Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
1.
Am J Epidemiol ; 2024 Aug 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39191652

RESUMO

Quality mentoring improves outcomes across career stages, including a sense of belonging, persistence, and productivity. However, the status quo in mentorship culture-including in epidemiology-is an ad hoc approach. This pervasive culture adversely affects individual mentees and the entire scientific research enterprise. Public health disciplines such as epidemiology bear a distinct responsibility to foster an inclusive mentorship culture, ensuring the next generation is equipped not only with methodological expertise but also with a commitment to diversity, equity, inclusion, and the principles of public health. In this commentary, we outline the primary attributes of effective mentors, core competencies, and the evidence base underlying how mentors can improve their skills with comprehensive training. We call on mentors, as well as institutional leaders, to make personal and structural changes, such as requiring mentor training, implementing evidence-based tools (e.g., individual development plans), and regularly evaluating mentorship quality. Institutional leaders can remove barriers (e.g., costs to enroll in mentor training) and facilitate mentorship in non-monetary ways, for example, by making it a formal part of the promotion process. Mentors and leaders must champion these changes, fortifying not only individual career trajectories but also advancing scientific integrity, inclusivity, and justice within the epidemiologic community.

2.
Clin Psychol Psychother ; 24(3): 661-669, 2017 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27503418

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Overly negative appraisals of negative life events characterize depression but patterns of emotion bias associated with life events in depression are not well understood. The goal of this paper is to determine under which situations emotional responses are stronger than expected given life events and which emotions are biased. METHODS: Depressed (n = 16) and non-depressed (n = 14) participants (mean age = 41.4 years) wrote about negative life events involving their own actions and inactions, and rated the current emotion elicited by those events. They also rated emotions elicited by someone else's actions and inactions. These ratings were compared with evaluations provided by a second, 'benchmark' group of non-depressed individuals (n = 20) in order to assess the magnitude and direction of possible biased emotional reactions in the two groups. RESULTS: Participants with depression reported greater anger and disgust than expected in response to both actions and inactions, whereas they reported greater guilt, shame, sadness, responsibility and fear than expected in response to inactions. Relative to non-depressed and benchmark participants, depressed participants were overly negative in the evaluation of their own life events, but not the life events of others. CONCLUSION: A standardized method for establishing emotional bias reveals a pattern of overly negative emotion only in depressed individuals' self-evaluations, and in particular with respect to anger and disgust, lending support to claims that major depressives' evaluations represent negative emotional bias and to clinical interventions that address this bias. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


Assuntos
Transtorno Depressivo Maior/psicologia , Emoções , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Adulto , Ira , Boston , Medo/psicologia , Feminino , Culpa , Humanos , Masculino , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Vergonha
3.
Am Psychol ; 58(4): 269-78, 2003 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12866392

RESUMO

Psychologists' appropriation of language and ideas from Thomas Kuhn's (1962, 1970b) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions reveals deep and contradictory concerns about truth, science, and the progress of the field. The author argues that psychologists, uncomfortably straddling natural and social science traditions, reference Structure for 2 reasons largely overlooked: first, because it presents an intermediate, naturalistic position in the war between relativist and rationalist views of scientific truth, and second, because it presents a psychologized model of scientific change. The author suggests that the history of this mutual influence--psychologists being influenced by Kuhn and vice versa--may usefully inform current practices of psychological science.


Assuntos
Teoria Psicológica , Psicologia/tendências , Previsões , Humanos , Idioma , Psicologia/métodos
4.
Am Psychol ; 59(4): 273-4, 2004.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15149278

RESUMO

Responds to comments made by numerous authors (see records 2004-14303-014, 2004-14303-015, and 2004-14303-016) on the current author's original article (see record 2003-05602-002), which presented an account of why psychologists have almost continuously invoked Kuhn since the 1970s to justify a wide array of the discipline's historical developments and epistemological proclivities. ((c) 2004 APA, all rights reserved)


Assuntos
Teoria Psicológica , Psicologia/tendências , Humanos , Ciência
5.
J Clin Psychol ; 62(3): 387-94, 2006 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16385540

RESUMO

Although clinical theories suggest that people with Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) experience a confused sense of self, little empirical research has directly examined the self in BPD (Heard & Linehan, 1993; Westen & Cohen, 1993). In this study, 43 female participants, 15 with BPD and 28 without BPD, completed the closed-ended version of Markus and Wurf's (1987) Possible Selves Questionnaire (PSQ). Participants with BPD were less likely than controls to endorse positive possible selves as current, but more likely to endorse negative possible selves as current, probable, desired, and important. Participants with BPD linked negative and positive selves to their desired selves, which is consistent with the unstable sense of self characteristic of BPD.


Assuntos
Transtorno da Personalidade Borderline/diagnóstico , Ego , Inventário de Personalidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Transtorno da Personalidade Borderline/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Imaginação , Controle Interno-Externo , Entrevista Psicológica , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Teoria Psicológica , Psicometria/estatística & dados numéricos , Valores de Referência , Autoimagem , Enquadramento Psicológico , Inquéritos e Questionários
6.
Psychol Sci ; 17(8): 649-53, 2006 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16913944

RESUMO

Loss aversion occurs because people expect losses to have greater hedonic impact than gains of equal magnitude. In two studies, people predicted that losses in a gambling task would have greater hedonic impact than would gains of equal magnitude, but when people actually gambled, losses did not have as much of an emotional impact as they predicted. People overestimated the hedonic impact of losses because they underestimated their tendency to rationalize losses and overestimated their tendency to dwell on losses. The asymmetrical impact of losses and gains was thus more a property of affective forecasts than a property of affective experience.


Assuntos
Afeto , Previsões , Jogo de Azar/psicologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa