Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Am J Obstet Gynecol ; 211(1): 37.e1-9, 2014 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24495666

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Women who suffered preeclampsia and eclampsia may report subjective cognitive difficulties in daily life, the interpretation of which is cumbersome, because these are affected by emotional factors. Previous studies only included preeclamptic women investigated shortly after pregnancy. We aimed to determine whether these subjective reports of cognitive difficulty could be interpreted as reflecting objective cognitive dysfunction. Therefore, cognitive functioning was assessed using standardized neurocognitive tests in both preeclamptic and eclamptic women several years following the index pregnancy. STUDY DESIGN: Forty-six formerly eclamptic, 51 formerly preeclamptic, and 48 control women who had normotensive pregnancies, age-matched, participated in this study. Average elapsed time since index pregnancy was 7 years. Neurocognitive tests were divided into 6 domains; visual perception, motor functions, working memory, long-term memory, attention, and executive functioning. Subjective cognitive functioning was measured by the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire and anxiety/depression by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. RESULTS: Both preeclamptic and eclamptic women performed worse on the motor functions domain (P < .05), without differences on the other domains. They scored worse on the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (P < .01), the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale anxiety (P < .01), and depression (P < .05) subscales. CONCLUSION: Women who suffered eclampsia and/or preeclampsia demonstrate no objective cognitive impairment as compared with controls. Contrary to the well-structured test setting, both groups do report more cognitive failures, which are thought to reflect neurocognitive dysfunction in complex, stressful daily-life situations. Such report of cognitive failures may be compounded by anxiety and depression. Future studies should focus on the relationship of neurocognitive functioning with structural cerebral abnormalities.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Eclampsia/psicologia , Pré-Eclâmpsia/psicologia , Adulto , Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Ansiedade/etiologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Depressão/diagnóstico , Depressão/etiologia , Eclampsia/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Análise Multivariada , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Pré-Eclâmpsia/fisiopatologia , Gravidez , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Transtornos Psicomotores/etiologia , Autorrelato , Inquéritos e Questionários
2.
Neurocase ; 18(1): 39-49, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21707259

RESUMO

We describe a patient (JS) with impaired recognition and distorted visual perception of faces after an ischemic stroke. Strikingly, JS reports that the faces of family members look distorted, while faces of other people look normal. After neurological and neuropsychological examination, we assessed response accuracy, response times, and skin conductance responses on a face recognition task in which photographs of close family members, celebrities and unfamiliar people were presented. JS' performance was compared to the performance of three healthy control participants. Results indicate that three aspects of face perception appear to be impaired in JS. First, she has impaired recognition of basic emotional expressions. Second, JS has poor recognition of familiar faces in general, but recognition of close family members is disproportionally impaired compared to faces of celebrities. Third, JS perceives faces of family members as distorted. In this paper we consider whether these impairments can be interpreted in terms of previously described disorders of face perception and recent models for face perception.


Assuntos
Face , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/patologia
3.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 54(8): 710-5, 2012 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22630341

RESUMO

AIM: The aim of this study was to identify subgroups of children born moderately preterm (MPT) and term with distinctive levels and patterns of functioning, and the perinatal and demographic factors that predict subgroup membership. METHOD: A total of 378 children aged 7 years, 248 MPT (138 males, 110 females; gestational age 32-36 wks) and a comparison group of 130 children born at term (58 males, 72 females; gestational age 38-41 wks), were selected from a community-based cohort study. Latent class analyses were performed on measures of intelligence, verbal memory, attention, executive functioning, and visuomotor and motor skills. χ(2) automatic interaction detection analyses were performed to detect associations between the subgroups and predictors. RESULTS: Four subgroups differing in levels of performance were identified, with parental education being the only statistically significant determinant of subgroup assignment (p < 0.01). The subgroup that performed the most poorly showed an irregular pattern of performance, with specific weakness in attentional skill and relative strength in intelligence and verbal memory. Parental education predicted classification probability in the preterm group (p = 0.04) but not in the term group (p = 0.15). INTERPRETATION: Our results show that the poorer performance of children born MPT reflects a higher proportion of children with below average performance rather than a subgroup with extremely poor performance. They indicate that MPT birth affects neurodevelopmental functioning at early school age only slightly, with effects being largest in such children with low parental education.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Testes Genéticos , Recém-Nascido Prematuro , Criança , Feminino , Seguimentos , Testes Genéticos/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/fisiologia , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/psicologia , Masculino , Modelos Genéticos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Pais/educação
4.
Neurocase ; 17(4): 297-312, 2011 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20818541

RESUMO

In this study, we describe a 58-year-old male patient (FZ) with a right-amygdala lesion after temporal lobe infarction. FZ is unable to recognize fearful facial expressions. Instead, he consistently misinterprets expressions of fear for expressions of surprise. Employing EEG/ERP measures, we investigated whether presentation of fearful and surprised facial expressions would lead to different response patterns. We also measured ERPs to aversively conditioned and unconditioned fearful faces. We compared ERPs elicited by supraliminally and subliminally presented conditioned fearful faces (CS+), unconditioned fearful faces (CS-) and surprised faces. Despite FZ's inability to recognize fearful facial expressions in emotion recognition tasks, ERP components showed different response patterns to pictures of surprised and fearful facial expressions, indicating that covert or implicit recognition of fear is still intact. Differences between ERPs to CS+ and CS- were only found when these stimuli were presented subliminally. This indicates that intact right amygdala function is not necessary for aversive conditioning. Previous studies have stressed the importance of the right amygdala for discriminating facial emotional expressions and for classical conditioning. Our study suggests that the right amygdala is necessary for explicit recognition of fear, while implicit recognition of fear and classical conditioning may still occur following lesion of the right amygdala.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/patologia , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Estado de Consciência , Medo/fisiologia , Inconsciente Psicológico , Condicionamento Clássico , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
5.
Brain Cogn ; 76(2): 286-93, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21429649

RESUMO

This article presents an overview of our studies in elderly and Alzheimer patients employing Kimura's dichotic digits paradigm as a measure for left hemispheric predominance for processing language stimuli. In addition to structural brain mechanisms, we demonstrated that attention modulates the direction and degree of ear asymmetry in dichotic listening. Elderly showed increasingly more difficulties focusing attention on the left ear (LE) with advancing age. Alzheimer patients showed severe deficits to allocate attention to the LE, which could result in a right ear advantage. These results may be attributed to a breakdown of the cortical attentional network which is mediated by frontal (inhibitory control of attention) and parietal regions (spatial attention and 'disengagement processes'). Both interhemispheric disconnectivity (callosal atrophy) and intrahemispheric disconnectivity (subcortical white matter lesions) appear to be important factors contributing to these findings.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Humanos
6.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 50(8): 920-30, 2009 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19457048

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The quality of very preterm infants' spontaneous movements at 11 to 16 weeks post-term age is a powerful predictor of their later neurological status. This study investigated whether early spontaneous movements also have predictive value for the intellectual and behavioural problems that children born very preterm often experience. METHODS: Spontaneous movement quality was assessed, using Prechtl's method, at 11 to 16 weeks post-term in 65 infants born at

Assuntos
Dano Encefálico Crônico/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/diagnóstico , Doenças do Prematuro/diagnóstico , Inteligência , Deficiências da Aprendizagem/diagnóstico , Atividade Motora , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/diagnóstico , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/psicologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/psicologia , Displasia Broncopulmonar/diagnóstico , Displasia Broncopulmonar/psicologia , Hemorragia Cerebral/diagnóstico , Hemorragia Cerebral/psicologia , Ventrículos Cerebrais , Criança , Transtornos do Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Pré-Escolar , Diagnóstico Precoce , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Doenças do Prematuro/psicologia , Unidades de Terapia Intensiva Neonatal , Controle Interno-Externo , Deficiências da Aprendizagem/psicologia , Leucomalácia Periventricular/diagnóstico , Leucomalácia Periventricular/psicologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Programas de Rastreamento , Exame Neurológico , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Prognóstico , Respiração Artificial , Neuronite Vestibular
7.
Neuropsychology ; 22(6): 755-64, 2008 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18999349

RESUMO

Follow-up studies of preterm children without serious neurological complications have consistently found deficits in visuomotor skills. To determine whether these deficits may be related to impaired elementary visuomotor processes, we investigated movement programming and execution of simple pointing movements in 7- to 10-year-old preterm (<34 weeks g.a. and/or b.w. <1800 g) and full-term children. Such detailed analysis of simple pointing movements provides information on the extent to which processes associated with dorsal and/or cerebellar functions are impaired. Multi-level analysis showed that movement programming and execution were slowed in the 7-, 9-, and 10-year-old preterm groups. This indicates impaired dorsal visual stream functioning in preterm children, but do not rule out impaired cerebellar functioning. At 8 years of age, there were no differences between the two groups in movement execution time. This could have reflected a transition in the development of movement control in the control group, which has been associated in typically developing children with a decrease in motor speed. Interestingly, a similar decrease was not found in the preterm group at 8 years of age.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/psicologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/fisiologia , Masculino , Movimento/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
9.
Neuropsychologia ; 45(10): 2322-32, 2007 Jun 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17382359

RESUMO

Cortical 'disconnection', involving disruption of white matter tracts in the brain, has been hypothesized as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline. Diffuse hyperintensities in the white matter (so called white matter hyperintensities, WMH) on T2-weighted MRI scans are regarded to represent ischemic damage of the subcortical fiber system and are found to be increased with advanced age. In the present study, we examined whether WMH might be a mediating factor for age-effects in dichotic listening. For that purpose, we examined the association between age, deep or subcortical white matter hyperintensities (DWMH), periventricular hyperintensities (PVH) and dichotic listening performance in healthy older adults with ages ranging from 50 to 81 years. We found decreased dichotic listening performance with increasing age, specifically for the left ear (LE) but not the right ear (RE) in a standard unbiased-attention condition, and for the unattended ears but not the attended ears in conditions in which attention was controlled. Interestingly, hierarchical regression analyses revealed that the age-effect is partially mediated by DWMH specifically in the hemisphere contralateral to the attended ear. Moreover, we found indications that RE performance is specifically affected by DWMH in the right and left frontal and temporal areas, and that LE performance is mostly affected by DWMH selectively in the left hemisphere, including the left temporal area. The present study underlines the idea that disconnectivity is involved in age-related cognitive decline.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Testes com Listas de Dissílabos , Audição/fisiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Auscultação , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos
10.
Hum Mov Sci ; 26(6): 927-42, 2007 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17904673

RESUMO

We used a modified double-step pointing task to study movement adaptations in 7- to 10-year-old typically developing children. We found that the majority (63%) were able to optimally adapt fast, goal-directed visually-guided movements to a late change in target location meeting the requirements of speed and accuracy. A minority (35%) failed to meet the requirement of accuracy resulting in a less optimal adaptation. The results showed that the ability to adapt movements optimally develops before the age of 7 years in typically developing children. Literature proposes a transition in development of motor control around the age of 8 years. The present results replicate and extend this by suggesting that this transition affects the later phases of fast, goal-directed visually-guided movements rather than the early phases, such as movement programming and acceleration. Finally, the results indicate that the optimally adapted movements were the result of a specific strategy in which a specific component of movement execution was slowed on all trials. This suggests that 7- to 10-year-old typically developing children have developed implicit knowledge about which movement components are the most efficient to adapt.


Assuntos
Retroalimentação , Movimento , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
Eur J Cancer ; 42(16): 2765-72, 2006 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16935489

RESUMO

Intellectual functioning (verbal, performance and full-scale IQ) in 43 children treated for acute lymphoblastic leukaemia (ALL) with chemotherapy-only was evaluated in a nationwide, prospective, sibling-controlled study. Intellectual assessment was performed at diagnosis and repeated shortly after cessation of 2 years treatment, including intrathecal and systemic chemotherapy. Using hierarchical regression analysis, patients' and siblings' (n=27) scores were longitudinally analysed and compared to assess possible changes and differences over time. At both assessments, before and after treatment, the patients showed average scores on intelligence tests compared to population norms. Longitudinal analysis and cross-sectional comparisons revealed no significant differences between patients and controls. Young patients showed a small relative decline, albeit not significant, on performance-IQ compared to healthy siblings. Despite intensive and potentially neurotoxic treatment, no evident negative effects on intelligence were found. However, it cannot be precluded that younger patients are at risk for a small decline in PIQ.


Assuntos
Protocolos de Quimioterapia Combinada Antineoplásica/uso terapêutico , Inteligência , Leucemia-Linfoma Linfoblástico de Células Precursoras/tratamento farmacológico , Adolescente , Distribuição por Idade , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Estudos Transversais , Humanos , Leucemia-Linfoma Linfoblástico de Células Precursoras/psicologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Análise de Regressão
12.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 21(5): 383-94, 2006 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16876383

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to examine the relationship between executive functions (EFs), as measured by neuropsychological tests, and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and comorbid behavior, as rated by parents and teachers. As intelligence and language ability are important covariates they were also assessed. METHOD: The sample consisted of 43 children aged 7-11 years who were referred for neuropsychological assessment at a tertiary clinical facility. Most of the children had the diagnosis of ADHD combined or inattentive type. Different aspects of EFs were assessed. RESULTS: EFs were not significantly related to symptoms of ADHD, but only to comorbid symptoms of depression and autistic symptomatology. Language ability rather than EFs best predicted teacher ratings of inattention. CONCLUSIONS: The results of the study do not support the EF theory of ADHD. The importance of screening for comorbid language disorders in children referred for ADHD is emphasized.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/epidemiologia , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/fisiopatologia , Transtorno da Conduta/epidemiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Resolução de Problemas/fisiologia , Criança , Comorbidade , Transtorno da Conduta/fisiopatologia , Docentes , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Masculino , Pais/psicologia , Análise de Regressão , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia
13.
Res Dev Disabil ; 27(2): 217-26, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16005604

RESUMO

Central coherence theory hypothesizes individuals with autism process information in a detail-focused fashion. The present study examined whether adolescents with an intellectual disability and comorbid autism spectrum disorder showed a weaker central coherence than age- and IQ-matched controls. The central coherence skills of 43 adolescents from schools for students with severe learning problems were examined with two cognitive tasks. In these two tasks, detail-focused processing is beneficial to global processing to perform the tasks accurately and quickly. The group with autism spectrum disorder performed better than the control group. Adolescents with an intellectual disability and with comorbid autism spectrum disorder have a weaker central coherence than age- and IQ-matched controls. Partial support was also given for variability in weak central coherence within the autism spectrum.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/epidemiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Deficiência Intelectual/epidemiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Comorbidade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
14.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 38(5): 585-98, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26949992

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Hypertensive disorders of pregnancy, like preeclampsia, are a leading cause of maternal and fetal morbidity/mortality worldwide. Preeclampsia can be complicated by the occurrence of convulsions (eclampsia). Women who experienced (pre)eclampsia more frequently report daily cognitive failures and showed increased emotional dysfunction several years later, but are not impaired on objective neurocognitive testing. In addition, women with preterm preeclampsia more often have cerebral white matter lesions (WML) on follow-up. We aimed to determine whether WML presence is related to cognitive dysfunction, anxiety, and depressive symptoms in (pre)eclamptic women. METHOD: Forty-one eclamptic, 49 preeclamptic, and 47 control women who had a normotensive pregnancy completed the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (CFQ), the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), and a broad neurocognitive test battery (visual perception and speed of information processing, motor functions, working memory, long-term memory, attention, and executive functioning). All underwent cerebral magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and WML presence was recorded. Median elapsed time since index pregnancy was 6 years. Average age was 40 years. RESULTS: WML were more prevalent in women who had experienced preterm (pre)eclampsia (<37 weeks; 40%) than in controls (21%, p = .03). In (pre)eclamptic women, CFQ and HADS scores were higher than those in controls (44 ± 16.1 vs. 36 ± 11.0, p < .001, and 11 ± 6.3 vs. 8 ± 5.5, p < .001). There was no difference in objective cognitive performance as measured by neurocognitive tests. Subjective and objective cognitive functioning, anxiety, and depressive symptoms were not related to WML presence. CONCLUSION: Formerly (pre)eclamptic women report cognitive dysfunction, but do not exhibit overt cognitive impairment when objectively tested on average 6 years following their pregnancy. The presence of WML is not related to objective nor to subjective cognitive impairment, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. Longitudinal studies are needed to study whether the presence of WML is a risk factor for developing objective cognitive impairment in the long term.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/patologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Hipertensão/complicações , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Substância Branca/patologia , Adulto , Córtex Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico por imagem , Depressão , Eclampsia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Hipertensão/diagnóstico por imagem , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Pré-Eclâmpsia/fisiopatologia , Gravidez , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Estudos Retrospectivos , Estatísticas não Paramétricas , Inquéritos e Questionários , Substância Branca/diagnóstico por imagem , Substância Branca/fisiopatologia
15.
J Psychiatr Res ; 39(6): 585-93, 2005 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16157161

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Executive functioning reflects not only what a patient does, but also how he does it or whether he does it at all [Lezak MD. The problem of assessing executive functions. Int. J. Psychol. 17 (1982) 281]. Standard test procedures strongly prompt subjects to certain behavior, so that initiative and the amount of voluntary effort one is willing to invest are therefore not being adequately assessed. METHODS: We developed the Cognitive Effort Test (CET); a test for executive functioning specifically aimed at measuring subject's free initiatives, and the amount of effort they invest voluntarily. It is a complex planning task, and performance is being judged by three subscales: Initiative, Planning, and Workload. 36 schizophrenia patients and 30 healthy controls were tested with the CET, and a battery of other cognitive tests (executive functioning, memory, attention and psychomotor speed) was added to investigate construct and divergent validity. Negative symptoms were also recorded (predictive validity). RESULTS: Patients scored below controls on Planning and Workload, but not on Initiative. The CET was significantly related to other tests for cognition but not to negative symptoms. CET Planning and Workload predicted group membership (patients-controls) better than the other tests for executive functioning combined. CONCLUSION: The CET appears to be a clinically useful test that measures an aspect of schizophrenia that is not being assessed by existing tests, presumably the voluntarily allocation of effort.


Assuntos
Cognição , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicometria , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas
16.
Brain ; 127(Pt 8): 1845-52, 2004 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15240433

RESUMO

Sex differences in cognition are consistently reported, men excelling in most visuospatial tasks and women in certain verbal tasks. It has been hypothesized that these sex differences in cognition results from a more bilateral pattern of language representation in women than in men. This bilateral pattern of language representation in women is thought to interfere with visuospatial functions in the right hemisphere. To test whether language representation is indeed more bilateral in the female than in the male brain, a meta-analysis was performed on studies that assessed language activity with functional imaging in healthy men and women. Effect sizes were weighted for sample size and the meta-analytic method was applied to obtain a combined effect size. Fourteen studies were included, providing data on 377 men and 442 women. Meta-analysis yielded a mean weighted effect d of 0.21 with a 95% confidence interval of -0.05 to 0.48, indicating no significant difference in language lateralization between men and women. This implies that the putative sex difference in language lateralization may be absent at the population level, or may be observed only with some, as yet not defined, language tasks. It is therefore not likely that differences in language lateralization underlie the general sex differences in cognitive performance, and the neuronal basis for these cognitive sex differences remains elusive.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Idioma , Caracteres Sexuais , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Tomografia Computadorizada de Emissão
17.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 20(4): 443-56, 2005 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15896559

RESUMO

The objective of this study was to examine the impact of comorbid specific language impairment (SLI) on verbal and spatial working memory in children with DSM-IV combined subtype Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD-C). Participants were a clinical sample of 812- to 1212-year-old children diagnosed with ADHD-C. A group of ADHD-C with SLI was compared to a group of ADHD-C without SLI, and a group of normal children, matched on age and nonverbal intelligence. The results show that ADHD-C children with SLI scored significantly lower than those without SLI and normal children, on verbal working memory measures only. Both ADHD groups performed normally on spatial working memory measures. It is concluded that working memory deficits are not a specific characteristic of ADHD but are associated with language impairments. The importance of screening for language disorders in studies of neuropsychological functioning in children with ADHD is emphasized.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/complicações , Transtornos da Linguagem/complicações , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Adolescente , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/fisiopatologia , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos
18.
Schizophr Res ; 69(2-3): 267-76, 2004 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15469198

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Schizophrenia patients perform below the norm on verbal fluency tests. The causes for this are unknown, but defective memory, executive functioning and psychomotor speed may play a role. METHOD: We examined 50 patients with schizophrenia and related disorders, and 25 healthy controls with a cognitive test battery containing tests for verbal memory, executive functioning and psychomotor speed, and a categorical fluency test. RESULTS: Patients obtained significantly lower test results than the controls on most cognitive measures including the verbal fluency test. During the fluency test, they formed as many clusters, and switched as often between clusters as the controls did, but they generated fewer words per cluster. Interestingly, in the control group, fluency performance was predicted by memory and executive functioning, but not by psychomotor speed. In patients, verbal fluency was predicted by psychomotor speed, but not by memory or executive functioning. DISCUSSION: We conclude that psychomotor speed could be a crucial factor in cognition, and its influence on cognitive test performance should be considered in schizophrenia research. Furthermore, these data illustrate the importance of qualitative analysis of cognitive impairments in schizophrenia patients, as traditional cognitive tests often only provide quantitative information.


Assuntos
Testes de Linguagem , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Distúrbios da Fala/etiologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Adulto , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Resolução de Problemas/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Aprendizagem Verbal/fisiologia
19.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair ; 18(4): 212-21, 2004 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15537992

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to examine the effects of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) on cognition, behavior, and the rest-activity rhythm in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, combined type (ADHD-CT). METHODS: Twenty-two children diagnosed with ADHD-CT received TENS treatment during 6 weeks, 2 times 30 min a day. Neuropsychological tests were administered to assess cognition, parent/teacher behavioral rating scales were used to measure behavior, and actigraphy was used to assess the rest-activity rhythm. RESULTS: TENS appeared to have a moderate beneficial influence on cognitive functions that load particularly on executive function. There was also improvement in behavior as measured by parent/teacher behavioral rating scales. Moreover, motor restlessness during sleep and motor activity during the day decreased by TENS. CONCLUSIONS: The effects of TENS in children with ADHD are modest but encouraging and warrant further research.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/terapia , Comportamento Infantil , Cognição , Estimulação Elétrica Nervosa Transcutânea , Adolescente , Criança , Ritmo Circadiano , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Atividade Motora , Descanso
20.
Psychiatry ; 66(2): 133-45, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12868293

RESUMO

PREVIOUS studies comparing Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients with the normal elderly suggest that AD patients experience less pain. In the present study, pain reporting in 20 patients with possible vascular dementia (VaD) was compared to 20 nondemented elderly who had comparable pain conditions. It was hypothesized that, due to de-afferentiation, the possible VaD patients would experience more pain than the cognitively intact elderly. Pain assessment was conducted using three visual analogue scales, (1) the Coloured Analogue Scale (CAS) for Pain Intensity, (2) the CAS for Pain Affect, and (3) the Faces Pain Scale (FPS); a verbal pain questionnaire, Number of Words Chosen--Affective (NWC-A) of the McGill Pain Questionnaire; and an observation scale, the Checklist of Nonverbal Pain Indicators (CNPI). Results showed a significant increase in the scores on the CAS for Pain Affect and the FPS in the demented patients compared to the control group. There was a tendency for an increase in scores on the CNPI in the VaD group. These results suggest that patients with possible VaD suffer more pain than healthy elderly without cognitive impairment.


Assuntos
Demência Vascular/fisiopatologia , Dor/diagnóstico , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Cognição , Demência Vascular/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Medição da Dor/métodos , Inquéritos e Questionários
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA