Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 66
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Psychol Sci ; 35(3): 250-262, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38289294

RESUMO

Fundamental frequency ( fo) is the most perceptually salient vocal acoustic parameter, yet little is known about how its perceptual influence varies across societies. We examined how fo affects key social perceptions and how socioecological variables modulate these effects in 2,647 adult listeners sampled from 44 locations across 22 nations. Low male fo increased men's perceptions of formidability and prestige, especially in societies with higher homicide rates and greater relational mobility in which male intrasexual competition may be more intense and rapid identification of high-status competitors may be exigent. High female fo increased women's perceptions of flirtatiousness where relational mobility was lower and threats to mating relationships may be greater. These results indicate that the influence of fo on social perceptions depends on socioecological variables, including those related to competition for status and mates.


Assuntos
Voz , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Homicídio , Percepção Social , Parceiros Sexuais
2.
Psychol Sci ; 33(3): 343-353, 2022 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35191784

RESUMO

The contributions of gonadal hormones to the development of human behavioral sex differences are subjects of intense scientific and social interest. Isolated gonadotropin-releasing-hormone deficiency (IGD) is a rare endocrine disorder that can reveal a possible role of early gonadal hormones. IGD is characterized by low or absent gonadal hormone production after the first trimester of gestation, but external genitalia and hence gender of rearing are concordant with chromosomal and gonadal sex. We investigated recalled childhood gender nonconformity in men (n = 65) and women (n = 32) with IGD and typically developing men (n = 463) and women (n = 1,207). Men with IGD showed elevated childhood gender nonconformity, particularly if they also reported undescended testes at birth, a marker of low perinatal androgens. Women with IGD did not differ from typically developing women. These results indicate that early androgen exposure after the first trimester contributes to male-typical gender-role behaviors in childhood.


Assuntos
Androgênios , Identidade de Gênero , Feminino , Humanos , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Rememoração Mental
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(5): 1633-1638, 2019 01 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30647112

RESUMO

Recent studies have called into question the idea that facial masculinity is a condition-dependent male ornament that indicates immunocompetence in humans. We add to this growing body of research by calculating an objective measure of facial masculinity/femininity using 3D images in a large sample (n = 1,233) of people of European ancestry. We show that facial masculinity is positively correlated with adult height in both males and females. However, facial masculinity scales with growth similarly in males and females, suggesting that facial masculinity is not exclusively a male ornament, as male ornaments are typically more sensitive to growth in males compared with females. Additionally, we measured immunocompetence via heterozygosity at the major histocompatibility complex (MHC), a widely-used genetic marker of immunity. We show that, while height is positively correlated with MHC heterozygosity, facial masculinity is not. Thus, facial masculinity does not reflect immunocompetence measured by MHC heterozygosity in humans. Overall, we find no support for the idea that facial masculinity is a condition-dependent male ornament that has evolved to indicate immunocompetence.


Assuntos
Face/fisiologia , Complexo Principal de Histocompatibilidade/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Beleza , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Feminino , Heterozigoto , Humanos , Imunocompetência/fisiologia , Masculino , Masculinidade , Fenômenos Fisiológicos/fisiologia , Caracteres Sexuais , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 45: e130, 2022 07 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35875966

RESUMO

Benenson et al. postulate that human females evolved unique survival adaptations to facilitate maternal and grandmaternal care. This hypothesis is consistent with the broader hypothesis that female phenotypes are more ecologically optimal, but further evidence is needed to make a compelling case that sex differences in self-protection are not primarily the result of more intense sexual selection on males.


Assuntos
Adaptação Biológica , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuais
5.
Horm Behav ; 127: 104886, 2021 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33202246

RESUMO

Circulating gonadal hormones have been linked to variation in the structure and function of the adult human brain, raising the question of how cognition is affected by sex hormones in adulthood. The impacts of progestogens and estrogens are of special interest due to the widespread use of hormone supplementation. Multiple studies have analyzed relationships between ovarian hormones and mental rotation performance, one of the largest known cognitive sex differences; however, results are conflicting. These discrepancies are likely due in part to modest sample sizes and reliance on self-report measures to assess menstrual cycle phase. The present study aimed to clarify the impact of progestogens and estrogens on visuospatial cognition by relating mental rotation task performance to salivary hormone concentrations. Across two studies totaling 528 naturally-cycling premenopausal women, an internal meta-analysis suggested a small, positive effect of within-subjects changes in progesterone on MRT performance (estimate = 0.44, p = 0.014), though this result should be interpreted with caution given multiple statistical analyses. Between-subjects differences and within-subject changes in estradiol did not significantly predict MRT. These results shed light on the potential cognitive effects of endogenous and exogenous hormone action, and the proximate mechanisms modulating spatial cognition.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/metabolismo , Ciclo Menstrual , Adolescente , Adulto , Estradiol/análise , Estradiol/metabolismo , Feminino , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/análise , Humanos , Ciclo Menstrual/metabolismo , Ciclo Menstrual/psicologia , Progesterona/análise , Progesterona/metabolismo , Saliva/química , Saliva/metabolismo , Caracteres Sexuais , Memória Espacial/fisiologia , Navegação Espacial/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
6.
Horm Behav ; 119: 104647, 2020 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31778719

RESUMO

Relatively little is known about the effects of endogenous and exogenous steroid hormones on ecologically relevant behavioral and cognitive phenotypes in women, such as emotion recognition, despite the widespread use of steroid hormone-altering hormonal contraceptives (HCs). Though some previous studies have examined the effect of HC use, estradiol, progesterone, and testosterone on emotion recognition in women, they have been limited by cross-sectional designs, small sample sizes (total n < 100), and compromised statistical power to detect significant effects. Using data from two test sessions in a large sample of naturally cycling women (NC; n = 192) and women on HCs (n = 203), we found no group differences in emotion recognition; further, the lack of group differences in emotion recognition was not modulated by item difficulty or emotional valence. Among NC women who provided saliva samples across two sessions that were assayed for estradiol and progesterone concentrations, we found no compelling evidence across models that between-subject differences and within-subject fluctuations in these ovarian hormones predicted emotion recognition accuracy, with the exception that between-subjects estradiol negatively predicted emotion recognition for emotions of neutral valence (p = .042). Among HC women who provided saliva samples across two sessions that were assayed for testosterone, we found no compelling evidence that between-subjects differences and within-subject fluctuations in testosterone predicted emotion recognition accuracy. Overall, our analyses provide little support for the idea that circulating endogenous or exogenous ovarian hormones influence emotion recognition in women.


Assuntos
Anticoncepcionais Orais Hormonais/farmacologia , Inteligência Emocional/efeitos dos fármacos , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/metabolismo , Reconhecimento Psicológico/efeitos dos fármacos , Adulto , Estudos Transversais , Inteligência Emocional/fisiologia , Emoções , Estradiol/análise , Estradiol/metabolismo , Feminino , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/análise , Humanos , Ovário/efeitos dos fármacos , Ovário/metabolismo , Progesterona/análise , Progesterona/metabolismo , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Saliva/química , Saliva/metabolismo , Testosterona/análise , Testosterona/metabolismo , Adulto Jovem
7.
Horm Behav ; 121: 104712, 2020 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32059854

RESUMO

Experiments in male rodents demonstrate that sensitivity to the organizational effects of steroid hormones decreases across the pubertal window, with earlier androgen exposure leading to greater masculinization of the brain and behavior. Similarly, some research suggests the timing of peripubertal exposure to sex steroids influences aspects of human psychology, including visuospatial cognition. However, prior studies have been limited by small samples and/or imprecise measures of pubertal timing. We conducted 4 studies to clarify whether the timing of peripubertal hormone exposure predicts performance on male-typed tests of spatial cognition in adulthood. In Studies 1 (n = 1095) and 2 (n = 173), we investigated associations between recalled pubertal age and spatial cognition in typically developing men, controlling for current testosterone levels in Study 2. In Study 3 (n = 51), we examined the relationship between spatial performance and the age at which peripubertal hormone replacement therapy was initiated in a sample of men with Isolated GnRH Deficiency. Across Studies 1-3, effect size estimates for the relationship between spatial performance and pubertal timing ranged from. -0.04 and -0.27, and spatial performance was unrelated to salivary testosterone in Study 2. In Study 4, we conducted two meta-analyses of Studies 1-3 and four previously published studies. The first meta-analysis was conducted on correlations between spatial performance and measures of the absolute age of pubertal timing, and the second replaced those correlations with correlations between spatial performance and measures of relative pubertal timing where available. Point estimates for correlations between pubertal timing and spatial cognition were -0.15 and -0.12 (both p < 0.001) in the first and second meta-analyses, respectively. These associations were robust to the exclusion of any individual study. Our results suggest that, for some aspects of neural development, sensitivity to gonadal hormones declines across puberty, with earlier pubertal hormone exposure predicting greater sex-typicality in psychological phenotypes in adulthood. These results shed light on the processes of behavioral and brain organization and have implications for the treatment of IGD and other conditions wherein pubertal timing is pharmacologically manipulated.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/fisiologia , Puberdade/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Esteroides/sangue , Adolescente , Desenvolvimento do Adolescente/fisiologia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Animais , Encéfalo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Criança , Feminino , Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/sangue , Humanos , Masculino , Neurogênese/fisiologia , Puberdade/sangue , Puberdade/psicologia , Maturidade Sexual/fisiologia , Esteroides/fisiologia , Testosterona/sangue , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
8.
Horm Behav ; 115: 104562, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31356808

RESUMO

Past work demonstrates that humans behave differently towards women across their menstrual cycles, even after exclusively visual exposure to women's faces. People may look at women's faces differently as a function of women's menstrual cycles. Analyses of participants' scanpaths (eye movement patterns) while they looked at women at different phases of their menstrual cycles revealed that observers exhibit more consistent scanpaths when examining women's faces when women are in a menstrual cycle phase that typically corresponds with peak fertility, whereas they exhibit more variable patterns when looking at women's faces when they are in phases that do not correspond with fertility. A multivariate classifier on participants' scanpaths predicted whether they were looking at the face of a woman in a more typically fertile- versus non-fertile-phase of her menstrual cycle with above-chance accuracy. These findings demonstrate that people look at women's faces differently as a function of women's menstrual cycles, and suggest that people are sensitive to fluctuating visual cues associated with women's menstrual cycle phase.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Face/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Fertilidade/fisiologia , Ciclo Menstrual/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Mulheres , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
9.
Horm Behav ; 97: 3-4, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28818501

RESUMO

Human genetic males are unlike rodent males in that neither the ability to convert testosterone to estrogen nor a functional estrogen receptor (ER) appears necessary for male-typical behavior, but a functional androgen receptor (AR) is required. Brain masculinization is probably mainly AR-mediated in human genetic males. ER binding may nevertheless have important masculinizing or defeminizing effects in human genetic females. Probably the strongest available evidence on this issue is derived from females exposed to synthetic estrogens in utero due to their mother's treatment with DES. As we review, the totality of evidence from this population indicates little or no effect of estrogens on sexuality in genetic females. In addition, if brain masculinization were ER-mediated in humans, it seems unlikely that sex hormone-binding globulin would bind estrogens so effectively as to prevent them from masculinizing the brain. In sum, current evidence suggests that estrogen plays a limited role in masculinizing the human brain and behavior.


Assuntos
Receptores Androgênicos , Receptores de Estrogênio , Encéfalo , Estrogênios , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testosterona
10.
Horm Behav ; 103: 45-53, 2018 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29864418

RESUMO

A robust body of research has demonstrated shifts in women's sexual desire and arousal across the menstrual cycle, with heightened desire and arousal coincident with heightened probability of conception (POC), and it is likely that ovarian hormones modulate these shifts. However, studies in which women are exposed to audiovisual sexual stimuli (AVSS) at high POC (mid-follicular) and low POC (luteal) phases have failed to detect significant differences in genital or subjective arousal patterns based on menstrual cycle phase. Here, we tested whether hormonal responsivity to AVSS differs as a function of cycle phase at testing, and whether phase during which participants were first exposed to AVSS influences hormonal responsivity in subsequent test sessions. Twenty-two naturally cycling heterosexual women were exposed to AVSS during the follicular and luteal phases, with phase at first test session counterbalanced across participants. Salivary samples were collected before and after AVSS exposure. Estradiol increased significantly during both follicular and luteal phase sessions, and increases were higher during the follicular phase. Testosterone (T) increased significantly only during the follicular phase session, while progesterone (P) did not change significantly during either cycle phase. Session order and current cycle phase interacted to predict P and T responses, such that P and T increased during the follicular phase in women who were first tested during the luteal phase. These data suggest that menstrual cycle phase influences hormonal responsivity to AVSS, and contribute to a growing body of clinical and empirical literature on the neuroendocrine modulators of women's sexuality.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/fisiologia , Ciclo Menstrual/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudos Transversais , Estradiol/sangue , Feminino , Fase Folicular/fisiologia , Heterossexualidade , Humanos , Libido/fisiologia , Fase Luteal/fisiologia , Indução da Ovulação , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Progesterona/sangue , Testosterona/sangue , Gravação em Vídeo , Adulto Jovem
11.
Horm Behav ; 106: 122-134, 2018 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30342884

RESUMO

Are estrous mate preference shifts robust? This question is the subject of controversy within human evolutionary sciences. For nearly two decades, mate preference shifts across the ovulatory cycle were considered an important feature of human sexual selection, directing women's attention toward mates with indicators of "good genes" in their fertile phase, when conception is possible. However, several recent studies on masculine faces, bodies and behaviors did not find evidence supporting this account, known as the good genes ovulatory shift hypothesis. Furthermore, evidence that preferences for masculine characteristics in men's voices are related to women's cycle phase and hormonal status is still equivocal. Here, we report two independent within-subject studies from different labs with large sample sizes (N = 202 tested twice in Study 1; N = 157 tested four times in Study 2) investigating cycle shifts in women's preferences for masculine voices. In both studies, hormonal status was assessed directly using salivary assays of steroid hormones. We did not find evidence for effects of cycle phase, conception risk, or steroid hormone levels on women's preferences for masculine voices. Rather, our studies partially provide evidence for cycle shifts in women's general attraction to men's voices regardless of masculine characteristics. Women's relationship status and self-reported stress did not moderate these findings, and the hormonal pattern that influences these shifts remains somewhat unclear. We consider how future work can clarify the mechanisms underlying psychological changes across the ovulatory cycle.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha , Masculinidade , Ciclo Menstrual/fisiologia , Voz , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Casamento/psicologia , Ciclo Menstrual/psicologia , Ovulação/fisiologia , Ovulação/psicologia , Reprodução/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Parceiros Sexuais/psicologia , Voz/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
12.
Evol Hum Behav ; 38(2): 249-258, 2017 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34629843

RESUMO

Fluctuating asymmetry (FA), deviation from perfect bilateral symmetry, is thought to reflect an organism's relative inability to maintain stable morphological development in the face of environmental and genetic stressors. Previous research has documented negative relationships between FA and attractiveness judgments in humans, but scant research has explored relationships between the human voice and this putative marker of genetic quality in either sex. Only one study (and in women only) has explored relationships between vocal attractiveness and asymmetry of the face, a feature-rich trait space central in prior work on human genetic quality and mate choice. We therefore examined this relationship in three studies comprising 231 men and 240 women from two Western samples as well as Hadza hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Voice recordings were collected and rated for attractiveness, and FA was computed from two-dimensional facial images as well as, for a subset of men, three-dimensional facial scans. Through meta-analysis of our results and those of prior studies, we found a negative association between FA and vocal attractiveness that was highly robust and statistically significant whether we included effect sizes from previously published work, or only those from the present research, and regardless of the inclusion of any individual sample or method of assessing FA (e.g., facial or limb FA). Weighted mean correlations between FA and vocal attractiveness across studies were -.23 for men and -.29 for women. This research thus offers strong support for the hypothesis that voices provide cues to genetic quality in humans.

13.
Horm Behav ; 91: 19-35, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27866819

RESUMO

Hormones orchestrate and coordinate human female sexual development, sexuality, and reproduction in relation to three types of phenotypic changes: life history transitions such as puberty and childbirth, responses to contextual factors such as caloric intake and stress, and cyclical patterns such as the ovulatory cycle. Here, we review the endocrinology underlying women's reproductive phenotypes, including sexual orientation and gender identity, mate preferences, competition for mates, sex drive, and maternal behavior. We highlight distinctive aspects of women's sexuality such as the possession of sexual ornaments, relatively cryptic fertile windows, extended sexual behavior across the ovulatory cycle, and a period of midlife reproductive senescence-and we focus on how hormonal mechanisms were shaped by selection to produce adaptive outcomes. We conclude with suggestions for future research to elucidate how hormonal mechanisms subserve women's reproductive phenotypes.


Assuntos
Hormônios Esteroides Gonadais/metabolismo , Reprodução/fisiologia , Comportamento Reprodutivo/fisiologia , Sexualidade/fisiologia , Endocrinologia , Feminino , Identidade de Gênero , Humanos , Libido/fisiologia , Casamento , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Maturidade Sexual/fisiologia , Parceiros Sexuais
15.
PLoS Genet ; 10(3): e1004224, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24651127

RESUMO

Human facial diversity is substantial, complex, and largely scientifically unexplained. We used spatially dense quasi-landmarks to measure face shape in population samples with mixed West African and European ancestry from three locations (United States, Brazil, and Cape Verde). Using bootstrapped response-based imputation modeling (BRIM), we uncover the relationships between facial variation and the effects of sex, genomic ancestry, and a subset of craniofacial candidate genes. The facial effects of these variables are summarized as response-based imputed predictor (RIP) variables, which are validated using self-reported sex, genomic ancestry, and observer-based facial ratings (femininity and proportional ancestry) and judgments (sex and population group). By jointly modeling sex, genomic ancestry, and genotype, the independent effects of particular alleles on facial features can be uncovered. Results on a set of 20 genes showing significant effects on facial features provide support for this approach as a novel means to identify genes affecting normal-range facial features and for approximating the appearance of a face from genetic markers.


Assuntos
DNA/genética , Face/anatomia & histologia , Genótipo , População Negra , Brasil , Etnicidade , Feminino , Genética Populacional , Humanos , Estados Unidos , População Branca/genética
16.
Proc Biol Sci ; 283(1829)2016 04 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27122553

RESUMO

In many primates, including humans, the vocalizations of males and females differ dramatically, with male vocalizations and vocal anatomy often seeming to exaggerate apparent body size. These traits may be favoured by sexual selection because low-frequency male vocalizations intimidate rivals and/or attract females, but this hypothesis has not been systematically tested across primates, nor is it clear why competitors and potential mates should attend to vocalization frequencies. Here we show across anthropoids that sexual dimorphism in fundamental frequency (F0) increased during evolutionary transitions towards polygyny, and decreased during transitions towards monogamy. Surprisingly, humans exhibit greater F0 sexual dimorphism than any other ape. We also show that low-F0 vocalizations predict perceptions of men's dominance and attractiveness, and predict hormone profiles (low cortisol and high testosterone) related to immune function. These results suggest that low male F0 signals condition to competitors and mates, and evolved in male anthropoids in response to the intensity of mating competition.


Assuntos
Haplorrinos/fisiologia , Preferência de Acasalamento Animal/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual Animal/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Vocalização Animal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Animais , Evolução Biológica , Tamanho Corporal/fisiologia , Feminino , Haplorrinos/anatomia & histologia , Haplorrinos/classificação , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/fisiologia , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Filogenia , Caracteres Sexuais , Testosterona/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
17.
Arch Sex Behav ; 45(8): 2091-2100, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26581567

RESUMO

Polymorphisms in the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) have previously been shown to associate with a variety of human behavioral phenotypes, including ADHD pathology, alcohol and tobacco craving, financial risk-taking in males, and broader personality traits such as novelty seeking. Recent research has linked the presence of a 7-repeat (7R) allele in a 48-bp variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) along exon III of DRD4 to age at first sexual intercourse, sexual desire, arousal and function, and infidelity and promiscuity. We hypothesized that carriers of longer DRD4 alleles may report interest in a wider variety of sexual behaviors and experiences than noncarriers. Participants completed a 37-item questionnaire measuring sexual interests as well as Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory, and were genotyped for the 48-bp VNTR on exon III of DRD4. Based on our final genotyped sample of female (n = 139) and male (n = 115) participants, we found that 7R carriers reported interest in a wider variety of sexual behaviors (r = 0.16) within a young adult heterosexual sample of European descent. To our knowledge, this is the first reported association between DRD4 exon III VNTR genotype and interest in a variety of sexual behaviors. We discuss these findings within the context of DRD4 research and broader trends in human evolutionary history.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta/genética , Polimorfismo Genético , Receptores de Dopamina D4/genética , Comportamento Sexual , Adulto , Alelos , Éxons , Feminino , Genótipo , Humanos , Masculino , Repetições Minissatélites , Razão de Chances , Autorrelato , Temperamento
18.
Horm Behav ; 71: 16-21, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25857930

RESUMO

The human face displays a wealth of information, including information about dominance and fecundity. Dominance and fecundity are also associated with lower concentrations of the stress hormone cortisol, suggesting that cortisol may negatively predict facial dominance and attractiveness. We digitally photographed 61 women's faces, had these images rated by men and women for dominance, attractiveness, and femininity, and explored relationships between these perceptions and women's salivary cortisol concentrations. In a first study, we found that women with more dominant-appearing, but not more attractive, faces had lower cortisol levels. These associations were not due to age, ethnicity, time since waking, testosterone, or its interaction with cortisol. In a second study, composite images of women with low cortisol were perceived as more dominant than those of women with high cortisol significantly more often than chance by two samples of viewers, with a similar but non-significant trend in a third sample. However, data on perceptions of attractiveness were mixed; low-cortisol images were viewed as more attractive by two samples of US viewers and as less attractive by a sample of Mexican viewers. Our results suggest that having a more dominant-appearing face may be associated with lower stress and hence lower cortisol in women, and provide further evidence regarding the information content of the human face.


Assuntos
Face , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Predomínio Social , Adolescente , Feminino , Feminilidade , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Desejabilidade Social , Percepção Social , Testosterona/metabolismo , Adulto Jovem
19.
Horm Behav ; 70: 14-21, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25644313

RESUMO

Across human societies and many nonhuman animals, males have greater interest in uncommitted sex (more unrestricted sociosexuality) than do females. Testosterone shows positive associations with male-typical sociosexual behavior in nonhuman animals. Yet, it remains unclear whether the human sex difference in sociosexual psychology (attitudes and desires) is mediated by testosterone, whether any relationships between testosterone and sociosexuality differ between men and women, and what the nature of these possible relationships might be. In studies to resolve these questions, we examined relationships between salivary testosterone concentrations and sociosexual psychology and behavior in men and women. We measured testosterone in all men in our sample, but only in those women taking oral contraception (OC-using women) in order to reduce the influence of ovulatory cycle variation in ovarian hormone production. We found that OC-using women did not differ from normally-ovulating women in sociosexual psychology or behavior, but that circulating testosterone mediated the sex difference in human sociosexuality and predicted sociosexual psychology in men but not OC-using women. Moreover, when sociosexual psychology was controlled, men's sociosexual behavior (number of sexual partners) was negatively related to testosterone, suggesting that testosterone drives sociosexual psychology in men and is inhibited when those desires are fulfilled. This more complex relationship between androgens and male sexuality may reconcile some conflicting prior reports.


Assuntos
Retroalimentação Psicológica/fisiologia , Parceiros Sexuais/psicologia , Testosterona/metabolismo , Animais , Atitude , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivação/fisiologia , Saliva/química , Caracteres Sexuais , Comportamento Sexual , Adulto Jovem
20.
Arch Sex Behav ; 43(5): 941-52, 2014 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24379080

RESUMO

Unlike men, heterosexual women's genital arousal is gender nonspecific, such that heterosexual women show relatively similar genital arousal to sexual stimuli depicting men and women but typically report greater subjective arousal to male stimuli. Based on the ovulatory-shift hypothesis-that women show a mid-cycle shift in preferences towards more masculine features during peak fertility-we predicted that heterosexual women's genital and subjective arousal would be gender specific (more arousal towards male stimuli) during peak fertility. Twenty-two naturally-cycling heterosexual women were assessed during the follicular and luteal phases of their menstrual cycle to examine the role of menstrual cycle phase in gender specificity of genital and subjective sexual arousal. Menstrual cycle phase was confirmed with salivary hormone assays; phase at the time of first testing was counterbalanced. Women's genital and subjective sexual arousal patterns were gender nonspecific, irrespective of cycle phase. Cycle phase at first testing session did not influence genital or subjective arousal in the second testing session. Similar to previous research, women's genital and subjective sexual arousal varied with cues of sexual activity, but neither genital nor subjective sexual arousal varied by gender cues, with the exception of masturbation stimuli, where women showed higher genital arousal to the stimuli depicting male compared to female actors. These data suggest that menstrual cycle phase does not influence the gender specificity of heterosexual women's genital and subjective sexual arousal.


Assuntos
Nível de Alerta , Heterossexualidade/fisiologia , Ciclo Menstrual/fisiologia , Comportamento Sexual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ciclo Menstrual/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa