet ; 25(6): 211-225, 2022 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36734056

RESUMEN

The University of Minnesota has played an important role in the resurgence and eventual mainstreaming of human behavioral genetics in psychology and psychiatry. We describe this history in the context of three major movements in behavioral genetics: (1) radical eugenics in the early 20th century, (2) resurgence of human behavioral genetics in the 1960s, largely using twin and adoption designs to obtain more precise estimates of genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in behavior; and (3) use of measured genotypes to understand behavior. University of Minnesota scientists made significant contributions especially in (2) and (3) in the domains of cognitive ability, drug abuse and mental health, and endophenotypes. These contributions are illustrated through a historical perspective of major figures and events in behavioral genetics.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Psiquiatría , Humanos , Historia del Siglo XX , Genética Conductual/historia , Eugenesia/historia , Cognición , Gemelos/genética , Psiquiatría/historia
14.
Cult Med Psychiatry ; 46(1): 59-75, 2022 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34448120

RESUMEN

Using the large datasets available with new gene sequencing and biobank projects, behavioral geneticists are developing tools that attempt to predict individual intelligence based on genetics. These predictive tools are meant to enable a 'precision education' that will transform society. These technological developments have not changed the fundamental aims of a program with a long history. Behavioral genetics is continuous with previous attempts to match personal characteristics to heredity, such as sociobiology and evolutionary psychology, and threatens racial and other forms of bias. From these older paradigms, it inherits an understanding of intelligence as informational processing shaped by mechanistic and computational metaphors as well as a view of society and education organized around competition. Because of these influences, these models misdescribe fundamental aspects of human engagement with the world and disregard other concepts of intelligence, which creates problems for the precision education that researchers hope to construct using genetic knowledge.


Asunto(s)
Eugenesia , Genética Conductual , Investigación Genética , Humanos , Inteligencia/genética , Pruebas de Inteligencia
15.
Int J Mol Sci ; 23(22)2022 Nov 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36430544

RESUMEN

The Trace Amine-Associated Receptor 1 (TAAR1) is one of the six functional receptors belonging to the family of monoamine-related G protein-coupled receptors (TAAR1-TAAR9) found in humans. However, the exact biological mechanisms of TAAR1 central and peripheral action remain to be fully understood. TAAR1 is widely expressed in the prefrontal cortex and several limbic regions, interplaying with the dopamine system to modulate the reward circuitry. Recent clinical trials suggest the efficacy of TAAR1 agonists as potential novel antipsychotic agents. Here, we characterize behavioral and neurochemical phenotypes of TAAR1 knockout mice, focusing on aggression and self-grooming behavior that both strongly depend on the monoaminergic signaling and cortico-striatal and cortico-limbic circuits. Overall, we report increased aggression in these knockout mice in the resident-intruder test, accompanied by reduced self-grooming behavior in the novelty-induced grooming test, and by higher cortical serotonin (5-HT) tissue levels. Further studies are necessary to explore whether TAAR1-based therapies can become potential novel treatments for a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders associated with aggression.


Asunto(s)
Genética Conductual , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G , Serotonina , Animales , Ratones , Agresión/fisiología , Aseo Animal/fisiología , Ratones Noqueados , Corteza Prefrontal/metabolismo , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/genética , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/metabolismo , Serotonina/metabolismo
16.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e161, 2022 09 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36098445

RESUMEN

As emphasized in early cultural evolutionary theory, understanding heritability of human traits - especially, behavioural traits - is difficult. The target article describes important ways that culture can enhance, or obscure, signatures of heritability in genetic studies. Here, we discuss the utility of calculating heritability for behavioural traits influenced by cultural evolution and point to conceptual and technical complications to consider in future models.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Cultural , Genética Conductual , Humanos
17.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e182, 2022 09 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36098400

RESUMEN

The 29 commentaries amplified our key arguments; offered extensions, implications, and applications of the framework; and pushed back and clarified. To help forge the path forward for cultural evolutionary behavioral genetics, we (1) focus on conceptual disagreements and misconceptions about the concepts of heritability and culture; (2) further discuss points raised about the intertwined relationship between culture and genes; and (3) address extensions to the proposed framework, particularly as it relates to cultural clusters, development, and power. These commentaries, and the deep engagement they represent, reinforce the importance of integrating cultural evolution and behavioral genetics.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Cultural , Evolución Biológica , Genética Conductual , Humanos
18.
New Dir Child Adolesc Dev ; 2022(183-184): 47-55, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36162231

RESUMEN

Gene-environment processes tell us how genetic predispositions and environments work together to influence children in schools. One type of gene-environment process that has been extensively studied using behavioral genetics methods is a gene-by-environment interaction. A gene-by-environment interaction shows us when the effect of your context on a phenotype differs depending on your genetic predispositions, or vice versa, when the effect of your genetic predispositions on a phenotype differs depending on your context. Developmental behavioral geneticists interested in children's school achievement have examined many different contexts within the gene-by-environment interaction model, including contexts measured from within children's home and school environments. However, this work has been overwhelmingly focused on WEIRD samples children, leaving us with non-inclusive scientific evidence. This can lead to detrimental outcomes when we overgeneralize this non-inclusive scientific evidence to racialized groups. We conclude with a call to include racialized children in more research samples.


Asunto(s)
Éxito Académico , Genética Conductual , Humanos , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Instituciones Académicas
19.
Stud Hist Philos Sci ; 93: 183-191, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35533541

RESUMEN

The so-called 'missing heritability problem' is often characterized by behavior geneticists as a numerical discrepancy between alternative kinds of heritability. For example, while 'traditional heritability' derived from twin and family studies indicates that approximately ∼50% of variation in intelligence is attributable to genetics, 'SNP heritability' derived from genome-wide association studies indicates that only ∼10% of variation in intelligence is attributable to genetics. This 40% gap in variance accounted for by alternative kinds of heritability is frequently referred to as what's "missing." Philosophers have picked up on this reading, suggesting that "dissolving" the missing heritability problem is merely a matter of closing the numerical gap between traditional and molecular kinds of heritability. We argue that this framing of the problem undervalues the severity of the many challenges to scientific understanding of the "heritability" of human behavior. On our view, resolving the numerical discrepancies between alternative kinds of heritability will do little to advance scientific explanation and understanding of behavior genetics. Thus, we propose a new conceptual framework of the missing heritability problem that comprises three independent methodological and explanatory challenges: the numerical gap, the prediction gap, and the mechanism gap.


Asunto(s)
Conducta , Medicina de la Conducta , Genética Conductual , Patrón de Herencia , Conducta/fisiología , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Humanos , Patrón de Herencia/genética , Gemelos/genética
20.
J Neurogenet ; 35(3): 119-131, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34151727

RESUMEN

The pursuit of understanding behavior has led to investigations of how genes, the environment, and the nervous system all work together to produce and influence behavior, giving rise to a field of research known as behavioral neurogenetics. This review focuses on the research journeys of two pioneers of aspects of behavioral neurogenetic research: Dr. Marla Sokolowski and Dr. Catharine Rankin as examples of how different approaches have been used to understand relationships between genes and behavior. Marla Sokolowski's research is centered around the discovery and analysis of foraging, a gene responsible for the natural behavioral polymorphism of Drosophila melanogaster larvae foraging behavior. Catharine Rankin's work began with demonstrating the ability to learn in Caenorhabditis elegans and then setting out to investigate the mechanisms underlying the "simplest" form of learning, habituation. Using these simple invertebrate organisms both investigators were able to perform in-depth dissections of behavior at genetic and molecular levels. By exploring their research and highlighting their findings we present ways their work has furthered our understanding of behavior and contributed to the field of behavioral neurogenetics.


Asunto(s)
Caenorhabditis elegans/genética , Drosophila/genética , Genética Conductual/historia , Animales , Historia del Siglo XX , Historia del Siglo XXI , Aprendizaje/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA