Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 308
Filtrar
1.
Front Psychol ; 12: 734576, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35370867

RESUMO

The primary aim of this study was to investigate the relation between self-perceived executive functions (EFs) and the school achievement of young adolescents (aged 10-12 years), while controlling for parental education and sex. We specifically focused on executive aspects of daily life behavior and the higher-order EFs, as measured with self-report, rather than on the more basic EFs which have been the primary focus of prior investigations. In two independent samples of sixth graders (N > 200 each), students evaluated their EFs on a self-report questionnaire, the Amsterdam Executive Functioning Inventory. School achievement in the domains of mathematics and reading comprehension were evaluated with nationally used, norm-based achievement tests. Results revealed that the self-perceived EFs of young adolescents were significantly correlated with their school achievement in both study samples. School achievement was also correlated with the level of parental education, but the factor sex did not have such influence. In study 1, self-perceived EFs explained additional variance in school achievement, while controlling for parental education and sex. In study 2, this was only the case for the most robust measure of school achievement, i.e., the end-of-primary-school final achievement test. Furthermore, besides the relation with achievement tests, we also found a relation between self-perceived EFs and teacher ratings behavioral problems in the classroom. Together, our findings imply that young students can properly reflect on the effectiveness and appropriateness of their EFs in a way that is relevant to their academic achievement and classroom behavior. The findings underscore the importance of considering the development of EFs and parental education in the evaluation of academic achievements in early adolescence.

2.
PLoS One ; 15(1): e0227607, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31929576

RESUMO

This large-scale cross-sectional study had the aim to investigate whether adolescent males and females differ in self-perceived self-regulation. The large sample size allowed us to investigate sex differences in three age-groups of young (n = 161), middle (n = 133) and late (n = 159) adolescents. Self-regulation was evaluated with a self-report questionnaire, the Amsterdam Executive Functioning Inventory (AEFI). This questionnaire gives a proxi for three executive functions that are important for proper self-regulation: (1) self-control & self-monitoring, (2) attention, and (3) planning & initiative taking. Results revealed clear sex differences in the self-regulation as perceived by mid-adolescents (i.e., 13-16 years). In this age period, females evaluated their attention higher than males, and they reported higher levels of self-control & self-monitoring. Our findings offer important new insights with respect to the decision making, academic achievements and behaviour of 13-16-year olds. Self-regulation is known to have a central role in academic achievement and in behavioural organisation. The sex differences in self-regulation in mid-adolescence may therefore explain part of the difference which males and females in this age-group exhibit in academic achievements and behavioural organisations. The results imply that self-regulation may be a relevant intervention target: rather than focussing on changing behaviour, interventions may focus more on self-insights and thereby changing the adolescent's perceptions about their behaviour. Increased self-insight may have the potency to actually change behaviour, which might be an interesting target for future investigation.


Assuntos
Autoimagem , Autocontrole , Adolescente , Comportamento do Adolescente/psicologia , Atenção , Criança , Estudos Transversais , Função Executiva , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicologia do Adolescente , Autorrelato , Caracteres Sexuais , Fatores Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
3.
J Fish Biol ; 88(4): 1661-8, 2016 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26940195

RESUMO

To investigate the link between personality and maximum food intake of inactive individuals, food-deprived three-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus at rest in their home compartments were provided with ad libitum prey items. Bolder individuals ate considerably more than shyer individuals, even after accounting for body size, while sociability did not have an effect. These findings support pace-of-life theory predicting that life-history strategies are linked to boldness.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Ingestão de Alimentos , Smegmamorpha/fisiologia , Animais , Tamanho Corporal , Assunção de Riscos , Comportamento Social
4.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 28(6): 954-73, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25066535

RESUMO

This study investigated psychometric properties (standardization and validity) of the Rey Complex Figure Organizational Strategy Score (RCF-OSS) in a sample of 217 healthy children aged 5-7 years. Our results showed that RCF-OSS performance changes significantly between 5 and 7 years of age. While most 5-year-olds used a local approach when copying the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF), 7-year-olds increasingly adopted a global approach. RCF-OSS performance correlated significantly, but moderately with measures of ROCF accuracy, executive functioning (fluency, working memory, reasoning), and non-executive functioning (visual-motor integration, visual attention, processing speed, numeracy). These findings seem to indicate that RCF-OSS performance reflects a range of cognitive skills at 5 to 7 years of age, including aspects of executive and non-executive functioning.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Psicometria/normas , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Atenção , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Valores de Referência , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
5.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 27(4): 433-9, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22572055

RESUMO

The Letter Digit Substitution Test (LDST) was administered to a sample of N=296 healthy children (aged between 8.03 and 15.87). The aim of the present study was to evaluate the impact of age, gender, and parental educational level on LDST performance and to establish demographically corrected normative data. The results showed that the relationship between age and LDST performance was curvilinear (i.e., improvements in test performance were more pronounced for younger children than for older children) and was moderated by gender (i.e., the gender differences were small at younger ages but increased as a function of age, with girls outperforming boys). Moreover, children who had parents with a higher level of education outperformed their counterparts who had parents with a lower level of education. Regression-based normative LDST data were established, and an automatic scoring program was provided.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento do Adolescente , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Processos Mentais , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Criança , Escolaridade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pais/educação , Valores de Referência , Fatores Sexuais
6.
Neurology ; 78(5): 352-60, 2012 Jan 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22262753

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Parietal lobe dysfunction is an important characteristic of early Alzheimer disease (AD). Functional studies have shown conflicting parietal activation patterns indicative of either compensatory or dysfunctional mechanisms. This study aimed at examining activation differences in early AD using a visuospatial task. We focused on functional characteristics of the parietal lobe and examined compensation or disconnection mechanisms by combining a fMRI task with effective connectivity measures from Granger causality mapping (GCM). METHODS: Eighteen male patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and 18 male cognitively healthy older individuals were given a mental rotation task with different rotation angles. RESULTS: There were no behavioral group differences on the fMRI task. Separate measurements at each angle revealed widespread activation group differences. More temporal and parietal activation in the higher angle condition was observed in patients with aMCI. The parametric modulation, which identifies regions associated with increasing angle, confirmed these results. The GCM showed increased connectivity within the parietal lobe and between parietal and temporal regions in patients with aMCI. Decreased connectivity was found between the inferior parietal lobule and posterior cingulate gyrus. Connectivity patterns correlated with memory performance scores in patients with aMCI. CONCLUSIONS: Our results demonstrate increased effective temporoparietal connectivity in patients with aMCI, while maintaining intact behavioral performance. This might be a compensational mechanism to counteract a parietal-posterior cingulate gyrus disconnection. These findings highlight the importance of connectivity changes in the pathophysiology of AD. In addition, effective connectivity may be a promising method for evaluating interventions aimed at the promotion of compensatory mechanisms.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Idoso , Mapeamento Encefálico , Causalidade , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imaginação/fisiologia , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia
7.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22265194

RESUMO

Fish consumption has shown its benefits for cognitive functioning in the elderly or children with disorders (e.g., autism, ADHD), but has rarely been investigated in relation to cognitive performance and school performance of healthy adolescents. We executed an observational study in 700 Dutch high school students aged 12-18 years. Fish consumption data, end term grades, scores on the Amsterdam Vocabulary Test, and scores on the Youth Self-Report were collected. Results revealed that 13.6% of the Dutch adolescents never ate fish, 6.4% met national guidelines, 16.9% reached half of the norm, and 63.1% did eat fish but too little to meet at least half of the norm. Analysis of variance, controlled for relevant covariates, showed significant differences between the four fish consumption groups in vocabulary (p=.05). A trend for significance was found for end term grades (p=.07). Contrast analyses demonstrated significant quadratic associations between fish consumption and vocabulary (p=.01) and end term grades (p=.01). Thus higher fish intake was associated with more advanced vocabulary and higher end term grades. However, eating more fish than the described norm seemed no longer beneficial.


Assuntos
Cognição , Dieta , Peixes , Adolescente , Animais , Criança , Escolaridade , Comportamento Alimentar , Humanos , Países Baixos , Estudantes , Vocabulário
8.
Genes Brain Behav ; 10(3): 354-64, 2011 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21255267

RESUMO

Low blood levels of B vitamins have been implicated in age-associated cognitive impairment. The present study investigated the association between genetic variation in folate metabolism and age-related cognitive decline in the ninth decade of life. Both the 677C>T (rs1801133) polymorphism and the scarcely studied 1298A>C (rs1801131) polymorphism of the MTHFR gene were assessed in relation to cognitive change over 8 years in older community-dwelling individuals. MTHFR genotype was determined in 476 participants of the Lothian Birth Cohort 1921, whose intelligence was measured in childhood in the Scottish Mental Survey of 1932. Cognitive performance on the domains of verbal memory, reasoning and verbal fluency was assessed at mean age of 79 (n = 476) and again at mean ages of 83 (n = 275) and 87 (n = 180). Using linear mixed models, the MTHFR 677C>T and 1298A>C variants were not associated with the rate of cognitive change between 79 and 87 years, neither in the total sample, nor in a subsample of individuals with erythrocyte folate levels below the median. APOE E4 allele carrier status did not interact with MTHFR genotype in affecting change in cognitive performance over 8 years. No significant combined effect of the two polymorphisms was found. In conclusion, MTHFR 677C>T and 1298A>C polymorphisms were not associated with individual change in cognitive functioning in the ninth decade of life. Although polymorphisms in the MTHFR gene may cause disturbances in folate metabolism, they do not appear to be accompanied by changes in cognitive functioning in old age.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/genética , Transtornos Cognitivos/enzimologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/genética , Metilenotetra-Hidrofolato Redutase (NADPH2)/genética , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/fisiopatologia , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Frequência do Gene , Genótipo , Humanos , Masculino , Metilenotetra-Hidrofolato Redutase (NADPH2)/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Escócia
9.
Neuroimage ; 55(3): 1287-97, 2011 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21232618

RESUMO

Advanced age is associated with declines in brain structure and in cognitive performance, but it is unclear which aspects of brain aging mediate cognitive declines. We inquired if individual differences in white matter integrity contribute to age differences in two cognitive domains with established vulnerability to aging: executive functioning and speed of processing. The participants were healthy volunteers aged 50-81, some of whom had elevated blood pressure, a known vascular risk factor. Using latent variable analyses, we examined whether age differences in regional white matter integrity mediated age-related differences in executive functions and speed of processing. Although diffusion-related latent variables showed stronger age differences than white matter volumes and white matter hyperintensity volumes, only one of them was significantly associated with cognitive performance. Smaller linear anisotropy partially mediated age-related reduction in speed of processing. The effect was significant in posterior (temporal-parietal-occipital) but not anterior (frontal) region, and appeared stronger for cognitive rather than reaction time measures of processing speed. The presence of hypertensive participants did not affect the results. We conclude that in healthy adults, deterioration of axonal integrity and ensuing breech of connectivity may underpin age-related slowing of information processing.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Axônios/fisiologia , Encéfalo/citologia , Encéfalo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Imagem de Tensor de Difusão , Feminino , Humanos , Hipertensão/fisiopatologia , Hipertensão/psicologia , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Estruturais , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Caracteres Sexuais , Adulto Jovem
10.
Neurobiol Aging ; 32(9): 1572-8, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19879667

RESUMO

Atrophy in the medial temporal lobe is generally considered to be highly associated with age-related memory decline. Volume loss in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex has extensively been investigated, but the posterior parts of the parahippocampal gyrus have received little attention. The present MRI study investigated whether volume differences in medial temporal lobe areas are differentially related to age-related memory decline. Thirty-nine subjects from a longitudinal study on cognitive aging (the Maastricht Aging Study) have been examined: 20 participants (mean age=67 years, range 52-80) with memory decline over a period of 12 years were matched to 19 participants without memory decline. Manual tracing was performed on 3T MR images to measure the volumes of the anterior, middle and posterior parts of the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus. A robust group difference and a significant association with memory decline were observed only in the posterior part of the parahippocampal gyrus. Our results may suggest that the posterior parahippocampal gyrus plays a key role in age-related memory decline.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/patologia , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Giro Para-Hipocampal/patologia , Idoso , Atrofia , Biomarcadores , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Progressão da Doença , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/patologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Valor Preditivo dos Testes
11.
Neuropsychologia ; 48(7): 2188-97, 2010 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20399796

RESUMO

Brain aging has been associated with both reduced and increased neural activity during task execution. The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether increased neural activation during memory encoding and retrieval is already present at the age of 60 as well as to obtain more insight into the mechanism behind increased activity. Eighteen young (mean age 21.3) and 18 older (mean age 59.9) right-handed male participants were administered two picture memory tasks in an fMRI environment. Neural activation was measured during encoding and retrieval of pictures of natural scenes (landscapes) and physical objects. Results indicated reduced medial temporal activity during encoding in older participants and increased activity during both encoding and retrieval in several other areas in the brain, including the inferior and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. This increased activation was not related to better memory performance. The present findings indicate that increased neural activation during memory tasks is present in individuals near the age of 60 compared to individuals near the age of 20, which extends findings from studies of more-advanced age groups. Also, increased activation was present even though cognitive performance at 60 was not as impaired as is generally found in more-advanced age groups. Although compensation is a plausible explanation of the increased activation at this age, we suggest that other mechanisms like disinhibition, dedifferentiation, or the recruitment of less-efficient cognitive strategies may be more likely.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adulto , Encéfalo/irrigação sanguínea , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Intenção , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Oxigênio/sangue , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Estatística como Assunto , Adulto Jovem
12.
Child Neuropsychol ; 16(4): 366-87, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20373180

RESUMO

We investigated age-related improvement in semantic category verbal fluency (VF) in 309 Dutch schoolchildren attending first to ninth grade. Quantitative analyses of number of correct responses as a function of time as well as qualitative analyses of clustering and switching were conducted. Overall, Dutch VF task performance, i.e., number of correct responses over 60 seconds, was not established before mid-adolescence. This is in line with previously published studies, using VF number of correct responses over 60 seconds as the main outcome measure and examining VF task performance across other cultures and languages (e.g., Italian, French, Hebrew). Next, mean cluster size, a measure of lexico-semantic knowledge, was not established until at least grade 3. In contrast, performance on the VF outcome measures "number of switches/clusters" was established at least 4 years later. Qualitative and quantitative Design Fluency (DF) outcome measures support the notion that the numbers of switches/clusters are valid measures of higher order cognitive functions, such as strategy use and cognitive flexibility. In line of this, VF number of correct responses during 16-60 seconds, a measure of controlled information processing, is established at least 2 years later (i.e., grades 7-8) than number of correct responses during the first 15 seconds time slide, a measure of automatic processing. Finally, environment, i.e., the level of parental education, primarily affected automatic and lexico-semantic knowledge. No effects of sex on VF performance were found. These data suggest that the alternative scoring methods of VF tasks can be used to acquire knowledge on development of lower and higher order cognitive functions in healthy children and the influence of the environment on it.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Idioma , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Cognição/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fatores Sexuais , Meio Social , Inquéritos e Questionários
13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20207122

RESUMO

Depressive symptoms in the community have a considerable impact on quality of life. Although long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFA) have frequently been implicated in depressed mood, their relationship with quality of life has scarcely been investigated. This study examined the cross-sectional associations between fish consumption and plasma phospholipid LCPUFA status on the one hand, and quality of life, as measured by the Short Form 36 questionnaire, on the other in a population-based sample. The mental health component of quality of life was not associated with LCPUFA status or fish consumption. Fish consumption showed a positive association with physical well-being, which remained significant after correction for LCPUFA status, suggesting that the relationship between fish consumption and physical well-being is independent of the LCPUFA content of fish. These findings indicate that fish consumption may serve as a proxy for a healthy lifestyle or a favorable nutritional status, which is reflected in better quality of life.


Assuntos
Ácidos Graxos Insaturados/sangue , Produtos Pesqueiros , Saúde Mental , Qualidade de Vida , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Animais , Estudos Transversais , Depressão/sangue , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
14.
Psychol Med ; 40(7): 1193-201, 2010 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19903364

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Affective symptoms are common in subjects with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), but there is disagreement whether these symptoms are predictive for Alzheimer's disease (AD). We investigated the predictive accuracy of affective symptoms for AD during a follow-up study in subjects with MCI, and whether the predictive accuracy was modified by age, the presence of amnestic MCI or the length of follow-up. METHOD: Newly referred subjects (n=263) with MCI older than 55 years were selected from a memory clinic and followed up after 2, 5 and 10 years. Predictors investigated were: symptoms of depression, anxiety, apathy and sleeping problems. RESULTS: Affective symptoms were present in 50-70% of the subjects. The average follow-up period was 5.4 years and 79 subjects (29%) developed AD. Sleeping problems were associated with a decreased risk for AD [odds ratio (OR) 0.35, p<0.001]. Symptoms of depression (OR 0.61, p=0.059) and anxiety (OR 0.58, p=0.051) showed a trend in the same direction. The OR of apathy for AD was 0.67 (p=0.14). Depression was associated with a decreased risk for AD only in subjects without amnestic MCI, but not in subjects with amnestic MCI. Moreover, anxiety was related to the risk for AD differently between subjects diagnosed with AD at the 5-year follow-up (OR 0.23) and subjects diagnosed with AD at the 10-year follow-up (OR 1.7). CONCLUSIONS: Affective symptoms are associated with a decreased risk for AD. The risk may be dependent on MCI subtype or length of follow-up, but it does not depend on age.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Doença de Alzheimer/epidemiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Transtorno Depressivo/epidemiologia , Idoso , Transtorno Depressivo/diagnóstico , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/diagnóstico , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/epidemiologia
15.
J Psychopharmacol ; 24(10): 1455-63, 2010 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19304860

RESUMO

In the present study, the effects of transiently lowering central serotonin levels by means of acute tryptophan depletion on measures of cognitive flexibility were examined. Flexible behaviour was measured in an Eriksen flanker task, and cardiac and electro-cortical responses to errors and congruent and incongruent stimuli were measured. The depletion was successful in lowering tryptophan levels and, as expected, it did not affect subjective mood. Depletion did not affect performance and electro-cortical measures and selectively affected cardiac measures. Depletion attenuated cardiac slowing to incongruent flanker stimuli but did not affect cardiac responses to congruent stimuli and errors. The selective effect on cardiac responses as compared to performance and electro-cortical measures was in accordance with earlier findings, as well as the attenuation of cardiac slowing. The selective effect on the cardiac response to incongruent stimuli was unexpected. Detailed analyses showed a close connection to the earlier reported attenuation of the cardiac response to negative feedback, and the effect is explained in terms of reduced anticipation of the feedback stimulus due to enhanced punishment prediction.


Assuntos
Deficiências Nutricionais/fisiopatologia , Frequência Cardíaca/fisiologia , Triptofano/deficiência , Adulto , Cognição/fisiologia , Estudos Cross-Over , Deficiências Nutricionais/sangue , Método Duplo-Cego , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Retroalimentação Psicológica , Humanos , Masculino , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Serotonina/fisiologia , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Triptofano/administração & dosagem , Triptofano/sangue , Adulto Jovem
16.
Neuroimage ; 49(3): 2083-93, 2010 Feb 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19850136

RESUMO

We investigated differences associated with age and hypertension, a common risk factor for vascular disease, in three aspects of white matter integrity--gross regional volumes of the white matter, volume of the white matter hyperintensities (WMH) and diffusion properties. We acquired MRI scans on 93 adult volunteers (age 50-77 years; 36 with diagnosis of hypertension or elevated blood pressure), and obtained all measures in seven brain regions: frontal, temporal, parietal and occipital white matter, and the genu, body and splenium of the corpus callosum. The results demonstrated robust age-related differences in diffusion-based indices of cerebral white matter integrity and age-related increase in the WMH volume, but no age differences in the gross regional volumes of the white matter. Hypertension was associated with decline in fractional anisotropy, and exacerbated age differences in fractional anisotropy more than those in the volume of WMH. These findings indicate that of all examined measures, diffusion-based indices of white matter integrity may be the most sensitive indicators of global and regional declines and vascular damage in the aging brain.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/patologia , Encéfalo/patologia , Hipertensão/patologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Imagem de Tensor de Difusão , Feminino , Humanos , Interpretação de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
17.
Eur J Epidemiol ; 24(10): 641-8, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19718502

RESUMO

The aim of the present study was to determine whether a diagnosis of diabetes mellitus (DM) in a primary setting is associated with an increased risk of subsequent depression. A retrospective cohort design was used based on the Registration Network Family Practice (RNH) database. Patients diagnosed with diabetes mellitus at or after the age of 40 and who were diagnosed between 01-01-1980 and 01-01-2007 (N = 6,140), were compared with age-matched controls from a reference group (N = 18,416) without a history of diabetes. Both groups were followed for an emerging first diagnosis of depression (and/or depressive feelings) until January 1, 2008. 2.0% of the people diagnosed with diabetes mellitus developed a depressive disorder, compared to 1.6% of the reference group. After statistical correction for confounding factors diabetes mellitus was associated with an increased risk of developing subsequent depression (HR 1.26; 95% CI: 1.12-1.42) and/or depressive feelings (HR 1.33; 95% CI: 1.18-1.46). After statistical adjustment practice identification code, age and depression preceding diabetes, were significantly related to a diagnosis of depression. Patients with diabetes mellitus are more likely to develop subsequent depression than persons without a history of diabetes. Results from this large longitudinal study based on a general practice population indicate that this association is weaker than previously found in cross-sectional research using self-report surveys. Several explanations for this dissimilarity are discussed


Assuntos
Depressão/epidemiologia , Transtorno Depressivo Maior/epidemiologia , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 2/epidemiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Estudos de Coortes , Depressão/diagnóstico , Transtorno Depressivo Maior/diagnóstico , Diabetes Mellitus Tipo 2/diagnóstico , Medicina de Família e Comunidade , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Países Baixos/epidemiologia , Estudos Retrospectivos , Medição de Risco , Fatores de Risco , Fatores de Tempo
18.
Tijdschr Gerontol Geriatr ; 40(2): 54-71, 2009 Apr.
Artigo em Holandês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19472572

RESUMO

Regression-based normative data for commonly used semantic verbal fluency test (VFT) scores were derived from data of the Maastricht Aging Study (N=1,825; age range 24-81). Animal naming, profession naming, and the summed score (animal + profession naming) were shown to be profoundly affected by demographical variables, especially age and educational level. The clinical utility of the newly established regression-based VFT norms was evaluated in a large sample of psychogeriatric patients (N=1,063, of which n=890 met the criteria for Alzheimer's or vascular dementia). Results showed that the animal naming VFT score was the most sensitive measure to distinguish normal aging from dementia (when the specificities of the various VFT measures were equated). Fifty patients were found to commit stuck-in-set perseverations, i.e. they generated animal names during test administration of the profession naming VFT. The prevalence of stuck-in-set perseveration was 11.3% in Parkinson dementia and frontal variant frontotemporal dementia, versus 4.6% and 5.3% in dementia of the Alzheimer type and vascular dementia, respectively. Bivariate analyses revealed that people who committed stuck-in-set perseverations were more deficient on a measure of free recall and on several tests of executive control, such as the Expanded Mental Control Test, the Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale and the Alternating Graphical Sequences Test as a measure of response inhibition. Using multivariate logistic regression analysis, only the Alternating Graphical Sequences Test retained an independent effect in the association with stuck-in-set perseveration.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Demência/diagnóstico , Demência/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/normas , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Doença de Alzheimer/psicologia , Demência Vascular/diagnóstico , Demência Vascular/psicologia , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Escolaridade , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise Multivariada , Semântica , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Terminologia como Assunto , Adulto Jovem
19.
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord ; 27(2): 173-81, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19202336

RESUMO

BACKGROUND/AIMS: In population studies, different mild cognitive impairment (MCI) definitions have been used to predict dementia at a later stage. This study compared predictive values of different MCI definitions for dementia, and the effect of age on the predictive values was investigated. METHODS: This study was conducted as part of an ongoing longitudinal study into the determinants of cognitive aging, the Maastricht Aging Study. RESULTS: MCI best predicted dementia when multiple cognitive domains were considered and subjective complaints were not (sensitivity: 0.66, specificity: 0.78). Age had a strong influence on the sensitivity of MCI for dementia (age 60-70 years: sensitivity = 0.56; age 70-85 years: sensitivity = 0.70). CONCLUSION: The inclusion of multiple cognitive domains and participants aged 70 years and older leads to the best prediction of dementia, regardless of subjective complaints.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Demência/psicologia , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Área Sob a Curva , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Demência/epidemiologia , Progressão da Doença , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Países Baixos/epidemiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Curva ROC
20.
Acta Psychiatr Scand ; 120(1): 45-52, 2009 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19133876

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Previous studies have reported an association between depression and poor cognitive functioning. Unknown is to what degree such associations are merely state-related or reflect an enduring depression vulnerability. This study examined whether cognitive deficits predict current and/or follow-up (sub)clinical depressive symptoms in the general population. METHOD: A population-based sample of 569 female twins and 43 of their sisters completed a neuropsychological battery. Cross-sectional and prospective associations between depressive symptoms measured at the subclinical [Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90)] and clinical level (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV disorders) and neuropsychological factors (episodic memory and information processing speed) were examined. RESULTS: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV disorders baseline depressive symptoms were significantly associated with information processing speed but not with episodic memory. Episodic memory was significantly associated with follow-up SCL-90 depressive symptoms. CONCLUSION: Being depressed is accompanied by slower information processing. Poor memory functioning may be a predictor for the onset of subclinical depressive symptoms.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Transtorno Depressivo/epidemiologia , Doenças em Gêmeos/epidemiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/genética , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Comorbidade , Estudos Transversais , Transtorno Depressivo/diagnóstico , Transtorno Depressivo/genética , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Doenças em Gêmeos/diagnóstico , Doenças em Gêmeos/genética , Doenças em Gêmeos/psicologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Países Baixos , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicometria , Fatores de Risco , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA