Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 110
Filtrar
1.
J Aging Health ; 32(9): 1029-1041, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31592706

RESUMO

Objective: Studies of "healthy" cognitive aging often focus on a limited set of measures that decline with age. The current study argues that defining and supporting healthy cognition requires understanding diverse cognitive performance across the lifespan. Method: Data from the Cambridge Centre for Aging and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) cohort was examined across a range of cognitive domains. Performance was related to lifestyle including education, social engagement, and enrichment activities. Results: Results indicate variable relationships between cognition and age (positive, negative, or no relationship). Principal components analysis indicated maintained cognitive diversity across the adult lifespan, and that cognition-lifestyle relationships differed by age and domain. Discussion: Our findings support a view of normal cognitive aging as a lifelong developmental process with diverse relationships between cognition, lifestyle, and age. This reinforces the need for large-scale studies of cognitive aging to include a wider range of both ages and cognitive tasks.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento Cognitivo , Envelhecimento Saudável , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Cognição , Estudos de Coortes , Escolaridade , Feminino , Nível de Saúde , Humanos , Estilo de Vida , Longevidade , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Participação Social , Adulto Jovem
2.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 14(6): 623-631, 2019 08 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31119296

RESUMO

Human older age ushers in functional decline across the majority of cognitive domains. A notable exception seems to be affective processing, with older people reporting higher levels of emotional well-being. Here we evaluated age-related changes in emotional reactivity and regulation in a representative subsample (N = 104; age range: 23-88 years) of the population-derived Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience cohort. Performance on a film-based emotion reactivity and regulation task in the magnetic resonance imaging scanner showed an age-related decline in positive reactivity, alongside a similar decline in the capacity to down-regulate negative affect. Decreased positivity with age was associated with reduced activation in the middle frontal gyrus. These findings, from the largest neuroimaging investigation to-date, provide no support for age-related increases in positive emotional reactivity.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Regulação Emocional/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Neuroimagem Funcional , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
3.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 13(5): 525-534, 2018 05 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29660102

RESUMO

There is accumulating evidence that youths with antisocial behavior or psychopathic traits show deficits in facial emotion recognition, but little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying these impairments. A number of neuroimaging studies have investigated brain activity during facial emotion processing in youths with Conduct Disorder (CD) and adults with psychopathy, but few of these studies tested for group differences in effective connectivity-i.e. changes in connectivity during emotion processing. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging and psycho-physiological interaction methods, we investigated the impact of CD and psychopathic traits on amygdala activity and effective connectivity in 46 male youths with CD and 25 typically-developing controls when processing emotional faces. All participants were aged 16-21 years. Relative to controls, youths with CD showed reduced amygdala activity when processing angry or sad faces relative to neutral faces, but the groups did not significantly differ in amygdala-related effective connectivity. In contrast, psychopathic traits were negatively correlated with amygdala-ventral anterior cingulate cortex connectivity for angry vs neutral faces, but were unrelated to amygdala responses to angry or sad faces. These findings suggest that CD and psychopathic traits have differential effects on amygdala activation and functional interactions between limbic regions during facial emotion processing.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/psicologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Giro do Cíngulo/fisiologia , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Adolescente , Tonsila do Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagem , Ira , Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtorno da Conduta/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtorno da Conduta/psicologia , Giro do Cíngulo/diagnóstico por imagem , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Vias Neurais/diagnóstico por imagem , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Testes de Personalidade , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
4.
Neuropsychologia ; 100: 51-63, 2017 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28400328

RESUMO

Neuroimaging studies of speech perception have consistently indicated a left-hemisphere dominance in the temporal lobes' responses to intelligible auditory speech signals (McGettigan and Scott, 2012). However, there are important communicative cues that cannot be extracted from auditory signals alone, including the direction of the talker's gaze. Previous work has implicated the superior temporal cortices in processing gaze direction, with evidence for predominantly right-lateralized responses (Carlin & Calder, 2013). The aim of the current study was to investigate whether the lateralization of responses to talker gaze differs in an auditory communicative context. Participants in a functional MRI experiment watched and listened to videos of spoken sentences in which the auditory intelligibility and talker gaze direction were manipulated factorially. We observed a left-dominant temporal lobe sensitivity to the talker's gaze direction, in which the left anterior superior temporal sulcus/gyrus and temporal pole showed an enhanced response to direct gaze - further investigation revealed that this pattern of lateralization was modulated by auditory intelligibility. Our results suggest flexibility in the distribution of neural responses to social cues in the face within the context of a challenging speech perception task.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Comunicação , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Fala/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Oxigênio/sangue , Lobo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagem , Adulto Jovem
5.
Cereb Cortex ; 27(1): 92-103, 2017 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27909005

RESUMO

Autism spectrum conditions (ASC) are associated with a number of atypicalities in face processing, including difficulties in face memory. However, the neural mechanisms underlying this difficulty are unclear. In neurotypical individuals, repeated presentation of the same face is associated with a reduction in activity, known as repetition suppression (RS), in the fusiform face area (FFA). However, to date, no studies have investigated RS to faces in individuals with ASC, or the relationship between RS and face memory. Here, we measured RS to faces and geometric shapes in individuals with a clinical diagnosis of an ASC and in age and IQ matched controls. Relative to controls, the ASC group showed reduced RS to faces in bilateral FFA and reduced performance on a standardized test of face memory. By contrast, RS to shapes in object-selective regions and object memory did not differ between groups. Individual variation in face-memory performance was positively correlated with RS in regions of left parietal and prefrontal cortex. These findings suggest difficulties in face memory in ASC may be a consequence of differences in the way faces are stored and/or maintained across a network of regions involved in both visual perception and short-term/working memory.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Espectro Autista/fisiopatologia , Reconhecimento Facial , Inibição Psicológica , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Memória , Priming de Repetição , Córtex Visual/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Transtorno do Espectro Autista/complicações , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
6.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 57(9): 1018-26, 2016 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27306512

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Neuroimaging methods that allow researchers to investigate structural covariance between brain regions are increasingly being used to study psychiatric disorders. Structural covariance analyses are particularly well suited for studying disorders with putative neurodevelopmental origins as they appear sensitive to changes in the synchronized maturation of different brain regions. We assessed interregional correlations in cortical thickness as a measure of structural covariance, and applied this method to investigate the coordinated development of different brain regions in conduct disorder (CD). We also assessed whether structural covariance measures could differentiate between the childhood-onset (CO-CD) and adolescence-onset (AO-CD) subtypes of CD, which may differ in terms of etiology and adult outcomes. METHODS: We examined interregional correlations in cortical thickness in male youths with CO-CD or AO-CD relative to healthy controls (HCs) in two independent datasets. The age range in the Cambridge sample was 16-21 years (mean: 18.0), whereas the age range of the Southampton sample was 13-18 years (mean: 16.7). We used FreeSurfer to perform segmentations and applied structural covariance methods to the resulting parcellations. RESULTS: In both samples, CO-CD participants displayed a strikingly higher number of significant cross-cortical correlations compared to HC or AO-CD participants, whereas AO-CD participants presented fewer significant correlations than HCs. Group differences in the strength of the interregional correlations were observed in both samples, and each set of results remained significant when controlling for IQ and comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. CONCLUSIONS: This study provides new evidence for quantitative differences in structural brain organization between the CO-CD and AO-CD subtypes, and supports the hypothesis that both subtypes of CD have neurodevelopmental origins.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/anatomia & histologia , Transtorno da Conduta/patologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idade de Início , Córtex Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagem , Córtex Cerebral/patologia , Transtorno da Conduta/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtorno da Conduta/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Delinquência Juvenil , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
7.
Mol Autism ; 7: 25, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27087911

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Autism Spectrum Conditions (ASC) are associated with a range of perceptual atypicalities, including abnormalities in gaze processing. Pellicano and Burr (Trends Cogn Sci 16(10):504-10, 2012) have argued that these atypicalities might be explained within a Bayesian framework, in which perception represents the combination of sensory information with prior knowledge. They propose that the Bayesian priors of individuals with ASC might be attenuated, such that their perception is less reliant on prior knowledge than neurotypical individuals. An important tenet of Bayesian decision theory is that increased uncertainty about incoming sensory information will lead to a greater influence of the prior on perception. Consistent with this, Mareschal et al. (Curr Biol 23(8):717-21, 2013) showed that when noise is added to the eyes of a face (increasing uncertainty about gaze direction), gaze is more likely to be perceived as direct. METHODS: We adopted the same paradigm as Mareschal et al. to determine whether the influence of a prior on gaze perception is reduced in neurotypical participants with high numbers of autistic traits (experiment 1) and in individuals with a clinical diagnosis of ASC (experiment 2). Participants were presented with synthetic faces and asked to make a judgement about the relative gaze directions of the faces. Uncertainty about gaze direction was manipulated by adding noise to the eyes of a face. RESULTS: Consistent with previous work, in both experiment 1 and experiment 2, participants showed a bias towards perceiving gaze as direct under conditions of uncertainty. However, there was no evidence that the magnitude of this bias was reduced either in the ASC group or in neurotypical controls with a high number of autistic traits. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings challenge the attenuated priors theory of perception in ASC (Trends Cogn Sci 16(10):504-10, 2012) and related proposals (Trends Cogn Sci 17(1):1, 2013, Front Hum Neurosci 8:302, 2014), and suggest priors for gaze direction are intact in high-functioning ASC.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Espectro Autista/fisiopatologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Teorema de Bayes , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Visual , Adulto Jovem
8.
Neurosci Lett ; 612: 7-13, 2016 Jan 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26655466

RESUMO

Recent theories distinguish anxiety from fear in the brain. Anxiety is associated with activation in ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, while fear is associated with activation in periaqueductal gray, with amygdala involved in processing aspects of both emotional responses. These theories propose that the amount of information available about threat determines which of the two defensive responses is elicited, with fear and anxiety associated with well-defined and uncertain threats respectively. However, a direct test of this hypothesis is lacking. Here we provide such a test using fMRI to record participants' brain activity while they performed a computer-based task which required to press a button to move an artificial agent to a target position while an artificial predator chased the agent. In one condition (associated with fear) the predator was visible, while in another condition (associated with anxiety) the predator was invisible. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala showed increased activity when the predator was invisible compared to visible, while the opposite effect was observed in periaqueductal gray. We also observed that participants with high but not low trait-anxiety showed an hippocampal activation with invisible threat at an earlier time stage during the trial. These findings help clarify the neural mechanisms that underlie different defensive emotions and shed light on how these mechanisms may contribute to exaggerated anxiety.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/psicologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Medo , Percepção Visual , Adulto , Antecipação Psicológica , Mapeamento Encefálico , Mecanismos de Defesa , Reação de Fuga , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Incerteza , Adulto Jovem
9.
Cortex ; 80: 51-60, 2016 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26613972

RESUMO

There is substantial variation in the magnitude of the repetition suppression (RS) effects across individuals; however the causes of this variation remain unclear. In a recent study, we found that RS in occipitotemporal cortex was negatively related to individual variation in autistic traits in a neurotypical population. Recent proposals have considered autistic behaviours within a Bayesian framework, suggesting that individuals with autism may have 'attenuated priors' (i.e., their perception is less influenced by prior information). Predictive coding represents a neural instantiation of Bayesian inference, and characterises RS as reduction in prediction error between 'top-down' (prior beliefs) and 'bottom-up' (stimulus related) inputs. In accordance with this, evidence shows that RS is greater when repetition of a stimulus is expected relative to when it is unexpected. Here, using an established paradigm which manipulates the probability of stimulus repetition, we investigated the effect of perceptual expectation on RS in a group of neurotypical individuals varying on a measure of autistic traits. We predicted that the magnitude of the perceptual expectation effect would be negatively related to individual differences in autistic traits. We found a significant effect of perceptual expectation on RS in face-selective regions (i.e., greater RS when repetitions were expected relative to unexpected). However, there was no evidence of a relationship between autistic traits and the magnitude of this effect in any face-selective region of interest (ROI). These findings provide a challenge for the proposal that autism spectrum conditions (ASC) may be associated with the attenuated influence of prior information.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/fisiopatologia , Face/fisiologia , Individualidade , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Probabilidade , Adulto Jovem
10.
Cogn Neurosci ; 7(1-4): 103-13, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26077121

RESUMO

Human beings have remarkable social attention skills. From the initial processing of cues, such as eye gaze, head direction, and body orientation, we perceive where other people are attending, allowing us to draw inferences about the intentions, desires, and dispositions of others. But before we can infer why someone is attending to something in the world we must first accurately represent where they are attending. Here we investigate the "where" of social attention perception, and employ adaptation paradigms to ascertain how head and body orientation are visually represented in the human brain. Across two experiments we show that the representation of two cues to social attention (head and body orientation) exists at the category-specific level. This suggests that aftereffects do not arise from "social attention cells" discovered in macaques or from abstract representations of "leftness" or "rightness."


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Percepção Social , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Cabeça , Humanos , Masculino , Tronco , Adulto Jovem
11.
Neuroimage Clin ; 8: 253-60, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26106549

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Previous studies have reported changes in gray matter volume in youths with conduct disorder (CD), although these differences are difficult to interpret as they may have been driven by alterations in cortical thickness, surface area (SA), or folding. The objective of this study was to use surface-based morphometry (SBM) methods to compare male youths with CD and age and sex-matched healthy controls (HCs) in cortical thickness, SA, and folding. We also tested for structural differences between the childhood-onset and adolescence-onset subtypes of CD and performed regression analyses to assess for relationships between CD symptoms and callous-unemotional (CU) traits and SBM-derived measures. METHODS: We acquired structural neuroimaging data from 20 HCs and 36 CD participants (18 with childhood-onset CD and 18 with adolescence-onset CD) and analyzed the data using FreeSurfer. RESULTS: Relative to HCs, youths with CD showed reduced cortical thickness in the superior temporal gyrus, reduced SA in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and increased cortical folding in the insula. There were no significant differences between the childhood-onset and adolescence-onset CD subgroups in cortical thickness or SA, but several frontal and temporal regions showed increased cortical folding in childhood-onset relative to adolescence-onset CD participants. Both CD subgroups also showed increased cortical folding relative to HCs. CD symptoms were negatively correlated with OFC SA whereas CU traits were positively correlated with insula folding. CONCLUSIONS: Cortical thinning in the superior temporal gyrus may contribute to the social cognitive impairments displayed by youths with CD, whereas reduced OFC SA may lead to impairments in emotion regulation and reward processing in youths with CD. The increased cortical folding observed in the insula may reflect a maturational delay in this region and could mediate the link between CU traits and empathy deficits. Altered cortical folding was observed in childhood-onset and adolescence-onset forms of CD.


Assuntos
Transtorno da Personalidade Antissocial/patologia , Córtex Cerebral/patologia , Transtorno da Conduta/patologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idade de Início , Córtex Cerebral/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
12.
Cognition ; 142: 123-37, 2015 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26036924

RESUMO

Traditional models of face perception emphasize distinct routes for processing face identity and expression. These models have been highly influential in guiding neural and behavioural research on the mechanisms of face perception. However, it is becoming clear that specialised brain areas for coding identity and expression may respond to both attributes and that identity and expression perception can interact. Here we use perceptual aftereffects to demonstrate the existence of dimensions in perceptual face space that code both identity and expression, further challenging the traditional view. Specifically, we find a significant positive association between face identity aftereffects and expression aftereffects, which dissociates from other face (gaze) and non-face (tilt) aftereffects. Importantly, individual variation in the adaptive calibration of these common dimensions significantly predicts ability to recognize both identity and expression. These results highlight the role of common dimensions in our ability to recognize identity and expression, and show why the high-level visual processing of these attributes is not entirely distinct.


Assuntos
Expressão Facial , Reconhecimento Facial , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Povo Asiático/psicologia , Face/anatomia & histologia , Feminino , Pós-Efeito de Figura , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Fatores Sexuais , População Branca/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
13.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 10(10): 1323-8, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25698700

RESUMO

Despite the risks, people enjoy giving advice. One explanation is that giving beneficial advice can result in reflected glory, ego boosts or reputation enhancement. However, giving poor advice can be socially harmful (being perceived as incompetent or untrustworthy). In both circumstances, we have a vested interest in the advice follower's success or failure, especially when it reflects specifically on us compared with when it is diffused between multiple advisors. We examined these dynamics using an Advisor-Advisee Game, where subjects acted as an Advisor to a confederate Advisee who selected one of the three options when trying to win money: accept the subject's advice, accept the advice of a second confederate Advisor or accept both Advisors' advice. Results showed that having one's advice accepted, compared with being rejected, resulted in activity in the ventral striatum--a core reward area. Furthermore, the ventral striatum was only active when the subject's advice led to the advisee winning, and not when the advisee won based on the confederate's advice. Finally, the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) was more active when the Advisee won or lost money based solely on the subject's advice compared with when the second Advisor's advice was accepted. One explanation for these findings is that the MPFC monitors self-relevant social information, while the ventral striatum is active when others accept advice and when their success leads to reflected glory.


Assuntos
Relações Interpessoais , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Recompensa , Percepção Social , Estriado Ventral/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino
14.
J Vis ; 15(1): 15.1.1, 2015 Jan 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25556250

RESUMO

Facial expression is theorized to be visually represented in a multidimensional expression space, relative to a norm. This norm-based coding is typically argued to be implemented by a two-pool opponent coding system. However, the evidence supporting the opponent coding of expression cannot rule out the presence of a third channel tuned to the center of each coded dimension. Here we used a paradigm not previously applied to facial expression to determine whether a central-channel model is necessary to explain expression coding. Participants identified expressions taken from a fear/antifear trajectory, first at baseline and then in two adaptation conditions. In one condition, participants adapted to the expression at the center of the trajectory. In the other condition, participants adapted to alternating images from the two ends of the trajectory. The range of expressions that participants perceived as lying at the center of the trajectory narrowed in both conditions, a pattern that is not predicted by the central-channel model but can be explained by the opponent-coding model. Adaptation to the center of the trajectory also increased identification of both fear and antifear, which may indicate a functional benefit for adaptive coding of facial expression.


Assuntos
Expressão Facial , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica , Adulto , Feminino , Pós-Efeito de Figura , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
15.
Cereb Cortex ; 25(9): 2876-82, 2015 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24770707

RESUMO

The superior temporal sulcus (STS) in the human and monkey is sensitive to the motion of complex forms such as facial and bodily actions. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to explore network-level explanations for how the form and motion information in dynamic facial expressions might be combined in the human STS. Ventral occipitotemporal areas selective for facial form were localized in occipital and fusiform face areas (OFA and FFA), and motion sensitivity was localized in the more dorsal temporal area V5. We then tested various connectivity models that modeled communication between the ventral form and dorsal motion pathways. We show that facial form information modulated transmission of motion information from V5 to the STS, and that this face-selective modulation likely originated in OFA. This finding shows that form-selective motion sensitivity in the STS can be explained in terms of modulation of gain control on information flow in the motion pathway, and provides a substantial constraint for theories of the perception of faces and biological motion.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Dinâmica não Linear , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Rede Nervosa/irrigação sanguínea , Oxigênio/sangue , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Estimulação Luminosa , Probabilidade , Lobo Temporal/irrigação sanguínea , Adulto Jovem
16.
Cereb Cortex ; 25(10): 3381-93, 2015 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24988131

RESUMO

Repeated viewing of a stimulus causes a change in perceptual sensitivity, known as a visual aftereffect. Similarly, in neuroimaging, repetitions of the same stimulus result in a reduction in the neural response, known as repetition suppression (RS). Previous research shows that aftereffects for faces are reduced in both children with autism and in first-degree relatives. With functional magnetic resonance imaging, we found that the magnitude of RS to faces in neurotypical participants was negatively correlated with individual differences in autistic traits. We replicated this finding in a second experiment, while additional experiments showed that autistic traits also negatively predicted RS to images of scenes and simple geometric shapes. These findings suggest that a core aspect of neural function--the brain's response to repetition--is modulated by autistic traits.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Transtorno Autístico/fisiopatologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Individualidade , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
17.
BMC Neurol ; 14: 204, 2014 Oct 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25412575

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: As greater numbers of us are living longer, it is increasingly important to understand how we can age healthily. Although old age is often stereotyped as a time of declining mental abilities and inflexibility, cognitive neuroscience reveals that older adults use neural and cognitive resources flexibly, recruiting novel neural regions and cognitive processes when necessary. Our aim in this project is to understand how age-related changes to neural structure and function interact to support cognitive abilities across the lifespan. METHODS/DESIGN: We are recruiting a population-based cohort of 3000 adults aged 18 and over into Stage 1 of the project, where they complete an interview including health and lifestyle questions, a core cognitive assessment, and a self-completed questionnaire of lifetime experiences and physical activity. Of those interviewed, 700 participants aged 18-87 (100 per age decile) continue to Stage 2 where they undergo cognitive testing and provide measures of brain structure and function. Cognition is assessed across multiple domains including attention and executive control, language, memory, emotion, action control and learning. A subset of 280 adults return for in-depth neurocognitive assessment in Stage 3, using functional neuroimaging experiments across our key cognitive domains.Formal statistical models will be used to examine the changes that occur with healthy ageing, and to evaluate age-related reorganisation in terms of cognitive and neural functions invoked to compensate for overall age-related brain structural decline. Taken together the three stages provide deep phenotyping that will allow us to measure neural activity and flexibility during performance across a number of core cognitive functions. This approach offers hypothesis-driven insights into the relationship between brain and behaviour in healthy ageing that are relevant to the general population. DISCUSSION: Our study is a unique resource of neuroimaging and cognitive measures relevant to change across the adult lifespan. Because we focus on normal age-related changes, our results may contribute to changing views about the ageing process, lead to targeted interventions, and reveal how normal ageing relates to frail ageing in clinicopathological conditions such as Alzheimer's disease.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Protocolos Clínicos , Envelhecimento Cognitivo/fisiologia , Neuroimagem/métodos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reino Unido , Adulto Jovem
18.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab ; 99(10): E2101-6, 2014 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25062455

RESUMO

CONTEXT: Mutations in the melanocortin-4 receptor (MC4R) represent the commonest genetic form of obesity and are associated with hyperphagia. OBJECTIVE: The aim of this study was to investigate whether melanocortin signaling modulates anticipatory food reward by studying the brain activation response to food cues in individuals with MC4R mutations. Design/Setting/Participants/Main Outcome Measure: We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure blood oxygen level-dependent responses to images of highly palatable, appetizing foods, bland foods, and non-food objects in eight obese individuals with MC4R mutations, 10 equally obese controls, and eight lean controls with normal MC4R genotypes. Based on previous evidence, we performed a region-of-interest analysis centered on the caudate/putamen (dorsal striatum) and ventral striatum. RESULTS: Compared to non-foods, appetizing foods were associated with activation in the dorsal and ventral striatum in lean controls and in MC4R-deficient individuals. Surprisingly, we observed reduced activation of the dorsal and ventral striatum in obese controls relative to MC4R-deficient patients and lean controls. There were no group differences for the contrast of disgusting foods with bland foods or non-foods, suggesting that the effects observed in response to appetizing foods were not related to arousal. CONCLUSION: We identified differences in the striatal response to food cues between two groups of obese individuals, those with and those without MC4R mutations. These findings are consistent with a role for central melanocortinergic circuits in the neural response to visual food cues.


Assuntos
Apetite/fisiologia , Hiperfagia/genética , Neostriado/metabolismo , Obesidade/genética , Receptor Tipo 4 de Melanocortina/genética , Transdução de Sinais/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Feminino , Alimentos , Humanos , Hiperfagia/metabolismo , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Mutação , Obesidade/metabolismo , Oxigênio/sangue , Receptor Tipo 4 de Melanocortina/metabolismo , Aumento de Peso/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
19.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 35(7): 3290-301, 2014 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25050425

RESUMO

Advantageous inequality (AI) aversion, or paying at a personal cost to achieve equal reward distribution, represents a unique feature of human behavior. Here, we show that individuals have strong preferences for fairness in both disadvantageous (DI) and advantageous inequality (AI) situations, such that they alter others' payoff at a personal financial cost. At the neural level, we found that both types of inequality activated the putamen, orbitofrontal cortex, and insula, regions implicated in motivation. Individual difference analyses found that those who spent more money to increase others' payoff had stronger activity in putamen when they encountered AI and less functional connectivity between putamen and both orbitofrontal cortex and anterior insula. Conversely, those who spent more money to reduce others' payoff had stronger activity in amygdala in response to DI and less functional connectivity between amygdala and ventral anterior cingulate cortex. These dissociations suggest that both types of inequality are processed by similar brain areas, yet modulated by different neural pathways.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Retroalimentação Psicológica/fisiologia , Relações Interpessoais , Recompensa , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Encéfalo/irrigação sanguínea , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Oxigênio/sangue , Estimulação Luminosa , Psicofísica , Punição , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
20.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 53(6): 677-687.e5, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24839886

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Conduct disorder (CD) in females is associated with negative adult outcomes including mental health problems and personality disorders. Although recent neuroimaging studies have reported changes in neural activity during facial emotion processing in males with CD or callous-unemotional (CU) traits, there have been no neuroimaging studies specifically assessing females with CD. We addressed this gap by investigating whether female adolescents with CD show atypical neural activation when processing emotional or neutral faces. METHOD: We acquired functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from 20 female adolescents with CD and 20 female control participants while they viewed angry, sad, and neutral faces. RESULTS: An omnibus group (CD, control) by facial emotion (angry, sad, neutral) analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed main effects of facial emotion in superior temporal cortex, fusiform gyrus, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and insula, and main effects of group in medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and right anterior insula. Female participants with CD showed reduced medial OFC and increased anterior insula responses relative to healthy controls. There were no significant group × facial emotion interactions. Lifetime CD symptoms were negatively correlated with amygdala, superior temporal cortex, fusiform gyrus, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity for the contrast "all-faces versus fixation." CU traits were negatively correlated with fusiform gyrus activity for the contrast sad versus neutral faces. CONCLUSION: Females with CD showed atypical neural activation during the processing of all facial expressions, irrespective of valence. Our results demonstrate that severity of CD symptoms and CU traits is important in explaining abnormal patterns of neural activity.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiopatologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Transtorno da Conduta/fisiopatologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA