Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 33
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cogn Emot ; 32(8): 1625-1636, 2018 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29411684

RESUMO

This research investigated whether precues engage proactive control to reduce emotional interference during speech production. A picture-word interference task required participants to name target pictures accompanied by taboo, negative, or neutral distractors. Proactive control was manipulated by presenting precues that signalled the type of distractor that would appear on the next trial. Experiment 1 included one block of trials with precues and one without, whereas Experiment 2 mixed precued and uncued trials. Consistent with previous research, picture naming was slowed in both experiments when distractors were taboo or negative compared to neutral, with the greatest slowing effect when distractors were taboo. Evidence that precues engaged proactive control to reduce interference from taboo (but not negative) distractors was found in Experiment 1. In contrast, mixing precued trials in Experiment 2 resulted in no taboo cueing benefit. These results suggest that item-level proactive control can be engaged under certain conditions to reduce taboo interference during speech production, findings that help to refine a role for cognitive control of distraction during speech production.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Fala/fisiologia , Tabu/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Semântica , Adulto Jovem
2.
Anxiety Stress Coping ; : 1-12, 2024 Jul 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38965756

RESUMO

Anxiety can have adverse effects on cognition such as impairing test performance or restricting working memory. One way of reducing anxiety is through humor, and the present research investigated if the perception of laughter, which is often seen as a reaction to humor, could impact self-reported anxiety. Participants completed the STAI battery containing subscales for both state and trait anxiety before and after one of three manipulations: a laughter sounds rating task, a neutral sounds rating task, or a working memory span task. Results showed that perceiving laughter decreased both state and trait anxiety, taking a working memory test increased state anxiety, and perceiving neutral sounds had no effect on either type of anxiety. These findings are interpreted as evidence that the positive emotions induced by hearing laughter help to regulate anxiety by undoing arousal, even when negative emotions are not present.

3.
Psychol Aging ; 39(3): 288-298, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829340

RESUMO

Emotional properties of words can profoundly affect their processing, depending on both the valence (pleasantness) and the degree of arousal (excitation) that the word elicits. Words that are strongly emotionally arousing (such as taboo words) can interfere with subsequent language processing (White & Abrams, 2021). However, little is known about whether or how aging affects the processing of highly arousing language. The present study provides a characterization of how adults across the lifespan evaluate highly arousing language with a simple rating task that included taboo words, which have previously been used to examine lexical interference caused by arousal, and humorous words, which are also highly arousing without being negatively valenced. While arousal ratings were strongly positively correlated with both tabooness and humor ratings for young adults, these relationships weakened with age and overall arousal ratings were lower for middle-aged and older adults compared to young adults. Age effects cannot be readily accounted for by age-related differences in psychosocial variables such as self-reported profanity avoidance or religiosity. The effect of age on arousal should be considered in the design of studies examining age-related changes in emotional language processing. Furthermore, age differences in arousal should be considered as a potential mechanism in studies exploring emotional language processing across adulthood. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Nível de Alerta , Emoções , Idioma , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Idoso , Emoções/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais
4.
Psychol Aging ; 39(3): 209-214, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829338

RESUMO

This is an introduction to the special issue "Adult Age Differences in Language, Communication, and Learning from Text." These articles illustrate the great variety of language use through the adult lifespan, tell us a little more-and invite further inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Comunicação , Idioma , Aprendizagem , Humanos , Adulto , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Adulto Jovem , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
5.
Am Psychol ; 79(1): 1-8, 2024 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38236211

RESUMO

Digital technologies are pervasive in every aspect of our daily lives. The proliferation of such technologies has also influenced the conduct of biomedical, behavioral, and social research. The articles in this special issue provide illustrative examples of the range of applications of digital technologies in psychological science research across a variety of populations. They highlight ethical, legal, and social issues that emerge when digital technologies are employed in psychological science research in the current era of rapid technological change, increasing prevalence of interdisciplinary team science, evolving understandings of ethical precepts and social norms, and promoting open science. This introduction to the special issue provides an overview of challenges to the Belmont principles of respect for persons, beneficence, and justice revealed in the 10 articles. The conclusions reached are that these ethical principles do not always adapt well to the digital environment and that practices cannot always be uniquely classified under one of these three headings. We recommend that conceptual work and practical guidance be undertaken to expand the interpretation of these principles in the light of evolving societal norms and emerging ethical issues. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Tecnologia Digital , Tecnologia , Estudos Interdisciplinares , Justiça Social , Normas Sociais
6.
Lang Speech ; : 238309241228863, 2024 Feb 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38357874

RESUMO

Emotion can have a profound effect on language processing, and taboo words have been increasingly used in research as highly emotional, negatively valenced stimuli. However, because taboo words as a lexical category are socially constructed and semantically idiosyncratic, they may also have complex emotional characteristics. This complexity may not be fully considered by researchers using taboo words as research stimuli. This study gathered tabooness, humor, and arousal ratings to provide a resource for researchers to better understand the sources and characteristics of the strong emotions generated by taboo words. A total of 411 participants aged 18-83 were recruited via online platforms, and all participants rated the same 264 words on tabooness, humor, and arousal. Analyses indicated that tabooness and humor ratings were positively related to each other, and both were predicted by arousal ratings. The set of ratings included here provides a tool for researchers using taboo stimuli, and our findings highlight methodological considerations while broadening our understanding of the cognitive and linguistic nature of highly emotional language.

7.
Psychol Aging ; 39(3): 299-312, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829341

RESUMO

Emotional content, specifically negative valence, can differentially influence speech production in younger and older adults' autobiographical narratives, which have been interpreted as reflecting age differences in emotion regulation. However, age differences in emotional reactivity are another possible explanation, as younger and older adults frequently differ in their affective responses to negative and positive pictures. The present experiment investigated whether a picture's valence (pleasantness) and arousal (intensity) influenced older adults' production of narratives about those pictures. Thirty younger and 30 older participants produced narratives about pictures that varied in valence (positive, negative, and neutral) and arousal (high, low). Narratives were recorded via Zoom, transcribed, and analyzed with Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count-22 to get measures of emotional word use, disfluencies, and linguistic distance. Results showed that negative valence increased age differences in speech production independent of picture arousal: Relative to younger adults, older adults used more positive words, fewer negative words, and had more silent pauses when telling narratives about negative pictures. In contrast, high arousal decreased age differences such that older adults used fewer positive words in narratives about positive pictures and more linguistically distant words evidenced by fewer present-tense verbs, relative to narratives about low-arousal pictures. Contrary to an explanation of enhanced regulation or control over emotions in older adulthood, these findings support the idea that older adults' speech production is influenced by their reactivity or affective response to emotional stimuli even when the task is not to communicate one's emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta , Emoções , Narração , Humanos , Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Idoso , Feminino , Masculino , Emoções/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Fala , Estimulação Luminosa , Adolescente
8.
Psychol Aging ; 24(2): 324-37, 2009 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19485651

RESUMO

The present research investigated younger and older adults' communicative goals and their effects on off-topic speech for autobiographical narratives. Participants indicated their communicative goals by rating preferences among paired goals, for example, focus-fascinating, one of which was designated as an expressive goal, appropriate for producing elaborative speech, and one of which was an objective goal, suited to producing concise speech. The participants then told stories about episodic and procedural topics, which were rated by groups of younger and older listeners. Age differences emerged in communicative goals, where younger adults clearly favored expressive goals for episodic topics and objective goals for procedural topics. In contrast, older adults' goals were more diverse, consisting of a mixture of expressive and objective goals for both topic types, without a clear preference. Younger adults' goals predicted ratings of off-topic speech assessed by listeners: Younger and older adults were perceived as equivalently focused, coherent, and clear for episodic topics, but older adults were perceived as less focused, less clear, and more talkative than younger adults on procedural topics. These results suggest that age-related changes in off-topic speech emerge as a result of younger adults selecting goals designed to produce more succinct stories.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Comunicação , Objetivos , Memória , Fala , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Linguística , Masculino , Motivação , Narração , Comportamento Verbal
9.
Memory ; 17(5): 481-92, 2009 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19378214

RESUMO

This research tested age-related differences in the retrieval of self-generated new associations under conditions that required intentional or incidental processing. Under intentional or incidental encoding conditions, young and older adults generated new associations by producing a response to a two-letter stem paired with a cue/prime word (e.g., throne-mo_). Memory for these new associations was tested under intentional or incidental retrieval conditions by pairing the word stem with the previous cue/prime word, its homophone partner, or a prime/cue not previously presented. Results indicated equivalent priming and cueing effects for both age groups in all conditions. These results suggest that generation of new associations can eliminate age-related associative deficits, even under intentional encoding and retrieval conditions that typically disadvantage older adults.


Assuntos
Associação , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Memória/fisiologia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Valores de Referência , Inquéritos e Questionários , Testes de Associação de Palavras , Adulto Jovem
10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30282517

RESUMO

Tip-of-the-tongue states (TOTs) are known to increase in frequency across adulthood, but there is wide variability in older adults' TOT rates, suggesting that individual difference factors contribute to TOT incidence. We investigated the role of affect by examining the relationship between self-reported anxiety and depression symptoms and the frequency of TOTs during a laboratory task. Participants were young, middle-aged and older adults in a population-based sample of adults aged 18-87. Increased anxiety was associated with fewer TOTs for the middle-aged group but more TOTs for the older adult group. There was no relationship between anxiety and TOTs for younger adults and no relationships between depression symptoms and TOT incidence for any age group. We discuss our results in terms of attentional control theory, which provides an explanation of how age may affect the relationship between anxiety and TOTs.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Depressão/fisiopatologia , Idioma , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30322318

RESUMO

Creative production has been reported to decline with age. This study investigated age-related differences in creative verbal production. Participants were 30 younger and 30 older adults. Study testing included language and cognitive assessments and the experimental task wherein participants created short stories incorporating words that are not commonly related, semantically or associatively. The resulting stories were rated by independent blinded judges for originality, cohesion, appropriateness, and organization. Younger adults' stories were rated as being significantly more original and more appropriate. Integrating unrelated words to create original stories requires activating widely distributed lexical-semantic networks to develop novel associations. Potential decreased neural network connectivity due to white matter degradation, commonly seen in normal aging, as well as decrements in front-executive disengagement may have affected older adults' performance on this story creation task. Future research using neuroimaging may elucidate possible neuroanatomic correlates of age-related changes in associative creative production..


Assuntos
Associação , Criatividade , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Obras de Ficção como Assunto , Humanos , Masculino , Semântica , Redação , Adulto Jovem
12.
Psychol Aging ; 22(4): 835-45, 2007 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18179301

RESUMO

Young (aged 18-23), young-old (aged 61-73), and old-old (aged 75-89) adults saw general knowledge questions whose answers were designated target words. Participants responded that they knew, did not know, or were having a tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) state for the answer. After TOT states, participants saw a 5-word list in which 1 was a "prime" containing the target's first syllable and that shared or differed in part of speech from the target. The question was then presented again, and target retrieval was attempted. Results revealed age differences in resolution of TOT states as a function of the prime's grammatical class. Following different part-of-speech primes, young and young-old adults showed increased resolution of TOT states relative to phonologically unrelated words, whereas old-old adults did not. In contrast, old-old adults demonstrated decreased resolution of TOT states following same part-of-speech primes, whereas young and young-old adults' TOT resolution was unaffected. These findings are consistent with interactive activation theories of speech production in which phonology can influence lexical selection and also suggest an increased susceptibility to phonological competitors in the later stages of the aging process.


Assuntos
Linguística , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/epidemiologia , Comportamento Verbal , Vocabulário , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Linguística/métodos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fonética , Teoria Psicológica
13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17828628

RESUMO

Young and older adults' ability to retrieve the spellings of high- and low-frequency words was assessed via tests of spelling recognition and production. One of the spelling production tests required participants to write down the correct spellings of auditorily presented words, and accuracy was used to categorize participants in both age groups as good or poor spellers. The results showed that individual spelling ability and word frequency contributed to age differences. Older adults who were poor spellers were less accurate in recognizing and producing correct spelling than young adults who were poor spellers. In contrast, no age differences occurred for good spellers. Furthermore, low-frequency words were especially difficult for young adults and poor spellers, relative to older adults and good spellers. These results indicate that aging alone is not detrimental to the processes underlying recognition or production of spelling but instead compounds existing problems caused by poor spelling.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Aprendizagem Verbal , Percepção Visual , Vocabulário , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
14.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 72(1): 100-106, 2017 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27371482

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: We tested the claim that age-related increases in knowledge interfere with word retrieval, leading to word finding failures. We did this by relating a measure of crystallized intelligence to tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states and picture naming accuracy. METHOD: Participants were from a large (N = 708), cross-sectional (aged 18-88 years), population-based sample from the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience cohort (Cam-CAN; www.cam-can.com). They completed (a) the Spot-the-Word Test (STW), a measure of crystallized intelligence in which participants circled the real word in word/nonword pairs, (b) a TOT-inducing task, and (c) a picture naming task. RESULTS: Age and STW independently predicted TOTs, with higher TOTs for older adults and for participants with lower STW scores. Tests of a moderator model examining interactions between STW and age indicated that STW was a significant negative predictor of TOTs in younger adults, but with increasing age, the effect size gradually approached zero. Results using picture naming accuracy replicated these findings. DISCUSSION: These results do not support the hypothesis that lifelong knowledge acquisition leads to interference that causes an age-related increase in TOTs. Instead, crystallized intelligence supports successful word retrieval, although this relationship weakens across adulthood.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento Cognitivo , Rememoração Mental , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Inteligência/estatística & dados numéricos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fonética , Psicometria , Adulto Jovem
15.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 24(2): 489-495, 2017 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27271052

RESUMO

While both semantic and highly emotional (i.e., taboo) words can interfere with speech production, different theoretical mechanisms have been proposed to explain why interference occurs. Two experiments investigated these theoretical approaches by comparing the magnitude of these two types of interference and the stages at which they occur during picture naming. Participants named target pictures superimposed with semantic, taboo, or unrelated distractor words that were presented at three different stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOA): -150 ms, 0 ms, or +150 ms. In addition, the duration of distractor presentation was manipulated across experiments, with distractors appearing for the duration of the picture (Experiment 1) or for 350 ms (Experiment 2). Taboo distractors interfered more than semantic distractors, i.e., slowed target naming times, at all SOAs. While distractor duration had no effect on type of interference at -150 or 0 SOAs, briefly presented distractors eliminated semantic interference but not taboo interference at +150 SOA. Discussion focuses on how existing speech production theories can explain interference from emotional distractors and the unique role that attention may play in taboo interference.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Semântica , Tabu/psicologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
16.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 42(1): 75-90, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26147668

RESUMO

When people read questions like "How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?", many mistakenly answer "2" despite knowing that Noah sailed the ark. This "Moses illusion" occurs when names share semantic features. Two experiments examined whether shared visual concepts (facial features) exacerbate Moses illusions for celebrity names. Questions contained an unrelated distractor name or a semantic distractor name that was visually similar or dissimilar to the correct target name. Both experiments revealed more Moses illusions occurred for questions containing a visually similar semantic distractor compared with either visually dissimilar or unrelated distractors. Furthermore, presenting a picture of the target (Experiment 1) or the visually similar distractor (Experiment 2) before the question increased accurate detection of the illusion, independent of distractor type. Results challenge theoretical explanations of the Moses illusion as resulting from purely shallow semantic processing and demonstrate the importance of visual information in processing proper names, even when presented in written form. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Ilusões , Leitura , Semântica , Adolescente , Adulto , Face , Pessoas Famosas , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Modelos Psicológicos , Nomes , Estimulação Luminosa , Testes Psicológicos , Tempo de Reação , Adulto Jovem
17.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 12(6): 1018-23, 2005 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16615322

RESUMO

During tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) experiences, phonologically related words have both facilitated and impeded word retrieval. In the present experiment, we examined whether phonologically related words' syntactic class (part of speech) is responsible for these differential effects. Sixty college students saw general knowledge questions whose answers were designated target words and responded "know," "don't know," or "TOT." Following "TOT" and "don't know" responses, the participants saw five words, one of which was a prime. The primes contained the target's first syllable and either shared or did not share the target's part of speech. Following presentation of the primes, retrieval of the target was attempted again. Different-part-of-speech primes facilitated resolution of TOT states, whereas same-part-of-speech primes had no effect, relative to phonologically unrelated words. These results support node structure theory's most-primed-wins principle and the transmission deficit model account of TOT states and detail the importance of syntactic class in the selection of words that are candidates for speech production.


Assuntos
Fonética , Percepção da Fala , Comportamento Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fala , Medida da Produção da Fala , Inquéritos e Questionários
18.
Psychol Aging ; 17(2): 226-35, 2002 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12061408

RESUMO

This experiment investigated whether phonological priming of syllables helps resolve tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states in young and older adults. Young, young-old, and old-old adults read general knowledge questions and responded "know," "TOT," or "don't know" accordingly. Participants then read a list of 10 words that included 3 phonological primes corresponding solely to the first, middle, or last syllable of the target word. Young and young-old adults resolved more TOTs after first-syllable primes, but old-old adults showed no increase in TOT resolution following any primes. These results indicate that presentation of the first syllable of a missing word strengthens the weakened phonological connections that cause TOTs and increases word retrieval, but not for old-old adults who experience greater deficits in the transmission of priming across these connections.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Idioma , Rememoração Mental , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Humanos , Masculino , Processos Mentais , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
19.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 30(3): 645-55, 2004 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15099133

RESUMO

In 2 experiments, the authors investigated phonologically mediated priming of preexisting and new associations in word retrieval. Young and older adults completed paired word stems with the first word that came to mind. Priming of preexisting associations occurred when word-stem pairs containing homophones (e.g., beech-s____) showed more completions with the target (e.g., sand) relative to unrelated pairs (e.g., batch-s____), with more priming for subordinate than for dominant homophones. Priming occurred for new associations independent of dominance such that word-stem pairs containing homophones (e.g., beech-l____ and beach-l____) were completed with the same word (e.g., laugh) more often than unrelated pairs (e.g., beech-l____ and batch-l____). No age differences in phonologically mediated priming were found for either type of association, suggesting age equivalence in the use of bottom-up phonological connections.


Assuntos
Fonética , Percepção da Fala , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reconhecimento Psicológico
20.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 40(2): 313-25, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24294916

RESUMO

Previous research has documented advantages and disadvantages of early bilinguals, defined as learning a 2nd language by school age and using both languages since that time. Relative to monolinguals, early bilinguals manifest deficits in lexical access but benefits in executive function. We investigated whether becoming bilingual after childhood (late bilinguals) can produce the cognitive advantages and disadvantages typical of early bilinguals. Participants were 30 monolingual English speakers, 30 late English-Spanish bilinguals, and 30 early Spanish-English bilinguals who completed a picture naming task (lexical access) and an attentional network task (executive function). Late and early bilinguals manifested equivalent cognitive effects in both tasks, demonstrating lexical access deficits and executive function benefits. These findings provide support for the hypothesis that cognitive effects associated with bilingualism arise as the result of proficient, habitual use of 2 languages and not of developmental changes associated with becoming bilingual during childhood.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Multilinguismo , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Nomes , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Psicolinguística , Tempo de Reação , Autorrelato , Comportamento Verbal , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa