Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 18 de 18
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(13): 7103-7107, 2020 03 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32179683

RESUMO

Honest reporting is essential for society to function well. However, people frequently lie when asked to provide information, such as misrepresenting their income to save money on taxes. A landmark finding published in PNAS [L. L. Shu, N. Mazar, F. Gino, D. Ariely, M. H. Bazerman, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 15197-15200 (2012)] provided evidence for a simple way of encouraging honest reporting: asking people to sign a veracity statement at the beginning instead of at the end of a self-report form. Since this finding was published, various government agencies have adopted this practice. However, in this project, we failed to replicate this result. Across five conceptual replications (n = 4,559) and one highly powered, preregistered, direct replication (n = 1,235) conducted with the authors of the original paper, we observed no effect of signing first on honest reporting. Given the policy applications of this result, it is important to update the scientific record regarding the veracity of these results.


Assuntos
Contratos , Enganação , Humanos
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(38): 15197-200, 2012 Sep 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22927408

RESUMO

Many written forms required by businesses and governments rely on honest reporting. Proof of honest intent is typically provided through signature at the end of, e.g., tax returns or insurance policy forms. Still, people sometimes cheat to advance their financial self-interests-at great costs to society. We test an easy-to-implement method to discourage dishonesty: signing at the beginning rather than at the end of a self-report, thereby reversing the order of the current practice. Using laboratory and field experiments, we find that signing before-rather than after-the opportunity to cheat makes ethics salient when they are needed most and significantly reduces dishonesty.


Assuntos
Ética , Fraude , Adulto , Comportamento , Comércio , Enganação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivação , Política Pública , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
3.
Harv Bus Rev ; 92(7-8): 116-9, 131, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25065255

RESUMO

We'd like to think that no smart, upstanding manager would ever overlook or turn a blind eye to threats or wrongdoing that ultimately imperil his or her business. Yet it happens all the time. We fall prey to obstacles that obscure or drown out important signals that things are amiss. Becoming a "first-class noticer," says Max H. Bazerman, a professor at Harvard Business School, requires conscious effort to fight ambiguity, motivated blindness, conflicts of interest, the slippery slope, and efforts of others to mislead us. As a manager, you can develop your noticing skills by acknowledging responsibility when things go wrong rather than blaming external forces beyond your control. Bazerman also advises taking an outsider's view to challenge the status quo. Given the string of ethical failures of corporations around the world in recent years--from BP to GM to JP Morgan Chase--it's clear that leaders not only need to act more responsibly themselves but also must develop keen noticing skills in their employees and across their organizations.


Assuntos
Ética nos Negócios , Gestão de Recursos Humanos/métodos , Estados Unidos
4.
Harv Bus Rev ; 89(4): 58-65, 137, 2011 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21510519

RESUMO

Companies are spending a great deal of time and money to install codes of ethics, ethics training, compliance programs, and in-house watchdogs. If these efforts worked, the money would be well spent. But unethical behavior appears to be on the rise. The authors observe that even the best-intentioned executives may be unaware of their own or their employees' unethical behavior. Drawing from extensive research on cognitive biases, they offer five reasons for this blindness and suggest what to do about them. Ill-conceived goals may actually encourage negative behavior. Brainstorm unintended consequences when devising your targets. Motivated blindness makes us overlook unethical behavior when remaining ignorant would benefit us. Root out conflicts of interest. Indirect blindness softens our assessment of unethical behavior when it's carried out by third parties. Take ownership of the implications when you outsource work. The slippery slope mutes our awareness when unethical behavior develops gradually. Be alert for even trivial infractions and investigate them immediately. Overvaluing outcomes may lead us to give a pass to unethical behavior. Examine good outcomes to ensure they're not driven by unethical tactics.


Assuntos
Pessoal Administrativo/ética , Comércio/ética , Humanos , Estados Unidos
5.
Harv Bus Rev ; 85(9): 72-6, 78, 148, 2007 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17886485

RESUMO

Negotiators often fail to achieve results because they channel too much effort into selling their own position and too little into understanding the other party's perspective. To get the best deal -or, sometimes, any deal at al--egotiators need to think like detectives, digging for information about why the other side wants what it does. This investigative approach entails a mind-set and a methodology, say Harvard Business School professors Malhotra and Bazerman. Inaccurate assumptions about the other side's motivations can lead negotiators to propose solutions to the wrong problems, needlessly give away value, or derail deals altogether. Consider, for example, the pharmaceutical company that deadlocked with a supplier over the issue of exclusivity in an ingredient purchase. Believing it was a ploy to raise the price, the drugmaker upped its offer--unsuccessfully. In fact, the supplier was balking because a relative's company needed a small amount of the ingredient to make a local product. Once the real motivation surfaced, a compromise quickly followed. Understanding the other side's motives and goals is the first principle of investigative negotiation. The second is to figure out what constraints the other party faces. Often when your counterpart's behavior appears unreasonable, his hands are tied somehow, and you can reach agreement by helping overcome those limitations. The third is to view onerous demands as a window into what the other party prizes most--and use that information to create opportunities. The fourth is to look for common ground; even fierce competitors may have complementary interests that lead to creative agreements. Finally, if a deal appears lost, stay at the table and keep trying to learn more. Even if you don't win, you can gain insights into a customer's future needs, the interests of similar customers, or the strategies of competitors.


Assuntos
Negociação/métodos , Pessoal Administrativo , Humanos , Estados Unidos
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 91(5): 857-71, 2006 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17059306

RESUMO

Individuals working in groups often egocentrically believe they have contributed more of the total work than is logically possible. Actively considering others' contributions effectively reduces these egocentric assessments, but this research suggests that undoing egocentric biases in groups may have some unexpected costs. Four experiments demonstrate that members who contributed much to the group outcome are actually less satisfied and less interested in future collaborations after considering others' contributions compared with those who contributed little. This was especially true in cooperative groups. Egocentric biases in responsibility allocation can create conflict, but this research suggests that undoing these biases can have some unfortunate consequences. Some members who look beyond their own perspective may not like what they see.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Ética , Processos Grupais , Satisfação Pessoal , Altruísmo , Autoria , Comportamento Competitivo/ética , Comportamento Competitivo/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Tomada de Decisões/ética , Ego , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Julgamento/ética , Julgamento/fisiologia , Manuscritos como Assunto , Grupo Associado , Percepção/ética , Percepção/fisiologia , Preconceito , Comportamento Social , Estudantes/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários
7.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 91(5): 872-89, 2006 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17059307

RESUMO

Group members often reason egocentrically, believing that they deserve more than their fair share of group resources. Leading people to consider other members' thoughts and perspectives can reduce these egocentric (self-centered) judgments such that people claim that it is fair for them to take less; however, the consideration of others' thoughts and perspectives actually increases egoistic (selfish) behavior such that people actually take more of available resources. A series of experiments demonstrates this pattern in competitive contexts in which considering others' perspectives activates egoistic theories of their likely behavior, leading people to counter by behaving more egoistically themselves. This reactive egoism is attenuated in cooperative contexts. Discussion focuses on the implications of reactive egoism in social interaction and on strategies for alleviating its potentially deleterious effects.


Assuntos
Altruísmo , Ética , Relações Interpessoais , Comportamento Social , Análise de Variância , Comportamento Competitivo/ética , Comportamento Competitivo/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Tomada de Decisões/ética , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Ego , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Julgamento/ética , Julgamento/fisiologia , Negociação/psicologia , Percepção/ética , Percepção/fisiologia , Estudantes/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários
8.
Harv Bus Rev ; 84(1): 88-97, 133, 2006 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16447372

RESUMO

By the time Merck withdrew its pain relief drug Vioxx from the market in 2004, more than 100 million prescriptions had been filled in the United States alone. Yet researchers now estimate that Vioxx may have been associated with as many as 25,000 heart attacks and strokes. Evidence of the drug's risks was available as early as 2000, so why did so many doctors keep prescribing it? The answer, say the authors, involves the phenomenon of bounded awareness--when cognitive blinders prevent a person from seeing, seeking, using, or sharing highly relevant, easily accessible, and readily perceivable information during the decision-making process. Doctors prescribing Vioxx, for instance, more often than not received positive feedback from patients. So, despite having access to information about the risks, physicians may have been blinded to the actual extent of the risks. Bounded awareness can occur at three points in the decision-making process. First, executives may fail to see or seek out the important information needed to make a sound decision. Second, they may fail to use the information that they do see because they aren't aware of its relevance. Third, executives may fail to share information with others, thereby bounding the organization's awareness. Drawing on examples such as the Challenger disaster and Citibank's failures in Japan, this article examines what prevents executives from seeing what's right in front of them and offers advice on how to increase awareness. Of course, not every decision requires executives to consciously broaden their focus. Collecting too much information for every decision would waste time and other valuable resources. The key is being mindful. If executives think an error could generate almost irrecoverable damage, then they should insist on getting all the information they need to make a wise decision.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Tomada de Decisões , Comércio/organização & administração , Humanos , Estados Unidos
9.
J Appl Psychol ; 87(1): 87-95, 2002 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11916219

RESUMO

This study investigated whether cognitions and behavior in an asymmetric social dilemma can be predicted by national culture. Results indicated that, as predicted, groups of decision makers from Japan--a collectivist, hierarchical culture-were more cooperative, expected others to be more cooperative, and were more likely to adopt an equal allocation distribution rule to resolve the dilemma than were groups of decision makers from the United States-an individualist, egalitarian culture. An opportunity for communication had a greater impact on expectations of others' behavior in groups of U.S. decision makers than in groups of Japanese decision makers.


Assuntos
Cognição , Tomada de Decisões , Condições Sociais , Adulto , Comunicação , Características Culturais , Feminino , Humanos , Japão/etnologia , Liderança , Masculino , Estados Unidos/etnologia
10.
Harv Bus Rev ; 81(3): 72-80, 140, 2003 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12632806

RESUMO

Think hard about the problems in your organization or about potential upheavals in the markets in which you operate. Could some of those problems--ones no one is attending to--turn into disasters? If you're like most executives, you'll sheepishly answer yes. As Harvard Business School professors Michael Watkins and Max Bazerman illustrate in this timely article, most of the "unexpected" events that buffet companies should have been anticipated--they're "predictable surprises." Such disasters take many forms, from financial scandals to disruptions in operations, from organizational upheavals to product failures. Some result in short-term losses or distractions, while others cause damage that takes years to repair. Some are truly catastrophic--the events of September 11, 2001, are a tragic example of a predictable surprise. The bad news is that all companies, including your own, are vulnerable to predictable surprises. The good news is that recent research helps explain why that's so and what companies can do to minimize their risk. The authors contend that organizations' inability to prepare for predictable surprises can be traced to three sets of vulnerabilities: psychological, organizational, and political. To address these vulnerabilities, the authors recommend the RPM approach. More than just the usual environmental scanning and contingency planning, RPM requires a chain of actions--recognizing, prioritizing, and mobilizing--that companies must meticulously adhere to. Failure to apply any one of these steps, the authors say, can leave an organization vulnerable. Given the extraordinarily high stakes involved, it should be every business leader's core responsibility to apply the RPM approach, the authors conclude.


Assuntos
Comércio/economia , Tomada de Decisões Gerenciais , Planejamento em Desastres/organização & administração , Liderança , Comércio/tendências , Administração Financeira , Humanos , Cultura Organizacional , Inovação Organizacional , Estados Unidos
11.
Harv Bus Rev ; 80(11): 96-102, 134, 2002 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12422793

RESUMO

On July 30, President Bush signed into law the Sarbanes-Oxley Act addressing corporate accountability. A response to recent financial scandals, the law tightened federal controls over the accounting industry and imposed tough new criminal penalties for fraud. The president proclaimed, "The era of low standards and false profits is over." If only it were that easy. The authors don't think corruption is the main cause of bad audits. Rather, they claim, the problem is unconscious bias. Without knowing it, we all tend to discount facts that contradict the conclusions we want to reach, and we uncritically embrace evidence that supports our positions. Accountants might seem immune to such distortions because they work with seemingly hard numbers and clear-cut standards. But the corporate-auditing arena is particularly fertile ground for self-serving biases. Because of the often subjective nature of accounting and the close relationships between accounting firms and their corporate clients, even the most honest and meticulous of auditors can unintentionally massage the numbers in ways that mask a company's true financial status, thereby misleading investors, regulators, and even management. Solving this problem will require far more aggressive action than the U.S. government has taken thus far. What's needed are practices and regulations that recognize the existence of bias and moderate its effects. True auditor independence will entail fundamental changes to the way the accounting industry operates, including full divestiture of consulting and tax services, rotation of auditing firms, and fixed-term contracts that prohibit client companies from firing their auditors. Less tangibly, auditors must come to appreciate the profound impact of self-serving biases on their judgment.


Assuntos
Contabilidade/normas , Comércio/economia , Auditoria Financeira/normas , Contabilidade/legislação & jurisprudência , Comércio/legislação & jurisprudência , Auditoria Financeira/legislação & jurisprudência , Fraude , Julgamento , Preconceito , Responsabilidade Social , Estados Unidos
12.
Harv Bus Rev ; 81(12): 56-64, 125, 2003 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14712544

RESUMO

Answer true or false: "I am an ethical manager." If you answered "true," here's an Uncomfortable fact: You're probably wrong. Most of us believe we can objectively size up a job candidate or a venture deal and reach a fair and rational conclusion that's in our, and our organization's, best interests. But more than two decades of psychological research indicates that most of us harbor unconscious biases that are often at odds with our consciously held beliefs. The flawed judgments arising from these biases are ethically problematic and undermine managers' fundamental work--to recruit and retain superior talent, boost individual and team performance, and collaborate effectively with partners. This article explores four related sources of unintentional unethical decision making. If you're surprised that a female colleague has poor people skills, you are displaying implicit bias--judging according to unconscious stereotypes rather than merit. Companies that give bonuses to employees who recommend their friends for open positions are encouraging ingroup bias--favoring people in their own circles. If you think you're better than the average worker in your company (and who doesn't?), you may be displaying the common tendency to overclaim credit. And although many conflicts of interest are overt, many more are subtle. Who knows, for instance, whether the promise of quick and certain payment figures into an attorney's recommendation to settle a winnable case rather than go to trial? How can you counter these biases if they're unconscious? Traditional ethics training is not enough. But by gathering better data, ridding the work environment of stereotypical cues, and broadening your mind-set when you make decisions, you can go a long way toward bringing your unconscious biases to light and submitting them to your conscious will.


Assuntos
Pessoal Administrativo/ética , Comércio/organização & administração , Tomada de Decisões/ética , Pessoal Administrativo/psicologia , Comércio/ética , Conflito de Interesses , Cultura , Coleta de Dados , Processos Grupais , Humanos , Gestão de Recursos Humanos/normas , Preconceito , Estados Unidos
13.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 37(3): 330-49, 2011 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21307176

RESUMO

People routinely engage in dishonest acts without feeling guilty about their behavior. When and why does this occur? Across four studies, people justified their dishonest deeds through moral disengagement and exhibited motivated forgetting of information that might otherwise limit their dishonesty. Using hypothetical scenarios (Studies 1 and 2) and real tasks involving the opportunity to cheat (Studies 3 and 4), the authors find that one's own dishonest behavior increased moral disengagement and motivated forgetting of moral rules. Such changes did not occur in the case of honest behavior or consideration of the dishonest behavior of others. In addition, increasing moral saliency by having participants read or sign an honor code significantly reduced unethical behavior and prevented subsequent moral disengagement. Although dishonest behavior motivated moral leniency and led to forgetting of moral rules, honest behavior motivated moral stringency and diligent recollection of moral rules.


Assuntos
Atitude , Consciência , Enganação , Princípios Morais , Análise de Variância , Tomada de Decisões , Feminino , Fraude , Humanos , Masculino , Motivação , Comportamento Social , Adulto Jovem
14.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 5(2): 209-12, 2010 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26162128

RESUMO

Bennis, Medin, and Bartels (2010, this issue) have contributed an interesting article on the comparative benefit of moral rules versus cost-benefit analysis (CBA). Many of their specific comments are accurate, useful, and insightful. At the same time, we believe they have misrepresented CBA and have reached a set of conclusions that are misguided and, if adopted wholesale, potentially dangerous. Overall, they offer wise suggestions for making CBA more effective, rather than eliminating CBA as a decision-making tool.

15.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 4(4): 379-83, 2009 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26158985

RESUMO

The optimal moment to address the question of how to improve human decision making has arrived. Thanks to 50 years of research by judgment and decision-making scholars, psychologists have developed a detailed picture of the ways in which human judgment is bounded. This article argues that the time has come to focus attention on the search for strategies that will improve bounded judgment because decision-making errors are costly and are growing more costly, decision makers are receptive, and academic insights are sure to follow from research on improvement. In addition to calling for research on improvement strategies, this article organizes the existing literature pertaining to improvement strategies and highlights promising directions for future research.

16.
J Am Diet Assoc ; 109(6): 1088-91, 2009 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19465193

RESUMO

Research during the last several decades indicates the failure of existing nutritional labels to substantially improve the healthfulness of consumers' food/beverage choices. The present study aims to fill this void by developing a nutrition metric that is more comprehensible to the average shopper. The healthfulness ratings of 205 sample foods/beverages by leading nutrition experts formed the basis for a linear regression that places weights on 12 nutritional components (ie, total fat, saturated fat, cholesterol, sodium, total carbohydrate, dietary fiber, sugars, protein, vitamin A, vitamin C, calcium, and iron) to predict the average healthfulness rating that experts would give to any food/beverage. Major benefits of the model include its basis in expert judgment, its straightforward application, the flexibility of transforming its output ratings to any linear scale, and its ease of interpretation. This metric serves the purpose of distilling expert knowledge into a form usable by consumers so that they are empowered to make more healthful decisions.


Assuntos
Prova Pericial , Alimentos/normas , Modelos Biológicos , Ciências da Nutrição , Valor Nutritivo , Humanos , Análise dos Mínimos Quadrados , Modelos Lineares
17.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 3(4): 324-38, 2008 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26158952

RESUMO

Although observers of human behavior have long been aware that people regularly struggle with internal conflict when deciding whether to behave responsibly or indulge in impulsivity, psychologists and economists did not begin to empirically investigate this type of want/should conflict until recently. In this article, we review and synthesize the latest research on want/should conflict, focusing our attention on the findings from an empirical literature on the topic that has blossomed over the last 15 years. We then turn to a discussion of how individuals and policy makers can use what has been learned about want/should conflict to help decision makers select far-sighted options.

18.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 1(2): 123-32, 2006 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26151467

RESUMO

We offer a psychological perspective to explain the failure of governments to create near-Pareto improvements. Our tools for analyzing these failures reflect the difficulties people have trading small losses for large gains: the fixed-pie approach to negotiations, the omission bias and status quo bias, parochialism and dysfunctional competition, and the neglect of secondary effects. We examine the role of human judgment in the failure to find wise trade-offs by discussing diverse applications of citizen and government decision making, including AIDS treatment, organ-donation systems, endangered-species protection, subsidies, and free trade. Our overall goal is to offer a psychological approach for understanding suboptimality in government decision making.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA