Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 129
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Dig Dis Sci ; 69(1): 216-227, 2024 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37955761

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Previous studies have indicated that red dichromatic imaging (RDI) improved the visibility of gastrointestinal bleeding. AIMS: To investigate the recognition of bleeding points during endoscopic submucosal dissection (ESD) under RDI compared with that under white light imaging (WLI). METHODS: Consecutive patients scheduled to undergo esophageal or gastric ESD at a single center were enrolled. Paired videos of active bleeding during ESD under WLI and RDI were created. Six endoscopists identified the virtual hemostasis point on still images after random video viewing. The distance between virtual hemostasis and actual bleeding points was scored in four levels (0-3 points), and the association with the color value was analyzed in both WLI and RDI. RESULTS: We evaluated 116 videos for 58 bleeding points. The median visibility score and recognition rate were significantly higher for RDI than for WLI (2.17 vs. 1.42, p < 0.001 and 62.1% vs 27.6%, p < 0.001). Additionally, the recognition rate of trainees in RDI was higher than that of experts in WLI (60.3% vs. 43.1%, p = 0.067). The median color difference of RDI was significantly higher than that of WLI (8.97 vs. 3.69, p < 0.001). Furthermore, the correlation coefficient between the visibility score and color difference was 0.712 (strong correlation). CONCLUSION: RDI can provide better recognition of bleeding points than WLI during ESD. Therefore, further studies are warranted to investigate whether RDI improves ESD outcomes.


Assuntos
Ressecção Endoscópica de Mucosa , Neoplasias Gástricas , Humanos , Ressecção Endoscópica de Mucosa/efeitos adversos , Ressecção Endoscópica de Mucosa/métodos , Hemorragia Gastrointestinal/diagnóstico por imagem , Hemorragia Gastrointestinal/etiologia , Hemorragia Gastrointestinal/cirurgia , Esôfago , Estômago , Neoplasias Gástricas/diagnóstico por imagem , Neoplasias Gástricas/cirurgia
2.
J Vis ; 24(5): 15, 2024 May 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38814934

RESUMO

Temporal asynchrony is a cue for the perceptual segregation of spatial regions. Past research found attribute invariance of this phenomenon such that asynchrony induces perceptual segmentation regardless of the changing attribute type, and it does so even when asynchrony occurs between different attributes. To test the generality of this finding and obtain insights into the underlying computational mechanism, we compared the segmentation performance for changes in luminance, color, motion direction, and their combinations. Our task was to detect the target quadrant in which a periodic alternation in attribute was phase-delayed compared to the remaining quadrants. When stimulus elements made a square-wave attribute change, target detection was not clearly attribute invariant, being more difficult for motion direction change than for luminance or color changes and nearly impossible for the combination of motion direction and luminance or color. We suspect that waveform mismatch might cause anomalous behavior of motion direction since a square-wave change in motion direction is a triangular-wave change in the spatial phase (i.e., a second-order change in the direction of the spatial phase change). In agreement with this idea, we found that the segregation performance was strongly affected by the waveform type (square wave, triangular wave, or their combination), and when this factor was controlled, the performance was nearly, though not perfectly, invariant against attribute type. The results were discussed with a model in which different visual attributes share a common asynchrony-based segmentation mechanism.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Espacial , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Adulto
3.
Hepatol Res ; 53(9): 844-856, 2023 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37237426

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Clinical trials enroll patients with active fibrotic nonalcoholic steatohepatitis (NASH) (nonalcoholic fatty liver disease [NAFLD] activity score ≥ 4) and significant fibrosis (F ≥ 2); however, screening failure rates are high following biopsy. We developed new scores to identify active fibrotic NASH using FibroScan and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). METHODS: We undertook prospective primary (n = 176), retrospective validation (n = 169), and University of California San Diego (UCSD; n = 234) studies of liver biopsy-proven NAFLD. Liver stiffness measurement (LSM) using FibroScan or magnetic resonance elastography (MRE), controlled attenuation parameter (CAP), or proton density fat fraction (PDFF), and aspartate aminotransferase (AST) were combined to develop a two-step strategy-FibroScan-based LSM followed by CAP with AST (F-CAST) and MRE-based LSM followed by PDFF with AST (M-PAST)-and compared with FibroScan-AST (FAST) and MRI-AST (MAST) for diagnosing active fibrotic NASH. Each model was categorized using rule-in and rule-out criteria. RESULTS: Areas under receiver operating characteristic curves (AUROCs) of F-CAST (0.826) and M-PAST (0.832) were significantly higher than those of FAST (0.744, p = 0.004) and MAST (0.710, p < 0.001). Following the rule-in criteria, positive predictive values of F-CAST (81.8%) and M-PAST (81.8%) were higher than those of FAST (73.5%) and MAST (70.0%). Following the rule-out criteria, negative predictive values of F-CAST (90.5%) and M-PAST (90.9%) were higher than those of FAST (84.0%) and MAST (73.9%). In the validation and UCSD cohorts, AUROCs did not differ significantly between F-CAST and FAST, but M-PAST had a higher diagnostic performance than MAST. CONCLUSIONS: The two-step strategy, especially M-PAST, showed reliability of rule-in/-out for active fibrotic NASH, with better predictive performance compared with MAST. This study is registered with ClinicalTrials.gov (number, UMIN000012757).

4.
Digestion ; 104(4): 270-282, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36649678

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: In patients with gastroesophageal reflux disease (GERD) on maintenance therapy with acid-suppressive drugs, it is not clear what background factors allow patients to discontinue the drugs. The aims of this study were to examine the relationship of the changes in the frequency and severity of gastrointestinal symptoms after discontinuation of acid-secretion inhibitors for erosive GERD (eGERD) with possible patient background factors and to identify factors that influence these changes. METHODS: This is a multicenter, open-label, interventional, exploratory study. eGERD patients with mild mucosal injury whose symptoms were under control and who were on maintenance therapy with acid-suppressive drugs were withdrawn from the drug treatment for 4 weeks. We examined the relationship of patient backgrounds (sex, age, body mass index, alcohol consumption, smoking habits), esophageal hiatal hernia, Helicobacter pylori infection, pepsinogen I and II concentrations and I/II ratios, blood gastrin levels before and after drug discontinuation with total score change in Frequency Scale for the Symptoms of GERD (FSSG). RESULTS: Of the 92 patients whose symptoms could be assessed before and after drug withdrawal, 66 patients (71.7% of the total) had FSSG <8 and no symptom relapse after the withdrawal. Furthermore, patient background factors that may be related to symptom relapse/non-relapse were examined, but no related factors were detected. The maintenance medications before discontinuation in the above 92 patients were a proton pump inhibitor (PPI) and vonoprazan (VPZ, a potassium ion competitive acid blocker). Since PPI and VPZ were administered to about the same number of patients, though incidentally, we additionally examined the relationship between patient background factors and symptom relapse/non-relapse by treatment group. As a result, no relevant background factors were detected in both groups. Although there were no significant differences between the two groups, the severity and frequency of symptom recurrence in the VPZ group tended to be higher than in the PPI group. CONCLUSIONS: Consideration of background factors is unlikely to be required in the discontinuation of maintenance therapy for eGERD. There was no significant difference in the extent of disease or frequency of recurrence during the discontinuation period, regardless of whether the drug before discontinuation was a PPI or VPZ.


Assuntos
Refluxo Gastroesofágico , Infecções por Helicobacter , Helicobacter pylori , Hérnia Hiatal , Humanos , Infecções por Helicobacter/tratamento farmacológico , Refluxo Gastroesofágico/tratamento farmacológico , Refluxo Gastroesofágico/diagnóstico , Inibidores da Bomba de Prótons/efeitos adversos , Resultado do Tratamento
5.
J Vis ; 23(12): 5, 2023 10 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37856108

RESUMO

To encode binocular disparity, the visual system uses a pair of left eye and right eye bandpass filters with either a position or a phase offset between them. Such pairs are considered to exit at multiple scales to encode a wide range of disparity. However, local disparity measurements by bandpass mechanisms can be ambiguous, particularly when the actual disparity is larger than a half-cycle of the preferred spatial frequency of the filter, which often occurs in fine scales. In this study, we investigated whether the visual system uses a coarse-to-fine interaction to resolve this ambiguity at finer scales for depth estimation from disparity. The stimuli were stereo grating patches composed of a target and comparison patterns. The target patterns contained spatial frequencies of 1 and 4 cycles per degree (cpd). The phase disparity of the low-frequency component was 0° (at the horopter), -90° (uncrossed), or 90° (crossed), and that of the high-frequency components was changed independent of the low-frequency disparity, in the range between -90° (uncrossed) and 90° (crossed). The observers' task was to indicate whether the target appeared closer to the comparison pattern, which always shared the disparity with the low-frequency component of the target. Regardless of whether the comparison pattern was a 1-cpd + 4-cpd compound or a 1-cpd simple grating, the perceived depth order of the target and the comparison varied in accordance with the phase disparity of the high-frequency component of the target. This effect occurred not only when the low-frequency component was at the horopter, but also when it contained a large disparity corresponding to one cycle of the high-frequency component (±90°). Our findings suggest a coarse-to-fine interaction in multiscale disparity processing, in which the depth interpretation of the high-frequency changes based on the disparity of the low-frequency component.


Assuntos
Percepção de Profundidade , Disparidade Visual , Humanos , Visão Binocular
6.
J Gastroenterol Hepatol ; 37(1): 97-103, 2022 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34478183

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND AIM: Comprehensive reports on the risk factors for bleeding and early death after percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy (PEG) are limited. In this multicenter study, we retrospectively investigated the risk factors for bleeding and early death after PEG. METHODS: Patients (n = 1234) who underwent PEG between 2015 and 2020 at Osaka Medical and Pharmaceutical University and its affiliated hospitals (11 institutions in total) were evaluated for postoperative bleeding and early death (within 60 days) after PEG according to patient characteristics, construction method, medical history, medications, preoperative hematological findings, and perioperative adverse events. Multivariate logistic regression was performed to identify independent predictors of bleeding and early death after PEG. RESULTS: The risk factors for bleeding after PEG were PEG tube insertion using the modified introducer method (odds ratio [OR], 4.37; P = 0.0003), low platelet count (OR, 0.99; P = 0.014), antiplatelet therapy (OR, 2.11; P = 0.036), and heparinization (OR, 4.50; P = 0.007). Risk factors for early death were low body mass index (BMI) (OR, 0.89; P = 0.015), low serum albumin levels (OR, 0.50; P = 0.035), and comorbidity of active cancer (OR, 4.03; P < 0.0001). There was no significant association between bleeding and early death after PEG. CONCLUSIONS: We identified several risk factors for bleeding and early death after PEG. Risk factors for bleeding were PEG tube insertion using the modified introducer method, low platelet count, antiplatelet therapy, and heparinization. Risk factors for early death were low BMI, low serum albumin levels, and comorbidity of active cancer.


Assuntos
Gastrostomia , Mortalidade Prematura , Hemorragia Pós-Operatória , Gastrostomia/efeitos adversos , Humanos , Neoplasias/epidemiologia , Inibidores da Agregação Plaquetária/efeitos adversos , Hemorragia Pós-Operatória/epidemiologia , Estudos Retrospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Albumina Sérica
7.
J Vis ; 22(10): 18, 2022 09 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36149676

RESUMO

Theories of visual confidence have largely been grounded in the gaussian signal detection framework. This framework is so dominant that idiosyncratic consequences from this distributional assumption have remained unappreciated. This article reports systematic comparisons of the gaussian signal detection framework to its logistic counterpart in the measurement of metacognitive accuracy. Because of the difference in their distribution kurtosis, these frameworks are found to provide different perspectives regarding the efficiency of confidence rating relative to objective decision (the logistic model intrinsically gives greater meta-d'/d' ratio than the gaussian model). These frameworks can also provide opposing conclusions regarding the metacognitive inefficiency along the internal evidence continuum (whether meta-d' is larger or smaller for higher levels of confidence). Previous theories developed on these lines of analysis may need to be revisited as the gaussian and logistic metacognitive models received somewhat equivalent support in our quantitative model comparisons. Despite these discrepancies, however, we found that across-condition or across-participant comparisons of metacognitive measures are relatively robust against the distributional assumptions, which provides much assurance to conventional research practice. We hope this article promotes the awareness for the significance of hidden modeling assumptions, contributing to the cumulative development of the relevant field.


Assuntos
Metacognição , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos
8.
J Vis ; 22(2): 17, 2022 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35195670

RESUMO

Complex visual processing involved in perceiving the object materials can be better elucidated by taking a variety of research approaches. Sharing stimulus and response data is an effective strategy to make the results of different studies directly comparable and can assist researchers with different backgrounds to jump into the field. Here, we constructed a database containing several sets of material images annotated with visual discrimination performance. We created the material images using physically based computer graphics techniques and conducted psychophysical experiments with them in both laboratory and crowdsourcing settings. The observer's task was to discriminate materials on one of six dimensions (gloss contrast, gloss distinctness of image, translucent vs. opaque, metal vs. plastic, metal vs. glass, and glossy vs. painted). The illumination consistency and object geometry were also varied. We used a nonverbal procedure (an oddity task) applicable for diverse use cases, such as cross-cultural, cross-species, clinical, or developmental studies. Results showed that the material discrimination depended on the illuminations and geometries and that the ability to discriminate the spatial consistency of specular highlights in glossiness perception showed larger individual differences than in other tasks. In addition, analysis of visual features showed that the parameters of higher order color texture statistics can partially, but not completely, explain task performance. The results obtained through crowdsourcing were highly correlated with those obtained in the laboratory, suggesting that our database can be used even when the experimental conditions are not strictly controlled in the laboratory. Several projects using our dataset are underway.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Sensibilidades de Contraste , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Propriedades de Superfície , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
9.
Endoscopy ; 53(7): 683-690, 2021 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33152774

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Endoscopic submucosal dissection (ESD) is a technically difficult and time-consuming procedure. We aimed to investigate the efficacy and safety of ESD using a multibending endoscope to treat superficial gastrointestinal neoplasms. METHODS: Patients with a single early gastric cancer who met the absolute or expanded indications for ESD according to the Japanese gastric cancer treatment guidelines were enrolled and randomly assigned to undergo ESD using a conventional endoscope (C-ESD) or a multibending endoscope (M-ESD). Randomization was stratified by ESD operator experience and tumor location. The primary outcome was ESD procedure time, calculated as the time from the start of submucosal injection to complete removal of the tumor. RESULTS: 60 patients were analyzed (30 C-ESD, 30 M-ESD). The mean (standard deviation [SD]) ESD procedure times for M-ESD and C-ESD were 34.6 (SD 17.2) and 47.2 (SD 26.7) minutes, respectively (P = 0.03). Muscle layer damage occurred significantly less frequently with M-ESD (0.2 [SD 0.7] vs. 0.7 [SD 1.0]; P = 0.04). There were no significant differences between the two techniques in procedure time or damage to muscle layers for tumors located in the lower third of the stomach. CONCLUSIONS: ESD procedure time was significantly shorter with the multibending endoscope and fewer muscles were damaged. We recommend multibending endoscopy for ESD in the upper and middle thirds of the stomach to reduce procedure time and incidence of complications.


Assuntos
Ressecção Endoscópica de Mucosa , Neoplasias Gástricas , Dissecação , Endoscópios , Ressecção Endoscópica de Mucosa/efeitos adversos , Mucosa Gástrica/cirurgia , Humanos , Estômago , Neoplasias Gástricas/cirurgia , Resultado do Tratamento
10.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 16(8): e1008018, 2020 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32813688

RESUMO

Visually inferring material properties is crucial for many tasks, yet poses significant computational challenges for biological vision. Liquids and gels are particularly challenging due to their extreme variability and complex behaviour. We reasoned that measuring and modelling viscosity perception is a useful case study for identifying general principles of complex visual inferences. In recent years, artificial Deep Neural Networks (DNNs) have yielded breakthroughs in challenging real-world vision tasks. However, to model human vision, the emphasis lies not on best possible performance, but on mimicking the specific pattern of successes and errors humans make. We trained a DNN to estimate the viscosity of liquids using 100.000 simulations depicting liquids with sixteen different viscosities interacting in ten different scenes (stirring, pouring, splashing, etc). We find that a shallow feedforward network trained for only 30 epochs predicts mean observer performance better than most individual observers. This is the first successful image-computable model of human viscosity perception. Further training improved accuracy, but predicted human perception less well. We analysed the network's features using representational similarity analysis (RSA) and a range of image descriptors (e.g. optic flow, colour saturation, GIST). This revealed clusters of units sensitive to specific classes of feature. We also find a distinct population of units that are poorly explained by hand-engineered features, but which are particularly important both for physical viscosity estimation, and for the specific pattern of human responses. The final layers represent many distinct stimulus characteristics-not just viscosity, which the network was trained on. Retraining the fully-connected layer with a reduced number of units achieves practically identical performance, but results in representations focused on viscosity, suggesting that network capacity is a crucial parameter determining whether artificial or biological neural networks use distributed vs. localized representations.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Redes Neurais de Computação , Viscosidade , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Biologia Computacional , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
11.
BMC Gastroenterol ; 21(1): 432, 2021 Nov 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34794376

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Although some kinds of endoluminal surgery for patients with proton pump inhibitor (PPI)-refractory gastroesophageal reflux disease (GERD) have been reported, there are few reports on their long-term outcomes. In 2014, we reported the effectiveness of endoscopic surgery for PPI-refractory GERD, which we invented and named endoscopic submucosal dissection for GERD (ESD-G) in 2008. Thereafter, we accumulated more cases and monitored the patients' condition postoperatively and describe the outcomes herein. PATIENTS AND METHODS: This single-center, single-arm trial was conducted at the Osaka Medical and Pharmaceutical University Hospital. We compared outcomes between before and 3-6 months after ESD-G. Additionally, we investigated the outcomes of patients 5 or more years after ESD-G. RESULTS: We performed 42 ESD-G procedures in 35 patients between 2008 and 2020. In seven patients, ESD-G was performed twice for various reasons. The frequency scale for the symptoms of GERD score was significantly improved 3-6 months after ESD-G (22 → 10, p < 0.0001); the Los Angeles classification for reflux esophagitis was clearly improved after ESD-G (p = 0.0423). The number of reflux episodes was not decreased by ESD-G. There was a significant difference in the potency unit of gastric acid secretion suppressants for controlling GERD-related symptoms between baseline and 3-6 months after ESD-G (p = 0.0009). In patients without a history of distal gastrectomy who underwent ESD-G, the potency unit of gastric acid secretion suppressants significantly decreased 5 or more years after ESD-G (p = 0.0121). CONCLUSION: ESD-G may be effective in patients with refractory GERD-related symptoms without a history of distal gastrectomy.


Assuntos
Ressecção Endoscópica de Mucosa , Esofagite Péptica , Refluxo Gastroesofágico , Endoscopia , Esofagite Péptica/tratamento farmacológico , Esofagite Péptica/etiologia , Refluxo Gastroesofágico/tratamento farmacológico , Refluxo Gastroesofágico/cirurgia , Humanos , Inibidores da Bomba de Prótons/uso terapêutico , Resultado do Tratamento
12.
BMC Gastroenterol ; 20(1): 396, 2020 Nov 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33228604

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: There are often specific endoscopic findings caused by deposition of lanthanum (La) in the gastric mucosa of patients taking lanthanum carbonate (LaC), a novel phosphate binder for patients on hemodialysis. We conducted a retrospective study to investigate the clinical significance of La deposition in the gastric mucosa, and the association between endoscopic features and histologic findings in the same population. METHODS: We compared background factors in patients taking LaC with and without La deposition in their gastroscopic biopsy specimen. We also investigated the relationship between gastric endoscopic biopsy specimens with La deposition and the concurrent endoscopic images. RESULTS: There was a significant difference in the total dose of LaC between the La-positive and La-negative groups (990 g [180-3150 g] vs. 480 g [225-1328 g]; p = 0.013). In 27 biopsy specimens with specific whitish mucosa, 10 showed mild histiocytic infiltration and 17 showed severe infiltration. In contrast, among 24 specimens with non-whitish mucosa, 5 showed no histiocytic infiltration, 10 showed mild infiltration, and 9 showed severe infiltration. There was a significant relationship between endoscopic features and the degree of histiocytic infiltration (p = 0.026). CONCLUSIONS: We demonstrated that La deposition in the gastric mucosa depended on the total dose of LaC and was not affected by background factors. The specific endoscopic features of La deposition are associated with the infiltration of histiocytes, which represents the body's normal response to foreign bodies. Trial registry The protocol was registered in the University Hospital Medical Information Network Clinical Trial Registry (UMIN000038929, https://upload.umin.ac.jp/cgi-open-bin/ctr/ctr_view.cgi?recptno=R000044393 ).


Assuntos
Mucosa Gástrica , Lantânio , Diálise Renal , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Gastroscopia , Humanos , Lantânio/efeitos adversos , Lantânio/farmacocinética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estudos Retrospectivos
13.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 14(4): e1006061, 2018 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29702644

RESUMO

Visual estimation of the material and shape of an object from a single image includes a hard ill-posed computational problem. However, in our daily life we feel we can estimate both reasonably well. The neural computation underlying this ability remains poorly understood. Here we propose that the human visual system uses different aspects of object images to separately estimate the contributions of the material and shape. Specifically, material perception relies mainly on the intensity gradient magnitude information, while shape perception relies mainly on the intensity gradient order information. A clue to this hypothesis was provided by the observation that luminance-histogram manipulation, which changes luminance gradient magnitudes but not the luminance-order map, effectively alters the material appearance but not the shape of an object. In agreement with this observation, we found that the simulated physical material changes do not significantly affect the intensity order information. A series of psychophysical experiments further indicate that human surface shape perception is robust against intensity manipulations provided they do not disturb the intensity order information. In addition, we show that the two types of gradient information can be utilized for the discrimination of albedo changes from highlights. These findings suggest that the visual system relies on these diagnostic image features to estimate physical properties in a distal world.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Biologia Computacional , Simulação por Computador , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Psicofísica , Propriedades de Superfície
14.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 112(33): E4620-7, 2015 Aug 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26240313

RESUMO

Human vision has a remarkable ability to perceive two layers at the same retinal locations, a transparent layer in front of a background surface. Critical image cues to perceptual transparency, studied extensively in the past, are changes in luminance or color that could be caused by light absorptions and reflections by the front layer, but such image changes may not be clearly visible when the front layer consists of a pure transparent material such as water. Our daily experiences with transparent materials of this kind suggest that an alternative potential cue of visual transparency is image deformations of a background pattern caused by light refraction. Although previous studies have indicated that these image deformations, at least static ones, play little role in perceptual transparency, here we show that dynamic image deformations of the background pattern, which could be produced by light refraction on a moving liquid's surface, can produce a vivid impression of a transparent liquid layer without the aid of any other visual cues as to the presence of a transparent layer. Furthermore, a transparent liquid layer perceptually emerges even from a randomly generated dynamic image deformation as long as it is similar to real liquid deformations in its spatiotemporal frequency profile. Our findings indicate that the brain can perceptually infer the presence of "invisible" transparent liquids by analyzing the spatiotemporal structure of dynamic image deformation, for which it uses a relatively simple computation that does not require high-level knowledge about the detailed physics of liquid deformation.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Simulação por Computador , Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção de Profundidade , Percepção de Forma , Humanos , Percepção de Movimento , Psicofísica , Refratometria , Software , Gravação em Vídeo , Visão Ocular , Água
15.
J Vis ; 18(8): 3, 2018 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30098175

RESUMO

Dynamic image deformation produces the perception of a transparent material that appears to deform the background image by light refraction. Since past studies on this phenomenon have mainly used subjective judgment about the presence of a transparent layer, it remains unsolved whether this is a real perceptual transparency effect in the sense that it forms surface representations, as do conventional transparency effects. Visual computation for color and luminance transparency, induced mainly by surface-contour information, can be decomposed into two components: surface formation to determine foreground and background layers, and scission to assign color and luminance to each layer. Here we show that deformation-induced perceptual transparency aids surface formation by color transparency and consequently resolves color scission. We asked observers to report the color of the front layer in a spatial region with a neutral physical color. The layer color could be seen as either reddish or greenish depending on the spatial context producing the color transparency, which was, however, ambiguous about the order of layers. We found that adding to the display a deformation-induced transparency that could specify the front layer significantly biased color scission in the predicted way if and only if the deformation-induced transparency was spatially coincident with the interpretation of color transparency. The results indicate that deformation-induced transparency is indeed a novel type of perceptual transparency that plays a role in surface formation in cooperation with color transparency.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Distorção da Percepção/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Visão Ocular
16.
Pathol Int ; 67(9): 447-452, 2017 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28799264

RESUMO

Lanthanum (La) deposition has been observed in gastrointestinal mucosa of dialysis patients treated with La carbonate to treat hyperphosphatemia in the 6 years since its authorization in Japan. We investigated gastrointestinal biopsies from 112 dialysis patients, and found 15 cases of histiocytic aggregation with crystalloids and one case of duodenitis with histiocyte aggregation without crystalloids in the 30 patients treated with La carbonate. No histiocytic lesions were observed in the 82 patients without La carbonate administration. So far in total 70 cases of La deposition in the alimentary tract have been reported, including our 16 cases. Neither clinical nor histological findings other than histiocytic aggregation were specific in the patients with La deposition. We also compared the groups with and without La deposition, revealing that the daily and total doses of La carbonate showed statistically significant correlations with La deposition. However the causality with their histologic features, e.g. intestinal metaplasia and degree of inflammation, were inconclusive between the two groups. Although no critical symptoms have been reported, it is necessary to accumulate more cases to clarify the mechanism of La deposition, because dialysis patients must take phosphate buffers for a long period.


Assuntos
Mucosa Gástrica/patologia , Mucosa Intestinal/patologia , Lantânio/análise , Idoso , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Feminino , Mucosa Gástrica/química , Histiócitos/patologia , Humanos , Hiperfosfatemia/etiologia , Hiperfosfatemia/prevenção & controle , Mucosa Intestinal/química , Lantânio/administração & dosagem , Lantânio/efeitos adversos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Diálise Renal/efeitos adversos
17.
J Vis ; 17(13): 15, 2017 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29192314

RESUMO

The majority of work on the perception of transparency has focused on static images with luminance-defined contour junctions, but recent work has shown that dynamic image sequences with dynamic image deformations also provide information about transparency. The present study demonstrates that when part of a static image is dynamically deformed, contour junctions at which deforming and nondeforming contours are connected facilitate the deformation-based perception of a transparent layer. We found that the impression of a transparent layer was stronger when a dynamically deforming area was adjacent to static nondeforming areas than when presented alone. When contour junctions were not formed at the dynamic-static boundaries, however, the impression of a transparent layer was not facilitated by the presence of static surrounding areas. The effect of the deformation-defined junctions was attenuated when the spatial pattern of luminance contrast at the junctions was inconsistent with the perceived transparency related to luminance contrast, while the effect did not change when the spatial luminance pattern was consistent with it. In addition, the results showed that contour completions across the junctions were required for the perception of a transparent layer. These results indicate that deformation-defined junctions that involve contour completion between deforming and nondeforming regions enhance the perception of a transparent layer, and that the deformation-based perceptual transparency can be promoted by the simultaneous presence of appropriately configured luminance and contrast-other features that can also by themselves produce the sensation of perceiving transparency.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Simulação por Computador , Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção de Profundidade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção de Movimento , Psicofísica , Percepção Visual
18.
J Vis ; 17(4): 8, 2017 04 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28423413

RESUMO

We are surrounded by many textures with fine dense structures, such as human hair and fabrics, whose individual elements are often finer than the spatial resolution limit of the visual system or that of a digitized image. Here we show that human observers have an ability to visually estimate subresolution fineness of those textures. We carried out a psychophysical experiment to show that observers could correctly discriminate differences in the fineness of hair-like dense line textures even when the thinnest line element was much finer than the resolution limit of the eye or that of the display. The physical image analysis of the textures, along with a theoretical analysis based on the central limit theorem, indicates that as the fineness of texture increases and the number of texture elements per resolvable unit increases, the intensity contrast of the texture decreases and the intensity histogram approaches a Gaussian shape. Subsequent psychophysical experiments showed that these image features indeed play critical roles in fineness perception; i.e., lowering the contrast made artificial and natural textures look finer, and this effect was most evident for textures with unimodal Gaussian-like intensity distributions. These findings indicate that the human visual system is able to estimate subresolution texture fineness on the basis of diagnostic image features correlated with subresolution fineness, such as the intensity contrast and the shape of the intensity histogram.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Distribuição Normal , Psicofísica
19.
J Vis ; 17(5): 7, 2017 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28505665

RESUMO

Color vision provides humans and animals with the abilities to discriminate colors based on the wavelength composition of light and to determine the location and identity of objects of interest in cluttered scenes (e.g., ripe fruit among foliage). However, we argue that color vision can inform us about much more than color alone. Since a trichromatic image carries more information about the optical properties of a scene than a monochromatic image does, color can help us recognize complex material qualities. Here we show that human vision uses color statistics of an image for the perception of an ecologically important surface condition (i.e., wetness). Psychophysical experiments showed that overall enhancement of chromatic saturation, combined with a luminance tone change that increases the darkness and glossiness of the image, tended to make dry scenes look wetter. Theoretical analysis along with image analysis of real objects indicated that our image transformation, which we call the wetness enhancing transformation, is consistent with actual optical changes produced by surface wetting. Furthermore, we found that the wetness enhancing transformation operator was more effective for the images with many colors (large hue entropy) than for those with few colors (small hue entropy). The hue entropy may be used to separate surface wetness from other surface states having similar optical properties. While surface wetness and surface color might seem to be independent, there are higher order color statistics that can influence wetness judgments, in accord with the ecological statistics. The present findings indicate that the visual system uses color image statistics in an elegant way to help estimate the complex physical status of a scene.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Cor , Luz , Molhabilidade , Humanos , Psicofísica , Propriedades de Superfície
20.
J Vis ; 17(6): 14, 2017 06 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28637053

RESUMO

Characterization of the functional relationship between sensory inputs and neuronal or observers' perceptual responses is one of the fundamental goals of systems neuroscience and psychophysics. Conventional methods, such as reverse correlation and spike-triggered data analyses are limited in their ability to resolve complex and inherently nonlinear neuronal/perceptual processes because these methods require input stimuli to be Gaussian with a zero mean. Recent studies have shown that analyses based on a generalized linear model (GLM) do not require such specific input characteristics and have advantages over conventional methods. GLM, however, relies on iterative optimization algorithms and its calculation costs become very expensive when estimating the nonlinear parameters of a large-scale system using large volumes of data. In this paper, we introduce a new analytical method for identifying a nonlinear system without relying on iterative calculations and yet also not requiring any specific stimulus distribution. We demonstrate the results of numerical simulations, showing that our noniterative method is as accurate as GLM in estimating nonlinear parameters in many cases and outperforms conventional, spike-triggered data analyses. As an example of the application of our method to actual psychophysical data, we investigated how different spatiotemporal frequency channels interact in assessments of motion direction. The nonlinear interaction estimated by our method was consistent with findings from previous vision studies and supports the validity of our method for nonlinear system identification.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Dinâmica não Linear , Potenciais de Ação , Algoritmos , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Psicofísica
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA