Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 93
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Neuroimage ; 273: 120076, 2023 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37004828

RESUMO

Brain responses to food are thought to reflect food's rewarding value and to fluctuate with dietary restraint. We propose that brain responses to food are dynamic and depend on attentional focus. Food pictures (high-caloric/low-caloric, palatable/unpalatable) were presented during fMRI-scanning, while attentional focus (hedonic/health/neutral) was induced in 52 female participants varying in dietary restraint. The level of brain activity was hardly different between palatable versus unpalatable foods or high-caloric versus low-caloric foods. Activity in several brain regions was higher in hedonic than in health or neutral attentional focus (p < .05, FWE-corrected). Palatability and calorie content could be decoded from multi-voxel activity patterns (p < .05, FDR-corrected). Dietary restraint did not significantly influence brain responses to food. So, level of brain activity in response to food stimuli depends on attentional focus, and may reflect salience, not reward value. Palatability and calorie content are reflected in patterns of brain activity.


Assuntos
Dieta , Alimentos , Feminino , Humanos , Encéfalo , Ingestão de Energia , Preferências Alimentares , Sinais (Psicologia) , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
2.
BMC Public Health ; 23(1): 696, 2023 04 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37060051

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The present study is the first step of a 3-year European project in which a tailored smartphone application will be developed and tested as a potential tool in the personalized treatment of children and adolescents with overweight. METHODS: In this study, 10 focus groups (n = 48 participants) were conducted in Belgium, The Netherlands and France with adolescents with overweight (12-16 years; n = 30) and parents of adolescents with overweight (n = 18) to investigate their perceptions on (un)healthy behavior, the drivers of these behaviors, and the needs of an eHealth application for weight loss. A thorough thematic analysis was performed using Nvivo12. RESULTS: Results show that adolescents with overweight have a well-articulated perspective on (un)healthy behavior and their needs. Parents underestimate their own influence on the (un)healthy behavior of their children and report difficulties in healthy lifestyle parenting, which makes their role as a coach rather ambiguous. Concerning the needs of an eHealth application, both parents and adolescents formulated some challenging expectations regarding the content and the format including information, a monitoring feature and features that increase participants' motivation to behave healthy. The results of this analysis will form the basis for designing a personalized eHealth application, which will be tested in a next phase. CONCLUSION: We can conclude that adolescents have a well-articulated perspective on healthy and unhealthy behavior and their needs, whereby a new app could be of great help. It could function as a day-by-day diary and as a supportive coach.


Assuntos
Aplicativos Móveis , Sobrepeso , Criança , Humanos , Adolescente , Sobrepeso/terapia , Smartphone , Medicina de Precisão , Estilo de Vida
3.
J Hum Nutr Diet ; 36(5): 2085-2098, 2023 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37545043

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: In today's obesogenic environment high-caloric palatable foods are omnipresent, making it hard for many to reach and maintain a healthy body weight. This study investigates the effects of a health versus hedonic mindset on daily-life snacking behaviour. The hypothesis is that a health mindset leads to reduced snacking behaviour compared to a hedonic mindset. This effect is expected to be most pronounced with high dietary restraint and least pronounced with high trait self-control. METHODS: For 3 weeks, degree of craving and amount of snacks that were craved and consumed were assessed four times a day, using smartphone Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA). A total of 111 female participants (body mass index range: 20-23.5) were randomly assigned to a 1-week health (n = 53) or hedonic (n = 58) mindset, occurring in week 2 of the EMA protocol. The mindset manipulations consisted of text messages, focusing either on the enjoyment of tasty food (hedonic) or on healthy living and eating (health). RESULTS: contrary to our hypotheses, mindset did not affect snacking behaviour. Instead, degree of craving and intake of snacks reduced significantly over time, not moderated by mindset, dietary restraint (Restraint Scale) or trait self-control (Brief Self-Control Scale). Importantly, this was not due to reduced compliance. Possibly, the reduced craving and snacking behaviour were due to monitoring and/or socially desirable answering tendencies. Additional time point analyses showed that craving was strongest in the late afternoon (3:30-5:00 PM), and-across mindset conditions-degree of craving correlated negatively with trait self-control. CONCLUSIONS: future studies could manipulate degree of monitoring and design individually tailored manipulations.


Assuntos
Comportamento Alimentar , Autocontrole , Humanos , Feminino , Lanches , Fissura , Dieta Saudável
4.
Appetite ; 168: 105761, 2022 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34662599

RESUMO

Research has shown repeatedly that overweight and obesity are associated with more fluctuations in attentional bias (AB), as reflected in trial-level bias scores (TL-BS). More specifically, people with a high BMI more frequently alternate their attention towards and away from food over time. The current study proposed and tested a model on the mechanism behind the positive association between variability of AB for food and BMI. It was hypothesized that poor executive control (poor response inhibition and lower sustained attention) would be related to both higher BMI and more fluctuations in AB for food and this would, at least partly, explain the significant relationship between variability of AB for food and BMI. To test our hypotheses, 99 female participants completed both an online food dot-probe task and an online stop-signal task. It was found that response inhibition was significantly related to BMI, whereas, contrary to our hypotheses, variability of AB for food was not related to BMI nor to executive control. In conclusion, this study revealed a relation between poor inhibitory control and higher BMI. However, a role of executive control in fluctuations in attention for food could not be established.


Assuntos
Viés de Atenção , Sinais (Psicologia) , Função Executiva , Feminino , Alimentos , Humanos , Sobrepeso
5.
Appetite ; 178: 106164, 2022 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35863505

RESUMO

Obesity reached pandemic proportions and weight-loss treatments are mostly ineffective. The level of brain activity in the reward circuitry is proposed to be proportionate to the reward value of food stimuli, and stronger in people with obesity. However, empirical evidence is inconsistent. This may be due to the double-sided nature of high caloric palatable foods: at once highly palatable and high in calories (unhealthy). This study hypothesizes that, viewing high caloric palatable foods, a hedonic attentional focus compared to a health and a neutral attentional focus elicits more activity in reward-related brain regions, mostly in people with obesity. Moreover, caloric content and food palatability can be decoded from multivoxel patterns of activity most accurately in people with obesity and in the corresponding attentional focus. During one fMRI-session, attentional focus (hedonic, health, neutral) was manipulated using a one-back task with individually tailored food stimuli in 32 healthy-weight people and 29 people with obesity. Univariate analyses (p < 0.05, FWE-corrected) showed that brain activity was not different for palatable vs. unpalatable foods, nor for high vs. low caloric foods. Instead, this was higher in the hedonic compared to the health and neutral attentional focus. Multivariate analyses (MVPA) (p < 0.05, FDR-corrected) showed that palatability and caloric content could be decoded above chance level, independently of either BMI or attentional focus. Thus, brain activity to visual food stimuli is neither proportionate to the reward value (palatability and/or caloric content), nor significantly moderated by BMI. Instead, it depends on people's attentional focus, and may reflect motivational salience. Furthermore, food palatability and caloric content are represented as patterns of brain activity, independently of BMI and attentional focus. So, food reward value is reflected in patterns, not levels, of brain activity.


Assuntos
Alimentos , Recompensa , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Ingestão de Energia , Humanos , Obesidade
6.
Appetite ; 148: 104609, 2020 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31954729

RESUMO

Research investigating neural responses to visual food stimuli has produced inconsistent results. Crucially, high-caloric palatable foods have a double-sided nature - they are often craved but are also considered unhealthy - which may have contributed to the inconsistency in the literature. Taking this double-sided nature into account in the current study, neural responses to individually tailored palatable and unpalatable high caloric food stimuli were measured, while participants' (females with overweight: n = 23) attentional focus was manipulated to be either hedonic or neutral. Notably, results showed that the level of neural activity was not significantly different for palatable than for unpalatable food stimuli. Instead, independent of food palatability, several brain regions (including regions in the mesocorticolimbic system) responded more strongly when attentional focus was hedonic than when neutral (p < 0.05, cluster-based FWE corrected). Multivariate analyses showed that food palatability could be decoded from multi-voxel patterns of neural activity (p < 0.05, FDR corrected), mostly with a hedonic attentional focus. These findings illustrate that the level of neural activity might not be proportionate to the palatability of foods, but that food palatability can be decoded from multi-voxel patterns of neural activity. Moreover, they underline the importance of considering attentional focus when measuring food-related neural responses.


Assuntos
Atenção , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Obesidade/psicologia , Recompensa , Paladar , Adulto , Dieta/psicologia , Ingestão de Energia , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Feminino , Alimentos , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise Multivariada , Obesidade/etiologia , Sobrepeso , Prazer
7.
Am J Psychother ; 73(1): 8-14, 2020 Mar 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32122161

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Although the effectiveness of interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT) and cognitive therapy (CT) for major depression has been established, little is known about how and for whom they work and how they compare in the long term. The latter is especially relevant for IPT because research on its long-term effects has been limited. This overview paper summarizes findings from a Dutch randomized controlled trial on the effects and mechanisms of change of IPT versus CT for major depression. METHODS: Adult outpatients with depression (N=182) were randomly assigned to CT (N=76), IPT (N=75), or a 2-month waitlist control group followed by patient's treatment of choice (N=31). The primary outcome was depression severity. Other outcomes were quality of life, social and general psychological functioning, and scores on various mechanism measures. Interventions were compared at the end of treatment and up to 17 months follow-up. RESULTS: On average, IPT and CT were both superior to waitlist, and their outcomes did not differ significantly from one another. However, the pathway through which change occurred appeared to differ. For a majority of participants, one of the interventions was predicted to be more beneficial than the other. No support for the theoretical models of change was found. CONCLUSIONS: Outcomes of IPT and CT did not appear to differ significantly. IPT may have an enduring effect not different from that of CT. The field would benefit from further refinement of study methods to disentangle mechanisms of change and from advances in the field of personalized medicine (i.e., person-specific analyses and treatment selection methods).


Assuntos
Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental , Depressão/psicologia , Depressão/terapia , Psicoterapia Interpessoal , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Qualidade de Vida , Resultado do Tratamento
8.
Appetite ; 135: 86-92, 2019 04 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30634007

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Empirical evidence of attentional bias (AB) for food in obese and restrained eaters is contradictory. It has been suggested that AB for food in obese people might reflect both food craving and food-related concerns. Thus, AB for food may be a dynamic process. METHODS: A new computational methodology (Zvielli, Bernstein, & Koster, 2015) was adopted to reanalyze the reaction time (RT) and dwell time of three food-related dot-probe task with eye-tracking studies (Werthmann et al., 2015; Werthmann et al., 2011, 2013). This new computing method uses a sequence of bias scores to express the dynamic changes of AB. Moreover, the variability of RT on filler trials was also calculated. The critical groups in these studies were overweight/obese adults, obese children, and healthy-weight restrained eaters. RESULTS: Both the variability of AB for food stimuli and the variability of RT on filler could significantly predict the variance in body mass index (BMI). When controlling for the variability of RT on filler trials and mean AB score, larger variability of AB for food stimuli still existed in obese children and aggregated dataset. The variability of AB for food stimuli demonstrated no significant correlation with restrained eating scores and dwell time variability. CONCLUSIONS: Overweight/obese individuals are characterized by more variability in attention, and this variability is mainly unspecific. It probably reflects less effective executive control ability.


Assuntos
Viés de Atenção , Sinais (Psicologia) , Comportamento Alimentar , Alimentos , Obesidade/psicologia , Tempo de Reação , Autocontrole , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Índice de Massa Corporal , Criança , Fissura , Função Executiva , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Sobrepeso
9.
Appetite ; 136: 86-92, 2019 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30682381

RESUMO

This study assessed internal reliability and test-retest reliability of attention bias scores for food derived from the dot probe task. A visual dot probe task with food and non-food pictures (presented for 3000 ms) was administered to 53 healthy women on two occasions. Attention bias scores for food were calculated based on manual response latencies (reaction time bias) and concurrent assessment of eye-movements (direction bias and dwell-time bias). Subjective hunger and blood glucose levels were measured on both testing occasions. Dietary restraint and eating disorder symptoms were assessed during the second session. Results showed that direction bias had poor internal and test-retest reliability. Dwell time bias had excellent internal and acceptable test-retest reliability. Reaction time bias had acceptable internal and good test-retest reliability. Exploratory correlational analyses found that hunger, blood glucose, dietary restraint and eating disorder symptoms were not consistently significantly correlated with indices of attention bias for food. Overall, these findings contradict previous studies that reported low reliability of attention bias indices derived from the visual dot probe task. The implications are that a longer stimulus presentation time (i.e. ≥ 3000 ms), the use of eye-tracking and the use of appetizing stimuli can yield reliable attention bias scores for food. However, the interpretation of dot-probe scores of attention bias for food based on a dot probe task with 3000 ms presentation time and the score's relationship to theoretically relevant constructs such as hunger, eating restraint and eating disorder symptoms, require further clarification.


Assuntos
Viés de Atenção/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Alimentos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Londres , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
10.
Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care ; 21(6): 444-448, 2018 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30124477

RESUMO

PURPOSE OF REVIEW: The dominant view in the literature is that increased neural reactivity to high-caloric palatable foods in the mesocorticolimbic system is a stable-specific characteristic of obese people. In this review, we argue that this viewpoint may not be justified, and we propose that the neural response to food stimuli is dynamic, and in synchrony with the current motivational and cognitive state of an individual. We will further motivate why a clear mental task in the scanner is a necessity for drawing conclusions from neural activity, and why multivariate approaches to functional MRI (fMRI) data-analysis may carry the field forward. RECENT FINDINGS: From the reviewed literature we draw the conclusions that: neural food-cue reactivity depends strongly on cognitive factors such as the use of cognitive regulation strategies, task demands, and focus of attention; neural activity in the mesocorticolimbic system is not proportionate to the hedonic value of presented food stimuli; and multivariate approaches to fMRI data-analysis have shown that hedonic value can be decoded from multivoxel patterns of neural activity. SUMMARY: Future research should take the dynamic nature of food-reward processing into account and take advantage from state-of-the-art multivariate approaches to fMRI data-analysis.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Alimentos , Recompensa , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Obesidade/psicologia
11.
Appetite ; 120: 297-301, 2018 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28939407

RESUMO

The texture of food can be a reason why children reject it: It matters if food is crispy, slimy, smooth or has pips and bits in it. In general, mere exposure is the best method to increase acceptance of food: becoming more familiar with a food by repeated exposure increases liking for it. However, exposure to texture can be difficult, as children can be reluctant to try tasting it. In the current study, it is tested if acceptance of a food with a specific texture is improved after exposure to the feel of it, with hands only. Sixty-six children (between 3 and 10 years old) were randomly assigned to either the exposure or control condition. In the exposure condition, children played with an colourless and odourless jelly with their hands and in the control group, children played a board game. Afterwards, children were asked to taste 3 desserts (in balanced order): smooth strawberry yoghurt, strawberry yoghurt with pieces and strawberry jelly. Results showed that the children in the exposure condition ate specifically more of the jelly dessert - the texture of which they had been pre-exposed to - compared to the children in control condition. No group differences were found for the other two desserts. The results imply that feeling the texture of a food with hands increases the acceptance of food with the same texture. Playing with food with hands seems therefore be a first step in getting familiar with food and might help to increase variety of food intake.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Percepção Gustatória , Percepção do Tato , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Paladar
13.
Appetite ; 105: 53-9, 2016 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27174250

RESUMO

Attention bias for food could be a cognitive pathway to overeating in obesity and restrained eating. Yet, empirical evidence for individual differences (e.g., in restrained eating and body mass index) in attention bias for food is mixed. We tested experimentally if temporarily induced health versus palatability mindsets influenced attention bias for food, and whether restrained eating moderated this relation. After manipulating mindset (health vs. palatability) experimentally, food-related attention bias was measured by eye-movements (EM) and response latencies (RL) during a visual probe task depicting high-calorie food and non-food. Restrained eating was assessed afterwards. A significant interaction of mindset and restrained eating on RL bias emerged, ß = 0.36, t(58) = 2.05, p = 0.045: A health mindset - as compared to a palatability mindset - attenuated attention bias for high-caloric food only in participants with higher eating restraint. No effects were observed on EM biases. The current results demonstrate that state differences in health versus palatability mindsets can cause attenuated attention bias for high-calorie food cues in participants with higher eating restraint. Our findings add to emerging evidence that state differences in mindsets can bias attention for food, above the influence of trait differences.


Assuntos
Atenção , Dieta Saudável , Atenção Plena , Modelos Psicológicos , Sobrepeso/prevenção & controle , Cooperação do Paciente , Autocontrole , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudos Cross-Over , Sinais (Psicologia) , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Preferências Alimentares , Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
14.
Appetite ; 96: 426-431, 2016 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26463017

RESUMO

This study examined children's spontaneous associations of special events with food. Children in primary education (N = 111, age between 10 and 13 years) at a school in Germany wrote down their first five associations with five special or festive events (Christmas, holidays, weekend, carnival and birthday). After completing the free-word association test, they were offered a choice between a candy and a toy. Finally, their body mass index (BMI) was measured. The first prediction was that overweight and obese children would associate special events more often with food than normal weight and leaner children. The second prediction was that choice for a candy would be predicted by a higher number of food-related associations. The first hypothesis was not supported: BMI was negatively related to number of food-related associations (the lower the BMI, the more food-related associations). The second hypothesis was also not supported: There was no relation between number of food-related associations and choice for a candy or toy. A possible explanation for the finding that leaner children reported more food-related associations is that for them specific sweets and snack food are more exclusively connected to special occasions than for overweight children. Speculatively, this may be the result of differences in food parenting styles between parents of heavier and leaner children. Parents of leaner children often have a more restrictive style, i.e., reserving specific foods for specific, relatively rare occasions whereas parents of overweight children adopt more liberal food rules.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Férias e Feriados , Sobrepeso/psicologia , Obesidade Infantil/psicologia , Magreza/psicologia , Adolescente , Índice de Massa Corporal , Criança , Ingestão de Energia , Feminino , Alemanha , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Poder Familiar
15.
Cereb Cortex ; 24(6): 1589-600, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23382517

RESUMO

Cue reactivity and craving can be influenced by cue exposure with response prevention (CERP). This study investigated the neural correlates of CERP using functional magnetic resonance imaging, while participants smelled chocolate (17 participants) or a control object (17 participants). CERP was interrupted by 7 scanning sequences measuring the brain response to neutral and chocolate pictures. Chocolate craving was hypothesized to be mirrored by activation in brain reward regions. As expected, control group craving remained similar throughout the session. A short exposure (30 min) increased chocolate craving in the experimental group, which was mirrored by significant group differences in activation in brain reward regions. Unexpectedly, a long exposure (60 min) did not lead to craving extinction in the experimental group, although craving started to decrease at this point. On a neural level, however, activation in regions of interest in the experimental group seemed to have extinguished after the long exposure, as activation levels returned to or fell below control group levels. These results indicate that brain reward activation during CERP is linked to craving, at least for a short exposure. Regarding a longer exposure, the decline in brain reward activation in the experimental group may be a precursor of a decrease in craving.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cacau , Doces , Fissura/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Ingestão de Alimentos , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Extinção Psicológica/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Fome/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Recompensa , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
16.
Appetite ; 91: 28-34, 2015 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25817483

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The aim of the present study was to investigate the role of automatic approach/avoidance tendencies for food in Anorexia Nervosa (AN). We used a longitudinal approach and tested whether a reduction in eating disorder symptoms is associated with enhanced approach tendencies towards food and whether approach tendencies towards food at baseline are predictive for treatment outcome after one year follow up. METHOD: The Affective Simon Task-manikin version (AST-manikin) was administered to measure automatic approach/avoidance tendencies towards high-caloric and low-caloric food in young AN patients. Percentage underweight and eating disorder symptoms as indexed by the EDE-Q were determined both during baseline and at one year follow up. RESULTS: At baseline anorexia patients showed an approach tendency for low caloric food, but not for high caloric food, whereas at 1 year follow up, they have an approach tendency for both high and low caloric food. Change in approach bias was neither associated with change in underweight nor with change in eating disorder symptoms. Strength of approach/avoidance tendencies was not predictive for percentage underweight. DISCUSSION: Although approach tendencies increased after one year, approach tendencies were neither associated with concurrent change in eating disorder symptoms nor predictive for treatment success as indexed by EDE-Q. This implicates that, so far, there is no reason to add a method designed to directly target approach/avoidance tendencies to the conventional approach to treat patients with a method designed to influence the more deliberate processes in AN.


Assuntos
Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Nutrição do Adolescente , Anorexia Nervosa/terapia , Terapia Comportamental , Fenômenos Fisiológicos da Nutrição Infantil , Comportamento de Escolha , Dieta , Magreza/prevenção & controle , Adolescente , Adulto , Anorexia Nervosa/dietoterapia , Anorexia Nervosa/fisiopatologia , Anorexia Nervosa/psicologia , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Criança , Terapia Combinada , Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais , Dieta/efeitos adversos , Dieta/psicologia , Progressão da Doença , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Países Baixos , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Autorrelato , Magreza/etiologia , Adulto Jovem
17.
Appetite ; 84: 181-7, 2015 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25312750

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Picky or fussy eating is common in early childhood and associated with a decreased preference for a variety of foods. The aim of the current study was to test experimentally which sensory food feature influences food acceptance, which, in turn is an indication for fussy eating, in young children (32 - 48 months). Another aim was to evaluate if the behavioural measurement of food acceptance is related to parental reports of their child's fussy eating behaviour, parental feeding styles and children's BMI. METHOD: In a repeated-measures-design, three sensory features were manipulated separately (i.e., colour, texture and taste) while keeping the other two features constant. The baseline measurement consisted of a well-liked yoghurt, which was presented before each manipulation variant. The number of spoons that children (N = 32) consumed from each variant were registered as behavioural indication for food acceptance. Parental reports of children's eating behaviour and parental feeding styles; and children's BMI were also measured. RESULTS: The manipulation of food texture caused a significant decrease in intake. Colour and taste manipulations of the yoghurt did not affect children's intake. Parental reports of children's fussy eating behaviour and parental feeding styles were not related to the behavioural observation of food acceptance. The behavioural measurement of food acceptance and parental accounts of fussy eating were not related to children's BMI. CONCLUSION: Food texture but not taste or colour alternations affected food acceptance, at least when consuming variations of a well-liked yoghurt. This knowledge is important for further research on picky-eating interventions. Parental reports of fussy eating did not concur with the behavioural observation of food acceptance. Further research is warranted to test whether these findings generalize to other food types.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil , Ingestão de Alimentos , Comportamento Alimentar , Preferências Alimentares , Qualidade dos Alimentos , Índice de Massa Corporal , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Obesidade/etiologia , Personalidade , Propriedades de Superfície
18.
Appetite ; 87: 98-107, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25528694

RESUMO

Previous studies have shown that one's brain response to high-calorie food cues can predict long-term weight gain or weight loss. The neural correlates that predict food intake in the short term have, however, hardly been investigated. This study examined which brain regions' activation predicts chocolate intake after participants had been either exposed to real chocolate or to control stimuli during approximately one hour, with interruptions for fMRI measurements. Further we investigated whether the variance in chocolate intake could be better explained by activated brain regions than by self-reported craving. In total, five brain regions correlated with subsequent chocolate intake. The activation of two reward regions (the right caudate and the left frontopolar cortex) correlated positively with intake in the exposure group. The activation of two regions associated with cognitive control (the left dorsolateral and left mid-dorsolateral PFC) correlated negatively with intake in the control group. When the regression analysis was conducted with the exposure and the control group together, an additional region's activation (the right anterior PFC) correlated positively with chocolate intake. In all analyses, the intake variance explained by neural correlates was above and beyond the variance explained by self-reported craving. These results are in line with neuroimaging research showing that brain responses are a better predictor of subsequent intake than self-reported craving. Therefore, our findings might provide for a missing link by associating brain activation, previously shown to predict weight change, with short-term intake.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cacau , Fissura/fisiologia , Ingestão de Alimentos/psicologia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Motivação , Paladar , Adolescente , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Neuroimagem , Recompensa , Resposta de Saciedade/fisiologia , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
20.
Int J Methods Psychiatr Res ; 33(3): e2028, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39030856

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The network approach to psychopathology posits that mental disorders emerge from dynamic interactions among psychopathology-relevant variables. Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) is frequently used to assess these variables in daily life. Considering the transdiagnostic nature of the network approach to psychopathology, this study describes the development of a transdiagnostic EMA protocol for psychopathology. METHODS: First, 96 clinicians completed an online survey, providing three EMA constructs for up to three disorders they specialize in, and three EMA constructs relevant across disorders (transdiagnostic constructs). Second, 12 focus groups were conducted with clinical experts for specific types of diagnoses (e.g., mood disorders, anxiety disorders). Finally, a selection of items was reached by consensus. Two raters independently coded the online survey responses with an inter-rater agreement of 87.3%. RESULTS: Jaccard indices showed up to 52.6% overlap in EMA items across types of diagnoses. The most frequently reported transdiagnostic constructs were mood, sleep quality, and stress. A final set of EMA items is created based on items' frequency and informativeness, ensuring completeness across diagnoses and minimizing burden. CONCLUSIONS: The described procedure resulted in a feasible EMA protocol to examine psychopathology transdiagnostically. Feasibility was helped by the overlap in mentioned symptoms across disorders. Such overlap raises questions about the validity of DSM categories.


Assuntos
Avaliação Momentânea Ecológica , Transtornos Mentais , Humanos , Transtornos Mentais/diagnóstico , Adulto , Masculino , Feminino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicopatologia/métodos , Grupos Focais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA