Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 58
Filtrar
1.
J Vis ; 24(5): 1, 2024 May 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38691088

RESUMO

Still life paintings comprise a wealth of data on visual perception. Prior work has shown that the color statistics of objects show a marked bias for warm colors. Here, we ask about the relative chromatic contrast of these object-associated colors compared with background colors in still life paintings. We reasoned that, owing to the memory color effect, where the color of familiar objects is perceived more saturated, warm colors will be relatively more saturated than cool colors in still life paintings as compared with photographs. We analyzed color in 108 slides of still life paintings of fruit from the teaching slide collection of the Fogg University Art Museum and 41 color-calibrated photographs of fruit from the McGill data set. The results show that the relatively higher chromatic contrast of warm colors was greater for paintings compared with photographs, consistent with the hypothesis.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Frutas , Pinturas , Fotografação , Humanos , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Fotografação/métodos , Cor , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia
2.
Neuroscientist ; : 10738584241234049, 2024 Mar 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38462512

RESUMO

In the history of neuroscience, Cajal stands tall. Many figures in the late 19th and early 20th centuries made major contributions to neuroscience-Sherrington, Ferrier, Jackson, Holmes, Adrian, and Békésy, to name a few. But in the public mind, Cajal is unique. His application of the Golgi method, with an array of histologic stains, unlocked a wealth of new knowledge on the structure and function of the brain. Here we argue that Cajal's success should not only be attributed to the importance of his scientific contributions but also to the artistic visual language that he created and to his pioneering self-branding, which exploited methods of the artist, including classical drawing and the new invention of photography. We argue that Cajal created his distinctive visual language and self-branding strategy by interweaving an ostensibly objective research product with an intimately subjective narrative about the brain and himself. His approach is evident in the use of photography, notably self-portraits, which furthered broad engagement initially inspired by his scientific drawings. Through his visual language, Cajal made an impact in art and culture far beyond the bounds of science, which has sustained his scientific legacy.

3.
Psychol Sci ; 34(12): 1350-1362, 2023 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37906163

RESUMO

Words and the concepts they represent vary across languages. Here we ask if mother-tongue concepts are altered by learning a second language. What happens when speakers of Tsimane', a language with few consensus color terms, learn Bolivian Spanish, a language with more terms? Three possibilities arise: Concepts in Tsimane' may remain unaffected, or they may be remapped, either by Tsimane' terms taking on new meanings or by borrowing Bolivian-Spanish terms. We found that adult bilingual speakers (n = 30) remapped Tsimane' concepts without importing Bolivian-Spanish terms into Tsimane'. All Tsimane' terms become more precise; for example, concepts of monolingual shandyes and yushñus (~green or blue, used synonymously by Tsimane' monolinguals; n = 71) come to reflect the Bolivian-Spanish distinction of verde (~green) and azul (~blue). These results show that learning a second language can change the concepts in the first language.


Assuntos
Idioma , Multilinguismo , Adulto , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Bolívia , Nível de Saúde
4.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 27(9): 791-804, 2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37394292

RESUMO

Hering's Opponent-Colors Theory has been central to understanding color appearance for 150 years. It aims to explain the phenomenology of colors with two linked propositions. First, a psychological hypothesis stipulates that any color is described necessarily and sufficiently by the extent to which it appears reddish-versus-greenish, bluish-versus-yellowish, and blackish-versus-whitish. Second, a physiological hypothesis stipulates that these perceptual mechanisms are encoded by three innate brain mechanisms. We review the evidence and conclude that neither side of the linking proposition is accurate: the theory is wrong. We sketch out an alternative, Utility-Based Coding, by which the known retinal cone-opponent mechanisms represent optimal encoding of spectral information given competing selective pressure to extract high-acuity spatial information; and phenomenological color categories represent an adaptive, efficient, output of the brain governed by behavioral demands.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Percepção de Cores , Humanos , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Cor
5.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 39(1-2): 54-57, 2022 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35624546
7.
Nat Commun ; 13(1): 661, 2022 02 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35115511

RESUMO

Hue and luminance contrast are basic visual features. Here we use multivariate analyses of magnetoencephalography data to investigate the timing of the neural computations that extract them, and whether they depend on common neural circuits. We show that hue and luminance-contrast polarity can be decoded from MEG data and, with lower accuracy, both features can be decoded across changes in the other feature. These results are consistent with the existence of both common and separable neural mechanisms. The decoding time course is earlier and more temporally precise for luminance polarity than hue, a result that does not depend on task, suggesting that luminance contrast is an updating signal that separates visual events. Meanwhile, cross-temporal generalization is slightly greater for representations of hue compared to luminance polarity, providing a neural correlate of the preeminence of hue in perceptual grouping and memory. Finally, decoding of luminance polarity varies depending on the hues used to obtain training and testing data. The pattern of results is consistent with observations that luminance contrast is mediated by both L-M and S cone sub-cortical mechanisms.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Defeitos da Visão Cromática/fisiopatologia , Cor , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Adulto , Defeitos da Visão Cromática/diagnóstico , Defeitos da Visão Cromática/diagnóstico por imagem , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Magnetoencefalografia/métodos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/citologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/citologia , Córtex Visual/diagnóstico por imagem , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
Curr Biol ; 31(16): R982-R983, 2021 Aug 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34428415

RESUMO

Bevil Conway introduces colors.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Cor
9.
Handb Clin Neurol ; 178: 131-153, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33832674

RESUMO

Color is a fundamental aspect of normal visual experience. This chapter provides an overview of the role of color in human behavior, a survey of current knowledge regarding the genetic, retinal, and neural mechanisms that enable color vision, and a review of inherited and acquired defects of color vision including a discussion of diagnostic tests.


Assuntos
Defeitos da Visão Cromática , Visão de Cores , Humanos , Retina
11.
eNeuro ; 8(2)2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33483324

RESUMO

What role does color play in the neural representation of complex shapes? We approached the question by measuring color responses of face-selective neurons, using fMRI-guided microelectrode recording of the middle and anterior face patches of inferior temporal cortex (IT) in rhesus macaques. Face-selective cells responded weakly to pure color (equiluminant) photographs of faces. But many of the cells nonetheless showed a bias for warm colors when assessed using images that preserved the luminance contrast relationships of the original photographs. This bias was also found for non-face-selective neurons. Fourier analysis uncovered two components: the first harmonic, accounting for most of the tuning, was biased toward reddish colors, corresponding to the L>M pole of the L-M cardinal axis. The second harmonic showed a bias for modulation between blue and yellow colors axis, corresponding to the S-cone axis. To test what role face-selective cells play in behavior, we related the information content of the neural population with the distribution of face colors. The analyses show that face-selective cells are not optimally tuned to discriminate face colors, but are consistent with the idea that face-selective cells contribute selectively to processing the green-red contrast of faces. The research supports the hypothesis that color-specific information related to the discrimination of objects, including faces, is handled by neural circuits that are independent of shape-selective cortex, as captured by the multistage parallel processing framework of IT (Lafer-Sousa and Conway, 2013).


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Animais , Percepção de Cores , Macaca mulatta , Estimulação Luminosa , Lobo Temporal
12.
Curr Biol ; 31(3): 515-526.e5, 2021 02 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33202253

RESUMO

The geometry that describes the relationship among colors, and the neural mechanisms that support color vision, are unsettled. Here, we use multivariate analyses of measurements of brain activity obtained with magnetoencephalography to reverse-engineer a geometry of the neural representation of color space. The analyses depend upon determining similarity relationships among the spatial patterns of neural responses to different colors and assessing how these relationships change in time. We evaluate the approach by relating the results to universal patterns in color naming. Two prominent patterns of color naming could be accounted for by the decoding results: the greater precision in naming warm colors compared to cool colors evident by an interaction of hue and lightness, and the preeminence among colors of reddish hues. Additional experiments showed that classifiers trained on responses to color words could decode color from data obtained using colored stimuli, but only at relatively long delays after stimulus onset. These results provide evidence that perceptual representations can give rise to semantic representations, but not the reverse. Taken together, the results uncover a dynamic geometry that provides neural correlates for color appearance and generates new hypotheses about the structure of color space.


Assuntos
Visão de Cores , Magnetoencefalografia , Cor , Percepção de Cores , Semântica
13.
Cognition ; 195: 104086, 2020 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31731116

RESUMO

Languages vary in their number of color terms. A widely accepted theory proposes that languages evolve, acquiring color terms in a stereotyped sequence. This theory, by Berlin and Kay (BK), is supported by analyzing best exemplars ("focal colors") of basic color terms in the World Color Survey (WCS) of 110 languages. But the instructions of the WCS were complex and the color chips confounded hue and saturation, which likely impacted focal-color selection. In addition, it is now known that even so-called early-stage languages nonetheless have a complete representation of color distributed across the population. These facts undermine the BK theory. Here we revisit the evolution of color terms using original color-naming data obtained with simple instructions in Tsimane', an Amazonian culture that has limited contact with industrialized society. We also collected data in Bolivian-Spanish speakers and English speakers. We discovered that information theory analysis of color-naming data was not influenced by color-chip saturation, which motivated a new analysis of the WCS data. Embedded within a universal pattern in which warm colors (reds, oranges) are always communicated more efficiently than cool colors (blues, greens), as languages increase in overall communicative efficiency about color, some colors undergo greater increases in communication efficiency compared to others. Communication efficiency increases first for yellow, then brown, then purple. The present analyses and results provide a new framework for understanding the evolution of color terms: what varies among cultures is not whether colors are seen differently, but the extent to which color is useful.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Cor , Comunicação , Comparação Transcultural , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Bolívia , Feminino , Humanos , Indígenas Sul-Americanos , Teoria da Informação , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicolinguística , Estados Unidos , Adulto Jovem
14.
Nat Commun ; 10(1): 3010, 2019 07 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31285438

RESUMO

What is color vision for? Here we compared the extent to which memory modulates color appearance of objects and faces. Participants matched the colors of stimuli illuminated by low-pressure sodium light, which renders scenes monochromatic. Matches for fruit were not predicted by stimulus identity. In contrast, matches for faces were predictable, but surprising: faces appeared green and looked sick. The paradoxical face-color percept could be explained by a Bayesian observer model constrained by efficient coding. The color-matching data suggest that the face-color prior is established by visual signals arising from the recently evolved L-M cone system, not the older S-cone channel. Taken together, the results show that when retinal mechanisms of color vision are impaired, the impact of memory on color perception is greatest for face color, supporting the idea that trichromatic color plays an important role in social communication.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Teorema de Bayes , Cor , Visão de Cores , Face , Feminino , Humanos , Luz , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones
15.
Nat Neurosci ; 22(7): 1057-1060, 2019 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31182868

RESUMO

We report a difference between humans and macaque monkeys in the functional organization of cortical regions implicated in pitch perception. Humans but not macaques showed regions with a strong preference for harmonic sounds compared to noise, measured with both synthetic tones and macaque vocalizations. In contrast, frequency-selective tonotopic maps were similar between the two species. This species difference may be driven by the unique demands of speech and music perception in humans.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Percepção da Altura Sonora/fisiologia , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Animais , Córtex Auditivo/anatomia & histologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Feminino , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Música , Especificidade da Espécie , Vocalização Animal
16.
Neuroimage ; 188: 427-444, 2019 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30521952

RESUMO

The extent to which the major subdivisions of prefrontal cortex (PFC) can be functionally partitioned is unclear. In approaching the question, it is often assumed that the organization is task dependent. Here we use fMRI to show that PFC can respond in a task-independent way, and we leverage these responses to uncover a stimulus-driven functional organization. The results were generated by mapping the relative location of responses to faces, bodies, scenes, disparity, color, and eccentricity in four passively fixating macaques. The results control for individual differences in functional architecture and provide the first account of a systematic visual stimulus-driven functional organization across PFC. Responses were focused in dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), in the ventral prearcuate region; and in ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC), extending into orbital PFC. Face patches were in the VLPFC focus and were characterized by a striking lack of response to non-face stimuli rather than an especially strong response to faces. Color-biased regions were near but distinct from face patches. One scene-biased region was consistently localized with different contrasts and overlapped the disparity-biased region to define the DLPFC focus. All visually responsive regions showed a peripheral visual-field bias. These results uncover an organizational scheme that presumably constrains the flow of information about different visual modalities into PFC.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Córtex Pré-Frontal/diagnóstico por imagem
17.
J Vis ; 18(11): 1, 2018 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30285103

RESUMO

We hypothesized that the parts of scenes identified by human observers as "objects" show distinct color properties from backgrounds, and that the brain uses this information towards object recognition. To test this hypothesis, we examined the color statistics of naturally and artificially colored objects and backgrounds in a database of over 20,000 images annotated with object labels. Objects tended to be warmer colored (L-cone response > M-cone response) and more saturated compared to backgrounds. That the distinguishing chromatic property of objects was defined mostly by the L-M post-receptoral mechanism, rather than the S mechanism, is consistent with the idea that trichromatic color vision evolved in response to a selective pressure to identify objects. We also show that classifiers trained using only color information could distinguish animate versus inanimate objects, and at a performance level that was comparable to classification using shape features. Animate/inanimate is considered a fundamental superordinate category distinction, previously thought to be computed by the brain using only shape information. Our results show that color could contribute to animate/inanimate, and likely other, object-category assignments. Finally, color-tuning measured in two macaque monkeys with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and confirmed by fMRI-guided microelectrode recording, supports the idea that responsiveness to color reflects the global functional organization of inferior temporal cortex, the brain region implicated in object vision. More strongly in IT than in V1, colors associated with objects elicited higher responses than colors less often associated with objects.


Assuntos
Visão de Cores/fisiologia , Cor , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Macaca , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia
19.
Vision Res ; 151: 2-6, 2018 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29959956

RESUMO

The study of color vision encompasses many disciplines, including art, biochemistry, biophysics, brain imaging, cognitive neuroscience, color preferences, colorimetry, computer modelling, design, electrophysiology, language and cognition, molecular genetics, neuroscience, physiological optics, psychophysics and physiological optics. Coupled with the elusive nature of the subjective experience of color, this wide range of disciplines makes the study of color as challenging as it is fascinating. This overview of the special issue Color: Cone Opponency and Beyond outlines the state of the science of color, and points to some of the many questions that remain to be answered in this exciting field.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Visão de Cores/fisiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Pesquisa Biomédica , Humanos
20.
Annu Rev Vis Sci ; 4: 381-402, 2018 09 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30059648

RESUMO

Inferior temporal cortex (IT) is a key part of the ventral visual pathway implicated in object, face, and scene perception. But how does IT work? Here, I describe an organizational scheme that marries form and function and provides a framework for future research. The scheme consists of a series of stages arranged along the posterior-anterior axis of IT, defined by anatomical connections and functional responses. Each stage comprises a complement of subregions that have a systematic spatial relationship. The organization of each stage is governed by an eccentricity template, and corresponding eccentricity representations across stages are interconnected. Foveal representations take on a role in high-acuity object vision (including face recognition); intermediate representations compute other aspects of object vision such as behavioral valence (using color and surface cues); and peripheral representations encode information about scenes. This multistage, parallel-processing model invokes an innately determined organization refined by visual experience that is consistent with principles of cortical development. The model is also consistent with principles of evolution, which suggest that visual cortex expanded through replication of retinotopic areas. Finally, the model predicts that the most extensively studied network within IT-the face patches-is not unique but rather one manifestation of a canonical set of operations that reveal general principles of how IT works.


Assuntos
Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Vias Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA