Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 29
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Infancy ; 29(5): 672-692, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38598268

RESUMO

Caregivers are instrumental in the development of infant emotion regulation; however, few studies have focused on delineating the real-time effectiveness of strategies that caregivers use to reduce infant distress. It is also unclear whether certain caregiver traits facilitate engagement in more successful regulation strategies. This study addressed these gaps by: (1) examining the differential effectiveness of maternal regulatory attempts (MRAs; behavioral strategies initiated by mothers to assist infants with regulating emotional states) in reducing 12- to 24-month-old infants' frustration during a toy removal task; and (2) assessing whether maternal mind-mindedness (mothers' attunement to their infant's mental state) predicted mothers' selection of MRAs. Multilevel modeling revealed that distraction and control were the most effective MRAs in reducing infant negative affect across 5-s intervals (N = 82 dyads; M infant age = 18 months; 45 females). Greater use of non-attuned mind-related speech predicted less engagement in effective MRAs, supporting a link between caregivers' socio-cognitive skills and provision of in-the-moment regulation support. These findings highlight the value of considering caregiver regulatory behaviors as a target for elucidating how maternal socialization of emotion regulation occurs in real-time. They also underscore mothers' important role as socializing agents in the development of this foundational developmental ability.


Assuntos
Regulação Emocional , Comportamento Materno , Relações Mãe-Filho , Humanos , Feminino , Lactente , Masculino , Relações Mãe-Filho/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Adulto , Comportamento do Lactente/fisiologia , Mães/psicologia , Pré-Escolar
2.
Yale J Biol Med ; 97(1): 3-16, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38559463

RESUMO

Social support refers to the help someone receives emotionally or instrumentally from their social network. Poor social support in the perinatal period has been associated with increased risk for symptoms of common mental disorders, including depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTS), which may impact parenting behavior. Whether social support impacts parenting behaviors, independent of mental health symptomatology, remains unclear. Among N=309 participants of the Scaling Up Maternal Mental healthcare by Increasing access to Treatment (SUMMIT Trial), a large perinatal depression and anxiety treatment trial, we explored the relations between perceived social support, perinatal depressive and PTS symptoms, and psychosocial stimulation provided by the parent in their home environment. Social support was measured at baseline using the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS). Perinatal depressive symptoms were measured by the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS) and PTS symptoms were measured by the Abbreviated PTSD Checklist (PCL-6) at baseline, 3-, and 6-months post-randomization. Psychosocial stimulation was assessed by the Home Observation Measurement of the Environment (HOME) when the infant was between 6 to 24 months. Using stepwise hierarchical regressions, we found: (1) perceived social support at baseline significantly predicted both depressive and PTS symptoms at 3-months post-randomization, even when controlling for baseline depressive and PTS symptoms; and (2) while neither depressive nor PTS symptoms were significantly associated with psychosocial stimulation, perceived social support at baseline was a significant predictor. Clinical implications regarding treatment of perinatal patients are discussed.


Assuntos
Depressão Pós-Parto , Feminino , Gravidez , Lactente , Humanos , Depressão Pós-Parto/diagnóstico , Depressão Pós-Parto/etiologia , Depressão Pós-Parto/psicologia , Saúde Mental , Mães/psicologia , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Apoio Social , Depressão/terapia
3.
Psychol Sci ; 33(1): 135-151, 2022 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34919451

RESUMO

Everyday face recognition presents a difficult challenge because faces vary naturally in appearance as a result of changes in lighting, expression, viewing angle, and hairstyle. We know little about how humans develop the ability to learn faces despite natural facial variability. In the current study, we provide the first examination of attentional mechanisms underlying adults' and infants' learning of naturally varying faces. Adults (n = 48) and 6- to 12-month-old infants (n = 48) viewed videos of models reading a storybook; the facial appearance of these models was either high or low in variability. Participants then viewed the learned face paired with a novel face. Infants showed adultlike prioritization of face over nonface regions; both age groups fixated the face region more in the high- than low-variability condition. Overall, however, infants showed less ability to resist contextual distractions during learning, which potentially contributed to their lack of discrimination between the learned and novel faces. Mechanisms underlying face learning across natural variability are discussed.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Aprendizagem , Adulto , Atenção , Face , Humanos , Lactente
4.
Perception ; 51(11): 820-840, 2022 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36154747

RESUMO

Familiar and unfamiliar faces are recognized in fundamentally different ways. One way in which recognition differs is in terms of the features that facilitate recognition: previous studies have shown that familiar face recognition depends more on internal facial features (i.e., eyes, nose and mouth), whereas unfamiliar face recognition depends more on external facial features (i.e., hair, ears and contour). However, very few studies have examined the recognition of faces that vary in both familiarity and race, and the reliance on different facial features, whilst also using faces that incorporate natural within-person variability. In the current study, we used an online version of the card sorting task to assess adults' (n = 258) recognition of faces that varied in familiarity and race when presented with either the whole face, internal features only, or external features only. Adults better recognized familiar faces than unfamiliar faces in both the whole face and the internal features only conditions, but not in the external features only condition. Reasons why adults did not show an own-race advantage in recognition are discussed.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Percepção de Forma , Adulto , Face , Humanos , Reconhecimento Psicológico
5.
Infancy ; 25(5): 658-676, 2020 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32857436

RESUMO

Seven-month-old infants display a robust attentional bias for fearful faces; however, the mechanisms driving this bias remain unclear. The objective of the current study was to replicate the attentional bias for fearful faces and to investigate how infants' online scanning patterns relate to this preference. Infants' visual scanning patterns toward fearful and happy faces were captured using eye tracking in a paired-preference task, specifically exploring if the fear preference is driven by increased attention to particular facial features. Infants allocated increased attention toward the fearful face compared to the happy face overall, thus successfully replicating the attentional bias, and greater attention toward the fearful eyes was associated with a greater magnitude of the fear preference. The current findings suggest that the fearful eyes are a salient facial feature in capturing infants' attention toward the fearful face and that increased scanning of the fearful eyes may be one mechanism driving the overall fear preference. In addition, scanning patterns, and attention to critical features specifically, are highlighted as a strategy for examining the mechanisms underlying the development of emotion recognition abilities in infancy.


Assuntos
Viés de Atenção/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Comportamento do Lactente/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Tecnologia de Rastreamento Ocular , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
6.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 182: 102-113, 2019 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30818225

RESUMO

Adults are less accurate at recognizing emotions expressed by individuals from a different cultural background. However, the research with children is less clear; whereas some studies suggest better emotion recognition for own-race and own-culture faces, others have found no such relationship. The current study examined the influence of race on emotion recognition in children and adults who share a cultural background (i.e., Canadian). Based on previous studies, we hypothesized that participants would demonstrate better emotion recognition for own-race faces. We also hypothesized that emotion recognition would improve across the lifespan (from childhood to adulthood) and as a function of emotion, such that recognition would be better for happy faces than for the other emotions. Children (n = 69; ages 6-10 years; 41 female) and adults (n = 82; mean age = 19.94 years; 72 female) of Western European or South Asian descent were asked to complete a five-alternative forced-choice emotion recognition task (anger, fear, happiness, sadness, and neutral) in which expressions were displayed by White and South Asian faces. As predicted, adults performed better than older children, who performed better than younger children, and participants performed best on happy faces. South Asian participants, but not White participants, performed better when judging own-race faces compared with other-race faces. This finding only partially supports an own-race bias in emotion recognition and may reflect the tendency in the literature to conflate culture and race. More studies are needed to understand cross-race emotion recognition when individuals share the same culture.


Assuntos
Povo Asiático/psicologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , População Branca/psicologia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Povo Asiático/etnologia , Canadá/etnologia , Criança , Cultura , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , População Branca/etnologia , Adulto Jovem
7.
Dev Psychobiol ; 59(4): 507-514, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28369808

RESUMO

Infants may recognize facial expressions of emotion more readily when familiar faces express the emotions. Studies 1 and 2 investigated whether familiarity influences two metrics of emotion processing: Categorization and spontaneous preference. In Study 1 (n = 32), we replicated previous findings showing an asymmetrical pattern of categorization of happy and fearful faces in 6.5-month-old infants, and extended these findings by demonstrating that infants' categorization did not differ when emotions were expressed by familiar (i.e., caregiver) faces. In Study 2 (n = 34), we replicated the spontaneous preference for fearful over happy expressions in 6.5-month-old infants, and extended these findings by demonstrating that the spontaneous preference for fear was also present for familiar faces. Thus, infants' performance on two metrics of emotion processing did not differ depending on face familiarity.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
8.
Dev Sci ; 18(2): 298-313, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25039290

RESUMO

We tested the capacity to perceive visual expressions of emotion, and to use those expressions as guides to social decisions, in three groups of 8- to 10-year-old Romanian children: children abandoned to institutions then randomly assigned to remain in 'care as usual' (institutional care); children abandoned to institutions then randomly assigned to a foster care intervention; and community children who had never been institutionalized. Experiment 1 examined children's recognition of happy, sad, fearful, and angry facial expressions that varied in intensity. Children assigned to institutional care had higher thresholds for identifying happy expressions than foster care or community children, but did not differ in their thresholds for identifying the other facial expressions. Moreover, the error rates of the three groups of children were the same for all of the facial expressions. Experiment 2 examined children's ability to use facial expressions of emotion to guide social decisions about whom to befriend and whom to help. Children assigned to institutional care were less accurate than foster care or community children at deciding whom to befriend; however, the groups did not differ in their ability to decide whom to help. Overall, although there were group differences in some abilities, all three groups of children performed well across tasks. The results are discussed in the context of theoretical accounts of the development of emotion processing.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil , Criança Institucionalizada/psicologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Análise de Variância , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Comportamento Social
9.
Dev Psychobiol ; 56(2): 249-61, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24285109

RESUMO

Exposure to faces is known to shape and change the face processing system; however, no study has yet documented infants' natural daily first-hand exposure to faces. One- and three-month-old infants' visual experience was recorded through head-mounted cameras. The video recordings were coded for faces to determine: (1) How often are infants exposed to faces? (2) To what type of faces are they exposed? and (3) Do frequently encountered face types reflect infants' typical pattern of perceptual narrowing? As hypothesized, infants spent a large proportion of their time (25%) exposed to faces; these faces were primarily female (70%), own-race (96%), and adult-age (81%). Infants were exposed to more individual exemplars of female, own-race, and adult-age faces than to male, other-race, and child- or older-adult-age faces. Each exposure to own-race faces was longer than to other-race faces. There were no differences in exposure duration related to the gender or age of the face. Previous research has found that the face types frequently experienced by our participants are preferred over and more successfully recognized than other face types. The patterns of face exposure revealed in the current study coincide with the known trajectory of perceptual narrowing seen later in infancy.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Face , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa
10.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 8950, 2022 05 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35624118

RESUMO

People often find it more difficult to recognize other- than own-race faces. This other-race effect is robust across numerous ethnic groups. Yet, it remains unclear how this effect changes in people who live in a multiracial environment, and in immigrants whose lifetime perceptual experience changes over time. In the present study, we developed a novel face recognition test that approximates face recognition in the real world. We tested five groups of White and East Asian adults (n = 120) living in racially homogeneous versus heterogeneous cities and East Asians who immigrated to a multiracial city between infancy and adulthood. Multiracial cities reduce the other-race effect. The magnitude of the other-race effect changes as a function of experience, mirroring the racial diversity in perceivers' living environment. Our study highlights the challenge of forming reliable face representations across naturalistic facial variability and suggests a facilitative role of multiracial environments in eliminating the other-race effect.


Assuntos
Face , Aprendizagem , Adulto , Cidades , Humanos , Grupos Raciais , Reconhecimento Psicológico
11.
Dev Psychobiol ; 53(4): 416-24, 2011 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21484782

RESUMO

Early experiences contribute powerfully to the development of neural systems that underlie various perceptual and cognitive abilities in humans. In one of the first studies to systematically control infants' exposure to a familiar object, we examined the effects of controlled experience on the neural correlates of visual recognition in two groups of infants. One group received 1 month of in-home familiarization to a 3D model of a female face. Another group received 1.5 min of in-lab familiarization to the 3D model of a female face, creating two conditions that differed in the amount and, importantly, the context of exposure to a familiar stimulus. Following familiarization, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded at 3 months of age while infants viewed pictures of the familiar face and an unfamiliar face. Results demonstrated that while both groups of infants discriminated between the familiar and unfamiliar faces, the pattern of neural processing was reversed for the two groups. Thus, the amount and context of visual exposure altered the neural correlates of recognition processes in young infants.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Eletroencefalografia , Face , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa
12.
Front Psychol ; 12: 733275, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34721201

RESUMO

The closure of in-person laboratories and decreased safety of face-to-face interactions resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic jeopardized the ability of many developmental researchers to continue data collection during this time. Disruptions in data collection are particularly damaging to longitudinal studies, in which the testing of different age groups occurs on a continuous basis, and data loss at one time point can have cascading effects across subsequent time points and threaten the viability of the study. In an effort to continue collecting data for a longitudinal study on emotion development started in-person pre-pandemic, we adapted two parent-infant interaction tasks (free-play task and toy removal task) for a remote testing framework. Our procedure for pivoting these tasks to a supervised, remote online testing framework is outlined and the associated strengths and challenges of testing in this format (e.g., feasibility and implementation, testing environment and task setup validity, and accessibility, recruitment, and diversity) are critically evaluated. Considerations for applying this framework to other behavioral tasks are discussed and recommendations are provided.

13.
Infant Behav Dev ; 65: 101630, 2021 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34418795

RESUMO

In this study, we examined whether infant temperament predicted study dropout at 3.5 and 7 months and whether dropout was stable across time. Dropout was measured across four experimental tasks (free-play, ERP, still-face, and eye tracking). Temperament was not related to dropout at either timepoint. Dropout was not stable across time, nor was it stable across tasks. These findings suggest that individual differences in temperament are not systematically related to study completion across experimental tasks with varied requirements. These findings additionally suggest that dropout is not consistent across tasks, which may support the utility of multi-study data collection methods.


Assuntos
Individualidade , Temperamento , Humanos , Lactente
14.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 42: 100759, 2020 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32072932

RESUMO

An important feature of the development of emotion recognition in infants is the emergence of a robust attentional bias for fearful faces. There is some debate about when this enhanced sensitivity to fearful expressions develops. The current study explored whether 3-month-olds demonstrate differential behavioral and neural responding to happy and fearful faces. Three-month-old infants (n = 69) participated in a behavioral task that assessed whether they show a visual preference for fearful faces and an event-related potential (ERP) task that assessed their neural responses to fearful and happy faces. Infants showed a looking preference for fearful over happy faces. They also showed differential neural responding over occiptotemporal regions that have been implicated in face perception (i.e., N290, P400), but not over frontocentral regions that have been implicated in attentional processes (i.e., Nc). These findings suggest that 3-month-olds display an early perceptual sensitivity to fearful faces, which may presage the emergence of the attentional bias for fearful faces in older infants. Tracking the ontogeny of this phenomenon is necessary to understand its relationship with later developmental outcomes.


Assuntos
Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Medo/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino
15.
Brain Sci ; 10(9)2020 Aug 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32847037

RESUMO

Infants' visual processing of emotion undergoes significant development across the first year of life, yet our knowledge regarding the mechanisms underlying these advances is limited. Additionally, infant emotion processing is commonly examined using static faces, which do not accurately depict real-world emotional displays. The goal of this study was to characterize 7-month-olds' visual scanning strategies when passively viewing dynamic emotional expressions to examine whether infants modify their scanning patterns depending on the emotion. Eye-tracking measures revealed differential attention towards the critical features (eyes, mouth) of expressions. The eyes captured the greatest attention for angry and neutral faces, and the mouth captured the greatest attention for happy faces. A time-course analysis further elucidated at what point during the trial differential scanning patterns emerged. The current results suggest that 7-month-olds are sensitive to the critical features of emotional expressions and scan them differently depending on the emotion. The scanning patterns presented in this study may serve as a link to understanding how infants begin to differentiate between expressions in the context of emotion recognition.

16.
Data Brief ; 29: 105070, 2020 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32071956

RESUMO

This dataset represents face experience coded frame-by-frame from nearly 170 hours of infant-perspective head-mounted-camera video, recorded during their daily life by 40 3-month-old infants. It includes information about the identity of the face (e.g., caregiver, relative), length of time the face was in the field of view, location in which the face occurred, and descriptions of the situation in which the infant experienced the face. Demographic information (e.g., age, gender) about the infants who recorded the videos is also provided. For elaboration on data collection methodology, interpretation, analysis, and discussion of early face experience captured by this dataset, please see our paper These are the people in your neighbourhood: Consistency and persistence in infants' exposure to caregivers', relatives', and strangers' faces across contexts [1].

17.
Child Dev ; 80(4): 1039-56, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19630892

RESUMO

Data are reported from 3 groups of children residing in Bucharest, Romania. Face recognition in currently institutionalized, previously institutionalized, and never-institutionalized children was assessed at 3 time points: preintervention (n = 121), 30 months of age (n = 99), and 42 months of age (n = 77). Children watched photographs of caregiver and stranger faces while event-related potentials were recorded. Results demonstrate that institutionalized children show pervasive cortical hypoarousal in response to faces and that foster care is somewhat effective in remediating this deficit by 42 months of age. All 3 groups of children distinguished between the familiar and unfamiliar faces. These results have the potential to inform an understanding of the role of early experience in the development of the neural systems that subserve face recognition.


Assuntos
Criança Institucionalizada , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Face , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Piscadela , Pré-Escolar , Estudos Transversais , Eletroencefalografia , Eletroculografia , Expressão Facial , Família/psicologia , Feminino , Cuidados no Lar de Adoção , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Lobo Occipital/fisiologia , Estudos Retrospectivos , Romênia , Percepção Visual
18.
Dev Psychol ; 45(1): 17-30, 2009 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19209987

RESUMO

To examine the neurobiological consequences of early institutionalization, the authors recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) from 3 groups of Romanian children--currently institutionalized, previously institutionalized but randomly assigned to foster care, and family-reared children--in response to pictures of happy, angry, fearful, and sad facial expressions of emotion. At 3 assessments (baseline, 30 months, and 42 months), institutionalized children showed markedly smaller amplitudes and longer latencies for the occipital components P1, N170, and P400 compared to family-reared children. By 42 months, ERP amplitudes and latencies of children placed in foster care were intermediate between the institutionalized and family-reared children, suggesting that foster care may be partially effective in ameliorating adverse neural changes caused by institutionalization. The age at which children were placed into foster care was unrelated to their ERP outcomes at 42 months. Facial emotion processing was similar in all 3 groups of children; specifically, fearful faces elicited larger amplitude and longer latency responses than happy faces for the frontocentral components P250 and Nc. These results have important implications for understanding of the role that experience plays in shaping the developing brain.


Assuntos
Emoções/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados Visuais/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Lobo Occipital/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Pré-Escolar , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Institucionalização , Masculino , Neurobiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação
19.
Vision Res ; 157: 230-241, 2019 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30291919

RESUMO

Faces are a frequent part of young infants' visual environments. Three-month-old infants spend approximately 25% of their time exposed to faces (Sugden, Mohamed-Ali, & Moulson, 2014; Jayaraman, Fausey, & Smith, 2015). These faces belong primarily to familiar people, like their caregivers, and are heavily weighted towards female, adult-aged, and own-race faces. To date, descriptions of infants' exposure to faces have focused on frequency-both overall frequency and relative frequency of different face types (e.g., familiar vs. unfamiliar; own-race vs. other-race). It is less clear whether faces of different types distinguish themselves in other ways that have implications for infants' learning. Here, we move beyond an evaluation of frequency to determine the dimensions by which familiar faces (i.e., caregivers and relatives, as identified by parental report) differentiate themselves from unfamiliar (i.e., stranger's) faces in the infant's early visual environment. Measuring infants' natural visual ecology with head-mounted infant-perspective cameras, we found that 3-month-olds were exposed to faces 21% of the time. The primary caregiver was the most frequent face (44% of exposure time) and non-primary caregivers were often second most frequent (17% of exposure time). Caregiver faces also distinguished themselves by their consistency across both contexts and time. For example, the primary caregiver's face was most likely to appear across contexts and locations. Primary caregiver faces were less likely to persist in the field of view, as compared to non-primary caregiver and stranger faces. Thus, the socially important faces in the infant's visual environment distinguish themselves not only through their overall frequency, but also through their consistency across contexts. This has implications for understanding how the early visual environment shapes learning about faces.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Cuidadores , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Análise de Regressão , Gravação em Vídeo
20.
PLoS One ; 13(2): e0192418, 2018.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29474367

RESUMO

Emotion recognition is important for social interaction and communication, yet previous research has identified a cross-cultural emotion recognition deficit: Recognition is less accurate for emotions expressed by individuals from a cultural group different than one's own. The current study examined whether social categorization based on race, in the absence of cultural differences, influences emotion recognition in a diverse context. South Asian and White Canadians in the Greater Toronto Area completed an emotion recognition task that required them to identify the seven basic emotional expressions when posed by members of the same two groups, allowing us to tease apart the contributions of culture and social group membership. Contrary to our hypothesis, there was no mutual in-group advantage in emotion recognition: Participants were not more accurate at recognizing emotions posed by their respective racial in-groups. Both groups were more accurate at recognizing expressions when posed by South Asian faces, and White participants were more accurate overall compared to South Asian participants. These results suggest that in a diverse environment, categorization based on race alone does not lead to the creation of social out-groups in a way that negatively impacts emotion recognition.


Assuntos
Emoções , Grupos Raciais , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ontário , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa