Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 38
Filtrar
1.
Perception ; 52(4): 255-265, 2023 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36919274

RESUMO

Serial dependence in vision reflects how perceptual decisions can be biased by what we have recently perceived. Serial dependence studies test single items' effects on perceptual decisions. However, our visual world contains multiple objects at any given moment, so it's important to understand how past experiences affect not only a single object but also perception in a more general sense. Here we asked the question: What effect does a single item have when there is more than one subsequently presented test item? We displayed a single line (inducer) at the screen center, then either a single test-line or two simultaneous test-lines, varying in orientation space to the inducer. Next, participants reported test-line orientation using a left or right located response circle (to indicate which test-line should be reported). The results demonstrated that the inducer influenced subsequent perceptual judgments of a test-line even when two test-lines were presented. Distant items caused repulsive serial dependence, while close items caused attractive serial dependence. This shows how a single inducer can influence test-line judgments, even when two test-lines are presented, and can produce attractive and repulsive serial dependence biases when the item to report is revealed after it has disappeared.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Percepção Visual , Humanos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Viés
2.
J Vis ; 21(10): 3, 2021 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34468704

RESUMO

Visual perception is, at any given moment, strongly influenced by its temporal context-what stimuli have recently been perceived and in what surroundings. We have previously shown that to-be-ignored items produce a bias upon subsequent perceptual decisions that acts in parallel with other biases induced by attended items. However, our previous investigations were confined to biases upon the perceived orientation of a visual search target, and it is unclear whether these biases influence perceptual decisions in a more general sense. Here, we test whether the biases from visual search targets and distractors affect the perceived orientation of a neutral test line, one that is neither a target nor a distractor. To do so, we asked participants to search for an oddly oriented line among distractors and report its location for a few trials and next presented a test line irrelevant to the search task. Participants were asked to report the orientation of the test line. Our results indicate that in tasks involving visual search, targets induce a positive bias upon a neutral test line if their orientations are similar, whereas distractors produce an attractive bias for similar test lines and a repulsive bias if the orientations of the test line and the average orientation of the distractors are far apart in feature space. In sum, our results show that both attentional role and proximity in feature space between previous and current stimuli determine the direction of biases in perceptual decisions.


Assuntos
Visão Ocular , Percepção Visual , Atenção , Viés , Humanos
3.
J Vis ; 21(5): 21, 2021 05 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34010953

RESUMO

Although confidence is commonly believed to be an essential element in decision-making, it remains unclear what gives rise to one's sense of confidence. Recent Bayesian theories propose that confidence is computed, in part, from the degree of uncertainty in sensory evidence. Alternatively, observers can use physical properties of the stimulus as a heuristic to confidence. In the current study, we developed ideal observer models for either hypothesis and compared their predictions against human data obtained from psychophysical experiments. Participants reported the orientation of a stimulus, and their confidence in this estimate, under varying levels of internal and external noise. As predicted by the Bayesian model, we found a consistent link between confidence and behavioral variability for a given stimulus orientation. Confidence was higher when orientation estimates were more precise, for both internal and external sources of noise. However, we observed the inverse pattern when comparing between stimulus orientations: although observers gave more precise orientation estimates for cardinal orientations (a phenomenon known as the oblique effect), they were more confident about oblique orientations. We show that these results are well explained by a strategy to confidence that is based on the perceived amount of noise in the stimulus. Altogether, our results suggest that confidence is not always computed from the degree of uncertainty in one's perceptual evidence but can instead be based on visual cues that function as simple Heuristics to confidence.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Julgamento , Teorema de Bayes , Heurística , Humanos , Incerteza
4.
J Vis ; 20(8): 20, 2020 08 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32810275

RESUMO

Observers can learn complex statistical properties of visual ensembles, such as their probability distributions. Even though ensemble encoding is considered critical for peripheral vision, whether observers learn such distributions in the periphery has not been studied. Here, we used a visual search task to investigate how the shape of distractor distributions influences search performance and ensemble encoding in peripheral and central vision. Observers looked for an oddly oriented bar among distractors taken from either uniform or Gaussian orientation distributions with the same mean and range. The search arrays were either presented in the foveal or peripheral visual fields. The repetition and role reversal effects on search times revealed observers' internal model of distractor distributions. Our results showed that the shape of the distractor distribution influenced search times only in foveal, but not in peripheral search. However, role reversal effects revealed that the shape of the distractor distribution could be encoded peripherally depending on the interitem spacing in the search array. Our results suggest that, although peripheral vision might rely heavily on summary statistical representations of feature distributions, it can also encode information about the distributions themselves.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Orientação Espacial/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Distribuição Normal , Probabilidade , Campos Visuais/fisiologia
5.
J Vis ; 19(9): 2, 2019 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31369043

RESUMO

Objects have a variety of different features that can be represented as probability distributions. Recent findings show that in addition to mean and variance, the visual system can also encode the shape of feature distributions for features like color or orientation. In an odd-one-out search task we investigated observers' ability to encode two feature distributions simultaneously. Our stimuli were defined by two distinct features (color and orientation) while only one was relevant to the search task. We investigated whether the irrelevant feature distribution influences learning of the task-relevant distribution and whether observers also encode the irrelevant distribution. Although considerable learning of feature distributions occurred, especially for color, our results also suggest that adding a second irrelevant feature distribution negatively affected the encoding of the relevant one and that little learning of the irrelevant distribution occurred. There was also an asymmetry between the two different features: Searching for the oddly oriented target was more difficult than searching for the oddly colored target, which was reflected in worse learning of the color distribution. Overall, the results demonstrate that it is possible to encode information about two feature distributions simultaneously but also reveal considerable limits to this encoding.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Orientação Espacial/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Atenção/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
6.
Cogn Emot ; 33(5): 1051-1058, 2019 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30205742

RESUMO

People hiss and swear when they make errors, frown and swear again when they encounter conflicting information. Such error- and conflict-related signs of negative affect are found even when there is no time pressure or external reward and the task itself is very simple. Previous studies, however, provide inconsistent evidence regarding the affective consequences of resolved conflicts, that is, conflicts that resulted in correct responses. We tested whether response accuracy in the Eriksen flanker task will moderate the effect of trial incongruence using affective priming to measure positive and negative affect. We found that responses to incongruent trials elicit positive affect irrespective of their accuracy. Errors, in turn, result in negative affect irrespective of trial congruence. The effects of conflicts and errors do not interact and affect different dimensions of affective priming. Conflicts change the speed of evaluative categorisation while errors are reflected in categorisation accuracy. We discuss the findings in light of the "reward value and prediction" model and the "affect as a feedback for predictions" framework and consider the possible mechanisms behind the divergent effects.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Sinais (Psicologia) , Recompensa , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e231, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30767804

RESUMO

We disagree with Rahnev & Denison (R&D) that optimality should be abandoned altogether. Rather, we argue that adopting a normative approach enables researchers to test hypotheses about the brain's computational goals, avoids just-so explanations, and offers insights into function that are simply inaccessible to the alternatives proposed by R&D.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Tomada de Decisões
8.
Psychol Sci ; 28(10): 1510-1517, 2017 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28862923

RESUMO

Colors are rarely uniform, yet little is known about how people represent color distributions. We introduce a new method for studying color ensembles based on intertrial learning in visual search. Participants looked for an oddly colored diamond among diamonds with colors taken from either uniform or Gaussian color distributions. On test trials, the targets had various distances in feature space from the mean of the preceding distractor color distribution. Targets on test trials therefore served as probes into probabilistic representations of distractor colors. Test-trial response times revealed a striking similarity between the physical distribution of colors and their internal representations. The results demonstrate that the visual system represents color ensembles in a more detailed way than previously thought, coding not only mean and variance but, most surprisingly, the actual shape (uniform or Gaussian) of the distribution of colors in the environment.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
J Vis ; 17(2): 21, 2017 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28245500

RESUMO

We recently demonstrated that observers are capable of encoding not only summary statistics, such as mean and variance of stimulus ensembles, but also the shape of the ensembles. Here, for the first time, we show the learning dynamics of this process, investigate the possible priors for the distribution shape, and demonstrate that observers are able to learn more complex distributions, such as bimodal ones. We used speeding and slowing of response times between trials (intertrial priming) in visual search for an oddly oriented line to assess internal models of distractor distributions. Experiment 1 demonstrates that two repetitions are sufficient for enabling learning of the shape of uniform distractor distributions. In Experiment 2, we compared Gaussian and uniform distractor distributions, finding that following only two repetitions Gaussian distributions are represented differently than uniform ones. Experiment 3 further showed that when distractor distributions are bimodal (with a 30° distance between two uniform intervals), observers initially treat them as uniform, and only with further repetitions do they begin to treat the distributions as bimodal. In sum, observers do not have strong initial priors for distribution shapes and quickly learn simple ones but have the ability to adjust their representations to more complex feature distributions as information accumulates with further repetitions of the same distractor distribution.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Distribuição Normal , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
10.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e143, 2017 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29342620

RESUMO

Hulleman & Olivers' (H&O's) proposal is a refreshing addition to the visual search literature. Although we like their proposal that fixations, not individual items should be considered a fundamental unit in visual search, we point out some unresolved problems that their account will have to solve. Additionally, we consider predictions that can be made from the account, in particular in relation to priming of visual search, finding that the account generates interesting testable predictions.

11.
Perception ; 45(8): 910-930, 2016 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27060181

RESUMO

In the well-known "dress" photograph, people either see the dress as blue with black stripes or as white with golden stripes. We suggest that the perception of colors is guided by the scene interpretation and the inferred positions of light sources. We tested this hypothesis in two online studies using color matching to estimate the colors observers see, while controlling for individual differences in gray point bias and color discrimination. Study 1 demonstrates that the interpretation of the dress corresponds to differences in perceived colors. Moreover, people who perceive the dress as blue-and-black are two times more likely to consider the light source as frontal, than those who see the white-and-gold dress. The inferred light sources, in turn, depend on the circadian changes in ambient light. The interpretation of the scene background as a wall or a mirror is consistent with the perceived colors as well. Study 2 shows that matching provides reliable results on differing devices and replicates the findings on scene interpretation and light sources. Additionally, we show that participants' environmental lighting conditions are an important cue for perceiving the dress colors. The exact mechanisms of how environmental lighting and circadian changes influence the perceived colors of the dress deserve further investigation.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Iluminação , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
12.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(3): 1086-99, 2016 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26170056

RESUMO

The Internet provides a convenient environment for data collection in psychology. Modern Web programming languages, such as JavaScript or Flash (ActionScript), facilitate complex experiments without the necessity of experimenter presence. Yet there is always a question of how much noise is added due to the differences between the setups used by participants and whether it is compensated for by increased ecological validity and larger sample sizes. This is especially a problem for experiments that measure response times (RTs), because they are more sensitive (and hence more susceptible to noise) than, for example, choices per se. We used a simple visual search task with different set sizes to compare laboratory performance with Web performance. The results suggest that although the locations (means) of RT distributions are different, other distribution parameters are not. Furthermore, the effect of experiment setting does not depend on set size, suggesting that task difficulty is not important in the choice of a data collection method. We also collected an additional online sample to investigate the effects of hardware and software diversity on the accuracy of RT data. We found that the high diversity of browsers, operating systems, and CPU performance may have a detrimental effect, though it can partly be compensated for by increased sample sizes and trial numbers. In sum, the findings show that Web-based experiments are an acceptable source of RT data, comparable to a common keyboard-based setup in the laboratory.


Assuntos
Coleta de Dados/métodos , Internet , Sistemas On-Line , Tempo de Reação , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Linguagens de Programação , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tamanho da Amostra , Software , Adulto Jovem
13.
Cogn Emot ; 29(6): 1091-106, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25319749

RESUMO

Even without explicit positive or negative reinforcement, experiences may influence preferences. According to the affective feedback in hypotheses testing account preferences are determined by the accuracy of hypotheses: correct hypotheses evoke positive affect, while incorrect ones evoke negative affect facilitating changes of hypotheses. Applying this to visual search, we suggest that accurate search should lead to more positive ratings of targets than distractors, while for errors targets should be rated more negatively. We test this in two experiments using time-limited search for a conjunction of gender and tint of faces. Accurate search led to more positive ratings for targets as compared to distractors or targets following errors. Errors led to more negative ratings for targets than for distractors. Critically, eye tracking revealed that the longer the fixation dwell times in target regions, the higher the target ratings for correct responses, and the lower the ratings for errors. The longer observers look at targets, the more positive their ratings if they answer correctly, and less positive, following errors. The findings support the affective feedback account and provide the first demonstration of negative effects on liking ratings following errors in visual search.


Assuntos
Afeto/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
14.
Cogn Emot ; 28(3): 385-415, 2014 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24044594

RESUMO

The present research aimed to assess the effect of recognition decision on subsequent affective evaluations of recognised and non-recognised objects. Consistent with the proposed account of post-decisional preferences, results showed that the effect of recognition on preferences depends upon objective familiarity. If stimuli are recognised, liking ratings are positively associated with exposure frequency; if stimuli are not recognised, this link is either absent (Experiment 1) or negative (Experiments 2 and 3). This interaction between familiarity and recognition exists even when recognition accuracy is at chance level and the "mere exposure" effect is absent. Finally, data obtained from repeated measurements of preferences and using manipulations of task order confirm that recognition decisions have a causal influence on preferences. The findings suggest that affective evaluation can provide fine-grained access to the efficacy of cognitive processing even in simple cognitive tasks.


Assuntos
Afeto , Tomada de Decisões , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Jovem
15.
Elife ; 122024 Aug 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39190585

RESUMO

Transcranial ultrasonic stimulation (TUS) is rapidly emerging as a promising non-invasive neuromodulation technique. TUS is already well-established in animal models, providing foundations to now optimize neuromodulatory efficacy for human applications. Across multiple studies, one promising protocol, pulsed at 1000 Hz, has consistently resulted in motor cortical inhibition in humans (Fomenko et al., 2020). At the same time, a parallel research line has highlighted the potentially confounding influence of peripheral auditory stimulation arising from TUS pulsing at audible frequencies. In this study, we disentangle direct neuromodulatory and indirect auditory contributions to motor inhibitory effects of TUS. To this end, we include tightly matched control conditions across four experiments, one preregistered, conducted independently at three institutions. We employed a combined transcranial ultrasonic and magnetic stimulation paradigm, where TMS-elicited motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) served as an index of corticospinal excitability. First, we replicated motor inhibitory effects of TUS but showed through both tight controls and manipulation of stimulation intensity, duration, and auditory masking conditions that this inhibition was driven by peripheral auditory stimulation, not direct neuromodulation. Furthermore, we consider neuromodulation beyond driving overall excitation/inhibition and show preliminary evidence of how TUS might interact with ongoing neural dynamics instead. Primarily, this study highlights the substantial shortcomings in accounting for the auditory confound in prior TUS-TMS work where only a flip-over sham and no active control was used. The field must critically reevaluate previous findings given the demonstrated impact of peripheral confounds. Furthermore, rigorous experimental design via (in)active control conditions is required to make substantiated claims in future TUS studies. Only when direct effects are disentangled from those driven by peripheral confounds can TUS fully realize its potential for research and clinical applications.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica , Potencial Evocado Motor , Córtex Motor , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Humanos , Adulto , Feminino , Masculino , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana/métodos , Potencial Evocado Motor/fisiologia , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Ondas Ultrassônicas
16.
Nat Commun ; 14(1): 7634, 2023 Nov 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37993430

RESUMO

Humans infer motion direction from noisy sensory signals. We hypothesize that to make these inferences more precise, the visual system computes motion direction not only from velocity but also spatial orientation signals - a 'streak' created by moving objects. We implement this hypothesis in a Bayesian model, which quantifies knowledge with probability distributions, and test its predictions using psychophysics and fMRI. Using a probabilistic pattern-based analysis, we decode probability distributions of motion direction from trial-by-trial activity in the human visual cortex. Corroborating the predictions, the decoded distributions have a bimodal shape, with peaks that predict the direction and magnitude of behavioral errors. Interestingly, we observe similar bimodality in the distribution of the observers' behavioral responses across trials. Together, these results suggest that observers use spatial orientation signals when estimating motion direction. More broadly, our findings indicate that the cortical representation of low-level visual features, such as motion direction, can reflect a combination of several qualitatively distinct signals.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Córtex Visual , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Teorema de Bayes , Probabilidade , Psicofísica , Córtex Visual/diagnóstico por imagem , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
17.
Vision Res ; 206: 108190, 2023 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36780808

RESUMO

It is well known that observers can use so-called summary statistics of visual ensembles to simplify perceptual processing. The assumption has been that instead of representing feature distributions in detail the visual system extracts the mean and variance of visual ensembles. But recent evidence from implicit testing using a method called feature distribution learning showed that far more detail of the distributions is retained than the summary statistic literature indicates. Observers also encode higher-order statistics such as the kurtosis of feature distributions of orientation and color. But this sort of learning has not been shown for more intricate aspects of visual information. Here we tested the learning of distractor ensembles for shape, using the feature distribution learning method. Using a linearized circular shape space, we found that learning of detailed distributions of shape does not occur for this shape space while observers were able to learn the mean and range of the distributions. Previous demonstrations of feature distribution learning involved simpler feature dimensions than the more complex shape space tested here, and our findings may therefore reveal important boundary conditions of feature distribution learning.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Percepção Visual , Humanos
18.
Front Neural Circuits ; 17: 1157228, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37123106

RESUMO

Introduction: How does gravity (or lack thereof) affect sensory-motor processing? We analyze sensorimotor estimation dynamics for line segments with varying direction (orientation) in a 7-day dry immersion (DI), a ground-based model of gravitational unloading. Methods: The measurements were carried out before the start of the DI, on the first, third, fifth and seventh days of the DI, and after its completion. At the memorization stage, the volunteers led the leading hand along the visible segment on a touchscreen display, and at the reproduction stage they repeated this movement on an empty screen. A control group followed the same procedure without DI. Results: Both in the DI and control groups, when memorizing, the overall error in estimating the lengths and directions of the segments was small and did not have pronounced dynamics; when reproducing, an oblique effect (higher variability of responses to oblique orientations compared to cardinal ones) was obtained. We then separated biases (systematic error) and uncertainty (random error) in subjects' responses. At the same time, two opposite trends were more pronounced in the DI group during the DI. On the one hand the cardinal bias (a repulsion of orientation estimates away from cardinal axes) and, to a small extent, the variability of direction estimates decreased. On the other hand, the overestimation bias in length estimates increased. Discussion: Such error pattern strongly supports the hypotheses of the vector encoding, in which the direction and length of the planned movement are encoded independently of each other when the DI disrupts primarily the movement length encoding.


Assuntos
Imersão , Orientação , Humanos , Orientação/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Sensação , Percepção
19.
Vision Res ; 188: 211-226, 2021 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34371249

RESUMO

The visual system is sensitive to statistical properties of complex scenes and can encode feature probability distributions in detail. But does the brain use these statistics to build probabilistic models of the ever-changing visual input? To investigate this, we examined how observers temporally integrate two different orientation distributions from sequentially presented visual search trials. If the encoded probabilistic information is used in a Bayesian optimal way, observers should weigh more reliable information more strongly, such as feature distributions with low variance. We therefore manipulated the variance of the two feature distributions. Participants performed sequential odd-one-out visual search for an oddly oriented line among distractors. During successive learning trials, the distractor orientations were sampled from two different Gaussian distributions on alternating trials. Then, observers performed a 'test trial' where the orientations of the target and distractors were switched, allowing us to assess observer's internal representation of distractor distributions based on changes in response times. In three experiments we observed that observer's search times on test trials depended mainly on the very last learning trial, indicating a strong recency effect. Since temporal integration has been previously observed with this method, we conclude that when the input is unreliable, the visual system relies more on the most recent stimulus. This indicates that the visual system prefers to utilize sensory history when the statistical properties of the environment are relatively stable.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Percepção Visual , Teorema de Bayes , Humanos , Probabilidade , Tempo de Reação
20.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 83(3): 1230-1239, 2021 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32333372

RESUMO

Humans have remarkable abilities to construct a stable visual world from continuously changing input. There is increasing evidence that momentary visual input blends with previous input to preserve perceptual continuity. Most studies have shown that such influences can be traced to characteristics of the attended object at a given moment. Little is known about the role of ignored stimuli in creating this continuity. This is important since while some input is selected for processing, other input must be actively ignored for efficient selection of the task-relevant stimuli. We asked whether attended targets and actively ignored distractor stimuli in an odd-one-out search task would bias observers' perception differently. Our observers searched for an oddly oriented line among distractors and were occasionally asked to report the orientation of the last visual search target they saw in an adjustment task. Our results show that at least two opposite biases from past stimuli influence current perception: A positive bias caused by serial dependence pulls perception of the target toward the previous target features, while a negative bias induced by the to-be-ignored distractor features pushes perception of the target away from the distractor distribution. Our results suggest that to-be-ignored items produce a perceptual bias that acts in parallel with other biases induced by attended items to optimize perception. Our results are the first to demonstrate how actively ignored information facilitates continuity in visual perception.


Assuntos
Orientação , Percepção Visual , Viés , Humanos , Orientação Espacial , Visão Ocular
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA