Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 48
Filtrar
1.
Toxics ; 12(2)2024 Feb 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38393221

RESUMO

To develop the risk prediction technology for mixture toxicity, a reliable and extensive dataset of experimental results is required. However, most published literature only provides data on combinations containing two or three substances, resulting in a limited dataset for predicting the toxicity of complex mixtures. Complex mixtures may have different mode of actions (MoAs) due to their varied composition, posing difficulty in the prediction using conventional toxicity prediction models, such as the concentration addition (CA) and independent action (IA) models. The aim of this study was to generate an experimental dataset comprising complex mixtures. To identify the target complex mixtures, we referred to the findings of the HBM4EU project. We identified three groups of seven to ten components that were commonly detected together in human bodies, namely environmental phenols, perfluorinated compounds, and heavy metal compounds, assuming these chemicals to have different MoAs. In addition, a separate mixture was added consisting of seven organophosphate flame retardants (OPFRs), which may have similar chemical structures. All target substances were tested for cytotoxicity using HepG2 cell lines, and subsequently 50 different complex mixtures were randomly generated with equitoxic mixtures of EC10 levels. To determine the interaction effect, we calculated the model deviation ratio (MDR) by comparing the observed EC10 with the predicted EC10 from the CA model, then categorized three types of interactions: antagonism, additivity, and synergism. Dose-response curves and EC values were calculated for all complex mixtures. Out of 50 mixtures, none demonstrated synergism, while six mixtures exhibited an antagonistic effect. The remaining mixtures exhibited additivity with MDRs ranging from 0.50 to 1.34. Our experimental data have been formatted to and constructed for the database. They will be utilized for further research aimed at developing the combined CA/IA approaches to support mixture risk assessment.

2.
Brain Sci ; 13(2)2023 Jan 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36831701

RESUMO

Reward learning and aversive conditioning have consequences for attentional selection, such that stimuli that come to signal reward and threat bias attention regardless of their valence. Appetitive and aversive stimuli have distinctive influences on response selection, such that they activate an approach and an avoidance response, respectively. However, whether the involuntary influence of reward- and threat-history-laden stimuli extends to the manner in which a response is directed remains unclear. Using a feedback-joystick task and a manikin task, which are common paradigms for examining valence-action bias, we demonstrate that reward- and threat-signalling stimuli do not modulate response selection. Stimuli that came to signal reward and threat via training biased attention and invigorated action in general, but they did not facilitate an approach and avoidance response, respectively. We conclude that attention can be biased towards a stimulus as a function of its prior association with reward or aversive outcomes without necessarily influencing approach vs. avoidance tendencies, such that the mechanisms underlying the involuntary control of attention and behaviour evoked by valent stimuli can be decoupled.

3.
Behav Neurosci ; 137(2): 89-94, 2023 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36521140

RESUMO

Attention is biased toward stimuli previously associated with reward. The same is true for aversive conditioning; stimuli previously associated with an aversive outcome also bias attention, suggesting that motivational salience guides attention. Most research that supports this conclusion has manipulated monetary gain-a secondary reinforcer-for reward learning, and electric shocks-a primary punisher-for aversive conditioning, making it difficult to directly compare their influence on attention. Therefore, in the present study, we matched for reinforcer dimensions by using primary taste as reinforcers/punishers and assessed their influence on attention. In a training phase, participants learned to associate three colors with sweet juice (reward), salt water (aversive), and no outcome (neutral), respectively. The two primary reinforcers were equated for valence based on choices made in a prior decision-making task. In a later test phase, these three colors were used for targets and distractors in a task in which participants oriented to a shape-defined target. An attentional bias in favor of the aversively conditioned and reward-associated colors was evident when comparing to the neutral color. Importantly, a direct comparison of rewarded and aversive stimuli revealed no significant differences. These results suggest that when matched for reinforcer dimensions and valence, reward and aversive outcomes bias attention in a similar manner and their effects are comparable, providing further evidence in support of the motivational salience account of learning-dependent attention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Viés de Atenção , Humanos , Atenção , Recompensa , Afeto , Condicionamento Psicológico
4.
Vision Res ; 202: 108155, 2023 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36417810

RESUMO

Physically salient but task-irrelevant stimuli have high attentional priority, although observers are able to capitalize on statistical regularities in the environment to more efficiently ignore such stimuli. Physically salient distractors that more frequently appear in a particular location are less distracting when they appear in this high probability location. Likewise, colors and orientations that are frequently associated with distractors become preferentially ignored with learning. Such statistically learned distractor suppression has been examined with respect to the frequency of elementary features across trials, and less is known about how statistics concerning the composition of distractor features within a trial influence attention, particularly with respect to how orientations combine to form shapes. Color, orientation, and location are also represented very early in vision, whereas more complex features such as shape are represented further downstream in the visual system; it remains unclear whether statistically leaned distractor suppression can operate over such downstream visual representations. In the present study, we demonstrate attentional capture by physically salient, shape-defined distractors that is reduced in magnitude for a high probability shape. Our findings demonstrate that statistical learning can modulate attentional priority at least at the level of basic shapes and is not restricted to modulations of priority at the earliest stages of visual information processing tied to elementary features.


Assuntos
Atenção , Aprendizagem , Humanos , Tempo de Reação , Percepção Visual , Cognição
5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(2): 277-283, 2023 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36536205

RESUMO

Attention is biased in favor of stimuli that signal either threat or reward; this experience-dependent attentional bias develops via associative learning and persists into extinction. Physically salient yet task-irrelevant stimuli are also prioritized by the attention system, but the attentional priority of a physically salient distractor can be suppressed when it appears in a location in which it has been frequently encountered in the past. Similar effects of statistical learning on distractor suppression have been observed for distractors appearing in a predictable color. A pair of recent studies demonstrate that statistically learned distractor suppression and valence-based attentional biases combine additively, suggesting independent influences of learning on attentional priority. One limitation of these prior studies, however, is that the effects of statistical learning were defined with respect to spatial attention and the effects of associative learning with respect to feature-based attention. A strong version of the independence account would predict additive influences on attention even when both sources of priority are represented within a single domain of attentional control, which we tested in the present study. The attentional priority of a distractor was elevated when its color was previously associated with electric shock and reduced when its shape was frequently encountered as a distractor in a prior training phase, with these two influences on priority combining additively. Our findings provide strong evidence for the idea that statistical learning and valance-based associative learning exert independent influences on the control of attention, which has implications for contemporary theories of selection history.


Assuntos
Atenção , Viés de Atenção , Humanos , Tempo de Reação , Aprendizagem , Condicionamento Clássico
6.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36429938

RESUMO

Although South Korea has implemented various smoking cessation services, women who are emotional laborers, as well as parcel delivery, transportation, and construction workers, have poor access to these services. This study evaluated the smoking-related characteristics of workers in these four occupations as well as the awareness of and need for smoking cessation services. In total 808 workers in these four occupations aged 19 years and above were recruited nationwide and had their data analyzed. The participants' age, marital status, number of work hours per week, job-related stress, age when they started smoking, average number of cigarettes a day, types of tobacco products, close relationships to others who smoke, number of attempts to quit smoking, plans to quit smoking, awareness of cessation services, prior utilization of cessation services, and need for cessation services were surveyed. Compared with parcel delivery workers, female emotional laborers and transportation and construction workers had more attempts to quit smoking, plans to quit smoking, and prior utilization of smoking cessation services, moreover, construction workers had a significantly lower awareness of smoking cessation services. Parcel delivery workers need smoking cessation programs, mobile applications to help them quit smoking, and improvements in their work environments. Cessation services and education should be promoted at workplaces and among managers.


Assuntos
Indústria da Construção , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar , Humanos , Feminino , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , República da Coreia/epidemiologia , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Ocupações
7.
Nanomaterials (Basel) ; 12(7)2022 Mar 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35407233

RESUMO

The alignment of nanowires (NWs) has been actively pursued for the production of electrical devices with high-operating performances. Among the generally available alignment processes, spin-coating is the simplest and fastest method for uniformly patterning the NWs. During spinning, the morphology of the aligned NWs is sensitively influenced by the resultant external drag and inertial forces. Herein, the assembly of highly and uniaxially aligned silicon nanowires (Si NWs) is achieved by introducing an off-center spin-coating method in which the applied external forces are modulated by positioning the target substrate away from the center of rotation. In addition, various influencing factors, such as the type of solvent, the spin acceleration time, the distance between the substrate and the center of rotation, and the surface energy of the substrate, are adjusted in order to optimize the alignment of the NWs. Next, a field-effect transistor (FET) incorporating the highly aligned Si NWs exhibits a high effective mobility of up to 85.7 cm2 V-1 s-1, and an on-current of 0.58 µA. Finally, the single device is enlarged and developed in order to obtain an ultrathin and flexible Si NW FET array. The resulting device has the potential to be widely expanded into applications such as wearable electronics and robotic systems.

8.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 130: 326-350, 2021 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34499927

RESUMO

The last ten years of attention research have witnessed a revolution, replacing a theoretical dichotomy (top-down vs. bottom-up control) with a trichotomy (biased by current goals, physical salience, and selection history). This third new mechanism of attentional control, selection history, is multifaceted. Some aspects of selection history must be learned over time whereas others reflect much more transient influences. A variety of different learning experiences can shape the attention system, including reward, aversive outcomes, past experience searching for a target, target‒non-target relations, and more. In this review, we provide an overview of the historical forces that led to the proposal of selection history as a distinct mechanism of attentional control. We then propose a formal definition of selection history, with concrete criteria, and identify different components of experience-driven attention that fit within this definition. The bulk of the review is devoted to exploring how these different components relate to one another. We conclude by proposing an integrative account of selection history centered on underlying themes that emerge from our review.


Assuntos
Atenção , Recompensa , Afeto , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Tempo de Reação
9.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 33(12): 2440-2460, 2021 11 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34407195

RESUMO

Rewarding and aversive outcomes have opposing effects on behavior, facilitating approach and avoidance, although we need to accurately anticipate each type of outcome to behave effectively. Attention is biased toward stimuli that have been learned to predict either type of outcome, and it remains an open question whether such orienting is driven by separate systems for value- and threat-based orienting or whether there exists a common underlying mechanism of attentional control driven by motivational salience. Here, we provide a direct comparison of the neural correlates of value- and threat-based attentional capture after associative learning. Across multiple measures of behavior and brain activation, our findings overwhelmingly support a motivational salience account of the control of attention. We conclude that there exists a core mechanism of experience-dependent attentional control driven by motivational salience and that prior characterizations of attention as being value driven or supporting threat monitoring need to be revisited.


Assuntos
Motivação , Recompensa , Condicionamento Clássico , Humanos , Aprendizagem , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33804218

RESUMO

Emotional labor is paid work that involves managing and regulating one's emotions during the job including evoking and suppressing one's feelings. This study examined the factors associated with successful smoking cessation through tailored smoking cessation counseling including stress management among female emotional labor workers. The study was conducted from 1 September 2015 to 31 December 2017. A total of 2674 women registered in the Comprehensive Smoking Cessation Service System and were grouped as either emotional labor workers for service and sales (1002) or other occupations (1672) for analysis. The participants received nine sessions of face-to-face and telephone smoking cessation counseling over 6 months, and follow-up assessments were conducted 4, 6, 12, and 24 weeks after counseling. Smoking cessation counseling involved a stress management program comprising stress tests, depression tests, color therapy, and a buddy program including peer support. Factors associated with successful smoking cessation included the number of counseling sessions, motivation rulers (Importance, Confidence, Readiness), average daily smoking amount, expired carbon monoxide (CO), and nicotine dependence. The most associated factor was the number of counseling sessions. Since counseling focused on stress management, it was the most important factor in smoking cessation, and continuous counseling could help those wanting to quit smoking.


Assuntos
Abandono do Hábito de Fumar , Aconselhamento , Emoções , Feminino , Humanos , Ocupações , República da Coreia
11.
Cognition ; 208: 104554, 2021 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33360961

RESUMO

Selection history exerts a powerful influence on the control of attention. Stimuli signalling reward and punishment capture attention even when physically non-salient and task-irrelevant. Repeated presentation of a salient distractor at a particular location generates learned suppression, resulting in reduced attentional processing at that location. A debate in the field concerns whether different components of selection history influence attention via a common underlying mechanism of learning-dependent control or via distinct, independent mechanisms. We probed this question with a particular focus on reward/punishment history and learned suppression. Participants were trained to suppress a particular location (high probability distractor location) and associate colours with reward or no outcome (no-reward). In a subsequent task, reward and no-reward distractors appeared in all locations equally often. In a separate experiment, we replaced reward with electric shocks. Reward and shock distractors captured attention more strongly than no-reward and no-shock distractors irrespective of their location. Distractors appearing in the high probability location showed reduced capture irrespective of their type. The results imply that reward and punishment learning and learned suppression have independent influences on the attentional system.


Assuntos
Atenção , Aprendizagem , Humanos , Punição , Tempo de Reação , Recompensa
12.
Emotion ; 21(4): 898-903, 2021 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32718173

RESUMO

Learning about aversive outcomes plays a role in the guidance of attention. Classical conditioning generates a bias to predictors of aversive outcomes, whereas instrumental learning potentiates a negatively reinforced avoidance behavior, which can be difficult to dissociate in the case of attention to aversively conditioned stimuli. The present study examined the relative contribution from these two learning processes to the control of attention. Participants were first provided an opportunity to avoid an electric shock by generating a saccade in the direction opposite one of two stimuli. In contradiction to the practiced avoidance behavior, such training resulted in a bias to orient toward the shock-associated stimulus, indicative of a more dominant role of classical conditioning in the control of attention. The findings are in parallel with the influence of positive reinforcement on attention, suggesting that the attention system may be guided by motivational relevance rather than a particular emotional valence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Atenção , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Condicionamento Clássico , Reforço Psicológico , Adolescente , Condicionamento Operante , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivação , Adulto Jovem
13.
Motiv Sci ; 7(2): 165-175, 2021 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35342778

RESUMO

Attention prioritizes stimuli previously associated with punishment. Despite the importance of this process for survival and adaptation, the potential generalization of punishment-related attentional biases has been largely ignored in the literature. This study aimed to determine whether stimulus-punishment associations learned in a specific context bias attention in another context (in which the stimulus was never paired with punishment). We examined this issue using an antisaccade task in which participants had to shift their gaze in the opposite direction of a colored square during stimulus-outcome learning. Two contexts and three colors were employed. One color was associated with punishment (i.e., electrical shock) in one context and never paired with punishment in the other context. For a second color, the punishment-context relationship was reversed. A third color never paired with shock in either context (neutral) was included in Experiment 1 but absent in Experiment 2. Participants then performed search for a shape-defined target in an extinction phase (in which no shock was delivered) in which attentional bias for the colors was assessed. Context was manipulated via the background image upon which the stimuli were presented. In each of the two experiments, a bias to selectively orient toward the color that had been associated with punishment in the current context was observed, suggesting that punishment-modulated attentional priority is context specific.

14.
J Korean Med Sci ; 35(43): e391, 2020 Nov 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33169560

RESUMO

Since mid-April 2020, cases of multisystem inflammatory syndrome in children (MIS-C) associated with coronavirus disease 2019 that mimics Kawasaki disease (KD) have been reported in Europe and North America. However, no cases have been reported in Korea. We describe an 11-year old boy with fever, abdominal pain, and diarrhea who developed hypotension requiring inotropes in intensive care unit. His blood test revealed elevated inflammatory markers, thrombocytopenia, hypoalbuminemia, and coagulopathy. Afterward, he developed signs of KD such as conjunctival injection, strawberry tongue, cracked lip, and coronary artery dilatation, and parenchymal consolidation without respiratory symptoms. Microbiological tests were all negative including severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction. However, serum immunoglobulin G against SARS-CoV-2 was positive in repeated tests using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay and fluorescent immunoassay. He was recovered well after intravenous immunoglobulin administration and discharged without complication on hospital day 13. We report the first Korean child who met all the criteria of MIS-C with features of incomplete KD or KD shock syndrome.


Assuntos
Infecções por Coronavirus/patologia , Pneumonia Viral/patologia , Síndrome de Resposta Inflamatória Sistêmica/diagnóstico , Abdome/diagnóstico por imagem , Anticorpos Antivirais/sangue , Betacoronavirus/genética , Betacoronavirus/imunologia , COVID-19 , Criança , Infecções por Coronavirus/complicações , Infecções por Coronavirus/virologia , Humanos , Imunoglobulinas Intravenosas/administração & dosagem , Masculino , Síndrome de Linfonodos Mucocutâneos/patologia , Pandemias , Pneumonia Viral/complicações , Pneumonia Viral/virologia , SARS-CoV-2 , Síndrome de Resposta Inflamatória Sistêmica/complicações , Tórax/diagnóstico por imagem , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X , Ultrassonografia
15.
J Community Health ; 45(5): 997-1005, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32303921

RESUMO

The purpose of this study was to identify factors that successfully influence to quit smoking in female college students. The study was conducted from September 1, 2015 to December 31, 2017. Participants included 477 female student smokers, the average age was 20.87 years. The participants received smoking cessation counseling over the course of nine visits and via telephone calls for a period of 6 months. Follow-up assessments were conducted at 4, 6, 12, and 24 weeks. The factors significantly associated with successful smoking cessation included the average number of cigarettes smoked per day, age of first smoking experience, nicotine dependency, number of counseling sessions, confidence, and readiness. The most influential factor was the number of counseling sessions. Hence, this study suggested to promote continuous counseling and to introduce counseling programs tailored to female students, which might increase their success rate of quitting.


Assuntos
Fumantes , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar , Adulto , Aconselhamento/estatística & dados numéricos , Feminino , Humanos , República da Coreia , Fumantes/psicologia , Fumantes/estatística & dados numéricos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/métodos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/estatística & dados numéricos , Estudantes , Universidades , Adulto Jovem
16.
J Affect Disord ; 263: 437-444, 2020 02 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31969275

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Given that current unimodal strategies for treating late-life depression are insufficient, the awareness of the necessity and importance of multidomain intervention has increased. We assessed the efficacy of multidomain intervention in reducing symptoms of late-life depression. METHODS: This was a 12-week community-based randomized controlled trial in 78 older adults diagnosed with major depressive disorder. Participants were randomly assigned to the multidomain intervention or supportive therapy group. We provided four home visits and 12 telephone calls over 12 weeks. Four therapeutic approaches (physical activity, healthy diet, social activity, and brief cognitive restructuring) were incorporated into the multidomain intervention. The primary outcome was the change in depressive symptoms, as measured by the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS). Secondarily, we investigated changes in resting-state functional connectivity. RESULTS: The MADRS total score was reduced more in the multidomain intervention group than in the supportive therapy group during the 12 weeks (intervention × time interaction, P = =0.007). After correction for multiple comparisons, the multidomain intervention group exhibited a lower MADRS total score at week 12 (score difference 5.117; P = =0.029). At follow-up, the multidomain intervention group also exhibited less functional connectivity between the posterior cingulate cortex and left inferior parietal lobule within the default mode network (FDR < 0.1). LIMITATIONS: Caution is needed in the interpretation of the results, considering the small sample size and high percentage of female participants. CONCLUSIONS: A 12-week multidomain intervention resulted in a greater reduction of depressive symptoms among the elderly with major depressive disorder than their counterparts who received supportive therapy.


Assuntos
Terapia Cognitivo-Comportamental , Transtorno Depressivo Maior , Dieta , Exercício Físico , Idoso , Depressão , Transtorno Depressivo Maior/terapia , Feminino , Giro do Cíngulo , Humanos , Comportamento Social , Resultado do Tratamento
17.
Psychol Res ; 84(8): 2122-2137, 2020 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31190092

RESUMO

Reward history is a powerful determinant of what we pay attention to. This influence of reward on attention varies substantially across individuals, being related to a variety of personality variables and clinical conditions. Currently, the ability to measure and quantify attention-to-reward is restricted to the use of psychophysical laboratory tasks, which limits research into the construct in a variety of ways. In the present study, we introduce a questionnaire designed to provide a brief and accessible means of assessing attention-to-reward. Scores on the questionnaire correlate with other measures known to be related to attention-to-reward and predict performance on multiple laboratory tasks measuring the construct. In demonstrating this relationship, we also provide evidence that attention-to-reward as measured in the lab, an automatic and implicit bias in information processing, is related to overt behaviors and motivations in everyday life as assessed via the questionnaire. Variation in scores on the questionnaire is additionally associated with a distinct biomarker in brain connectivity, and the questionnaire exhibits acceptable test-retest reliability. Overall, the Value-Driven Attention Questionnaire (VDAQ) provides a useful proxy-measure of attention-to-reward that is much more accessible than typical laboratory assessments.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Motivação/fisiologia , Recompensa , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo , Núcleo Caudado/fisiologia , Cognição , Estimulação Elétrica , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Feminino , Neuroimagem Funcional , Humanos , Individualidade , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Punição , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Área Tegmentar Ventral/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31547229

RESUMO

There were high smoking rates among young male college students in Korea. We aimed to investigate the prevalence of and factors affecting abstinence from smoking following smoking cessation service attendance in this population. Data were collected between 1 August 2015 and 20 August 2018. Participants were administered more than nine face-to-face and telephone counseling sessions by trained tobacco cessation specialists for six months. Follow-up assessments were conducted at 4, 6, and 12 weeks, and 6 months after the quit date. A total of 3978 male college student smokers were enrolled; their mean age was 23.17 (±3.45) years. Almost one-third of the participants (64.9%) reported that they had attempted to quit during the past year. The number of cigarettes smoked per day, CO ppm, and Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence score at the baseline were negatively associated with abstinence, while motivational variables-importance, confidence, and readiness-were positively associated with abstinence. Our results suggest that provision of visiting smoking cessation services can be an active intervention platform for college student smokers who need professional assistance or support. Active and accessible support should be provided to such people. Visiting a smoking cessation service may result in increased long-term abstinence rates in such students.


Assuntos
Fumantes/psicologia , Fumantes/estatística & dados numéricos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Estudantes/psicologia , Estudantes/estatística & dados numéricos , Fumar Tabaco/psicologia , Tabagismo/psicologia , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , República da Coreia , Estudos Retrospectivos , Adulto Jovem
19.
Front Psychiatry ; 10: 615, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31555158

RESUMO

Background: It has been suggested that maintaining the efficient organization of the brain's functional connectivity (FC) supports neuroflexibility under neurogenerative stress. This study examined psychological resilience-related FC in 112 older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Methods: Using a resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) approach, we investigated reorganization of the orbitofrontal gyrus (OFG)/amygdala (AMG)/hippocampus (HP)/parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) FC according to the different levels of resilience scale. Results: Compared with the low resilient group, the high resilient group had greater connectivity strengths between the left inferior OFG and right superior OFG (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected), between the right inferior OFG and left PHG (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected), and between the right middle OFG and left PHG (false discovery rate < 0.05). Conclusion: Psychological resilience may be associated with enhancement of the orbitofrontal network in the elderly with MCI.

20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31554231

RESUMO

Intersection and non-intersection locations are commonly used as spatial units of analysis for modeling pedestrian crashes. While both location types have been previously studied, comparing results is difficult given the different data and methods used to identify crash-risk locations. In this study, a systematic and replicable protocol was developed in GIS (Geographic Information System) to create a consistent spatial unit of analysis for use in pedestrian crash modelling. Four publicly accessible datasets were used to identify unique intersection and non-intersection locations: Roadway intersection points, roadway lanes, legal speed limits, and pedestrian crash records. Two algorithms were developed and tested using five search radii (ranging from 20 to 100 m) to assess the protocol reliability. The algorithms, which were designed to identify crash-risk locations at intersection and non-intersection areas detected 87.2% of the pedestrian crash locations (r: 20 m). Agreement rates between algorithm results and the crash data were 94.1% for intersection and 98.0% for non-intersection locations, respectively. The buffer size of 20 m generally showed the highest performance in the analyses. The present protocol offered an efficient and reliable method to create spatial analysis units for pedestrian crash modeling. It provided researchers a cost-effective method to identify unique intersection and non-intersection locations. Additional search radii should be tested in future studies to refine the capture of crash-risk locations.


Assuntos
Acidentes de Trânsito , Modelos Teóricos , Pedestres , Algoritmos , Sistemas de Informação Geográfica , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA