Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
1.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e149, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31064594

RESUMO

Replication failures were among the triggers of a reform movement which, in a very short time, has been enormously useful in raising standards and improving methods. As a result, the massive multilab multi-experiment replication projects have served their purpose and will die out. We describe other types of replications - both friendly and adversarial - that should continue to be beneficial.

2.
Forensic Sci Int Synerg ; 4: 100200, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35647506

RESUMO

The success of forensic science depends heavily on human reasoning abilities. Although we typically navigate our lives well using those abilities, decades of psychological science research shows that human reasoning is not always rational. In addition, forensic science often demands that its practitioners reason in non-natural ways. This article addresses how characteristics of human reasoning (either specific to an individual or in general) and characteristics of situations (either specific to a case or in general in a lab) can contribute to errors before, during, or after forensic analyses. In feature comparison judgments, such as fingerprints or firearms, a main challenge is to avoid biases from extraneous knowledge or arising from the comparison method itself. In causal and process judgments, for example fire scenes or pathology, a main challenge is to keep multiple potential hypotheses open as the investigation continues. Considering the contributions to forensic science judgments by persons, situations, and their interaction, reveals ways to develop procedures to decrease errors and improve accuracy.

3.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 17(2): 311-333, 2022 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34597198

RESUMO

The COVID-19 pandemic has extensively changed the state of psychological science from what research questions psychologists can ask to which methodologies psychologists can use to investigate them. In this article, we offer a perspective on how to optimize new research in the pandemic's wake. Because this pandemic is inherently a social phenomenon-an event that hinges on human-to-human contact-we focus on socially relevant subfields of psychology. We highlight specific psychological phenomena that have likely shifted as a result of the pandemic and discuss theoretical, methodological, and practical considerations of conducting research on these phenomena. After this discussion, we evaluate metascientific issues that have been amplified by the pandemic. We aim to demonstrate how theoretically grounded views on the COVID-19 pandemic can help make psychological science stronger-not weaker-in its wake.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Humanos , Pandemias , SARS-CoV-2
4.
Elife ; 102021 11 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34751133

RESUMO

Any large dataset can be analyzed in a number of ways, and it is possible that the use of different analysis strategies will lead to different results and conclusions. One way to assess whether the results obtained depend on the analysis strategy chosen is to employ multiple analysts and leave each of them free to follow their own approach. Here, we present consensus-based guidance for conducting and reporting such multi-analyst studies, and we discuss how broader adoption of the multi-analyst approach has the potential to strengthen the robustness of results and conclusions obtained from analyses of datasets in basic and applied research.


Assuntos
Consenso , Análise de Dados , Conjuntos de Dados como Assunto , Pesquisa
5.
Behav Brain Sci ; 33(2-3): 96-7, 2010 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20546665

RESUMO

We emphasize the value of the socio-ecological approach in addressing the problem of population variances. The socio-ecological perspective studies how social and natural habitats shape human behaviors, and are in turn shaped by those behaviors. This focus on system-level factors is particularly well-suited to studying the origins of group differences in human behavior.


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Meio Social , Humanos , Personalidade , Projetos de Pesquisa
7.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 10(6): 886-99, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26581743

RESUMO

Crisis of replicability is one term that psychological scientists use for the current introspective phase we are in-I argue instead that we are going through a revolution analogous to a political revolution. Revolution 2.0 is an uprising focused on how we should be doing science now (i.e., in a 2.0 world). The precipitating events of the revolution have already been well-documented: failures to replicate, questionable research practices, fraud, etc. And the fact that none of these events is new to our field has also been well-documented. I suggest four interconnected reasons as to why this time is different: changing technology, changing demographics of researchers, limited resources, and misaligned incentives. I then describe two reasons why the revolution is more likely to catch on this time: technology (as part of the solution) and the fact that these concerns cut across social and life sciences-that is, we are not alone. Neither side in the revolution has behaved well, and each has characterized the other in extreme terms (although, of course, each has had a few extreme actors). Some suggested reforms are already taking hold (e.g., journals asking for more transparency in methods and analysis decisions; journals publishing replications) but the feared tyrannical requirements have, of course, not taken root (e.g., few journals require open data; there is no ban on exploratory analyses). Still, we have not yet made needed advances in the ways in which we accumulate, connect, and extract conclusions from our aggregated research. However, we are now ready to move forward by adopting incremental changes and by acknowledging the multiplicity of goals within psychological science.


Assuntos
Pesquisa Comportamental/história , Psicologia/história , Acesso à Informação/história , Pesquisa Comportamental/métodos , Pesquisa Comportamental/tendências , História do Século XX , História do Século XXI , Humanos , Psicologia/métodos , Psicologia/tendências , Editoração/história , Editoração/tendências , Projetos de Pesquisa/tendências , Estados Unidos
8.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 41(5): 643-58, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25758707

RESUMO

Legal and prescriptive theories of blame generally propose that judgments about an actor's mental state (e.g., her knowledge or intent) should remain separate from judgments about whether the actor caused an outcome. Three experiments, however, show that, even in the absence of intent or immorality, actors who have knowledge relevant to a potential outcome will be rated more causal of that outcome than their ignorant counterparts, even when their actions were identical. Additional analysis revealed that this effect was mediated by counterfactual thinking--that is, by imagining ways the outcome could have been prevented. Specifically, when actors had knowledge, participants generated more counterfactuals about ways the outcome could have been different that the actor could control, which in turn increased causal assignment to the actor. These results are consistent with the Crediting Causality Model, but conflict with some legal and moral theories of blame.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Pensamento , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Intenção , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
9.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 146: 41-50, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24374491

RESUMO

Cultural mindset is related to performance on a variety of cognitive tasks. In particular, studies of both chronic and situationally-primed mindsets show that individuals with a relatively interdependent mindset (i.e., an emphasis on relationships and connections among individuals) are more sensitive to background contextual information than individuals with a more independent mindset. Two experiments tested whether priming cultural mindset would affect sensitivity to background causes in a contingency learning and causal inference task. Participants were primed (either independent or interdependent), and then saw complete contingency information on each of 12 trials for two cover stories in Experiment 1 (hiking causing skin rashes, severed brakes causing wrecked cars) and two additional cover stories in Experiment 2 (school deadlines causing stress, fertilizers causing plant growth). We expected that relative to independent-primed participants, those interdependent-primed would give more weight to the explicitly-presented data indicative of hidden alternative background causes, but they did not do so. In Experiment 1, interdependents gave less weight to the data indicative of hidden background causes for the car accident cover story and showed a decreased sensitivity to the contingencies for that story. In Experiment 2, interdependents placed less weight on the observable data for cover stories that supported more extra-experimental causes, while independents' sensitivity did not vary with these extra-experimental causes. Thus, interdependents were more sensitive to background causes not explicitly presented in the experiment, but this sensitivity hurt rather than improved their acquisition of the explicitly-presented contingency information.


Assuntos
Cultura , Aprendizagem , Pensamento , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
10.
Dev Psychol ; 47(4): 1065-77, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21443337

RESUMO

Do children and adults use the same cues to judge whether someone is a reliable source of information? In 4 experiments, we investigated whether children (ages 5 and 6) and adults used information regarding accuracy, confidence, and calibration (i.e., how well an informant's confidence predicts the likelihood of being correct) to judge informants' credibility. We found that both children and adults used information about confidence and accuracy to judge credibility; however, only adults used information about informants' calibration. Adults discredited informants who exhibited poor calibration, but children did not. Requiring adult participants to complete a secondary task while evaluating informants' credibility impaired their ability to make use of calibration information. Thus, children and adults may differ in how they infer credibility because of the cognitive demands of using calibration.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Julgamento/fisiologia , Processos Mentais/fisiologia , Confiança/psicologia , Adolescente , Calibragem , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Nomes , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
12.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 17(2): 168-73, 2010 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20382915

RESUMO

Assessing informants' credibility is critical to several aspects of the legal process (e.g., when police interrogate suspects or jurors evaluate witnesses). There is a large body of research--from various areas of psychology and allied fields--about how people evaluate each others' credibility. We review the literature on lie detection and interpersonal perception to demonstrate that inferences regarding credibility may be multiply determined. Specifically, characteristics of the informant, of the listener, and of the situation affect people's perceptions of informants' credibility. We conclude with a discussion of research on calibration (i.e., an informant's confidence-accuracy relation) because it offers fruitful avenues for future credibility research in the legal domain.


Assuntos
Ciências Forenses , Jurisprudência , Confiança/psicologia , Crime , Enganação , Ciências Forenses/legislação & jurisprudência , Ciências Forenses/normas , Humanos , Detecção de Mentiras/psicologia , Comunicação não Verbal , Psicologia/legislação & jurisprudência , Percepção Social , Fala
14.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 5(2): 115-22, 2010 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26162119

RESUMO

Social and cognitive psychologists each study factors that influence the believability of arguments, but they have worked mostly in parallel. We briefly examine and compare the dominant theories explaining argument believability in the social persuasion literature and the cognitive category-based induction literature. Although the two areas ask similar questions, they use different paradigms to study different aspects of the issues. We describe each area's major paradigms and questions and then examine the conclusions that each area draws regarding the role of five variables important to argument believability: (a) the number of sources/premises, (b) the similarity between sources/premises, (c) individual differences in characteristics of the reasoner, (d) the available resources, and (e) the reasoner's background knowledge and beliefs. Comparing the two literatures provides a more complete picture of the factors influencing argument believability and provides fruitful new avenues for integration and exploration.

16.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 8(3): 313, 2013 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26172973
17.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 8(4): 412-3, 2013 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26173119
18.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 7(6): 655-6, 2012 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26168126
20.
Psychol Sci ; 18(1): 46-50, 2007 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17362377

RESUMO

Confident witnesses are deemed more credible than unconfident ones, and accurate witnesses are deemed more credible than inaccurate ones. But are those effects independent? Two experiments show that errors in testimony damage the overall credibility of witnesses who were confident about the erroneous testimony more than that of witnesses who were not confident about it. Furthermore, after making an error, less confident witnesses may appear more credible than more confident ones. Our interpretation of these results is that people make inferences about source calibration when evaluating testimony and other social communication.


Assuntos
Memória , Confiança , Adulto , Conflito Psicológico , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
Detalhe da pesquisa