Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 12 de 12
Filtrar
1.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 35(2): 310-315, 2017 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28090654

RESUMO

Pictures are named more slowly in the context of semantically related pictures than in the context of unrelated pictures. This semantic blocking effect has been studied extensively in adult participants, and one study has revealed its presence in 6-year-old children. However, little is known about the development of the effect with age. In this study, a blocked cyclic naming procedure was arranged for 5- to 7-year-old and 10- to 12-year-old children. The semantic blocking effect obtained did not differ in size between the two age groups. This finding is tentatively interpreted as evidence that the semantic blocking effect does not have the same underlying cause as interference effects typically observed in naming tasks involving a distractor stimulus, like the Stroop task. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? The semantic blocking effect has been demonstrated in adults, but little is known about its development in childhood. Age-related changes in performance in children have been used to distinguish various types of inhibitory control. What does this study add? A semantic blocking effect was obtained in 5- to 7-year-old children and - for the first time - in 10- to 12-year-old children. In the two age groups, the effect was equal in size and did not show up in the first cycles of the experiment. The findings are argued to be in line with the distinction unintentional vs. intentional inhibitory control.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Semântica
2.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 24(3): 721-733, 2017 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27714665

RESUMO

The picture-word interference (PWI) paradigm and the Stroop color-word interference task are often assumed to reflect the same underlying processes. On the basis of a PRP study, Dell'Acqua et al. (Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14: 717-722, 2007) argued that this assumption is incorrect. In this article, we first discuss the definitions of Stroop- and picture-word interference. Next, we argue that both effects consist of at least four components that correspond to four characteristics of the distractor word: (1) response-set membership, (2) task relevance, (3) semantic relatedness, and (4) lexicality. On the basis of this theoretical analysis, we conclude that the typical Stroop effect and the typical PWI effect mainly differ in the relative contributions of these four components. Finally, the results of an interference task are reported in which only the nature of the target - color or picture - was manipulated and all other distractor task characteristics were kept constant. The results showed no difference between color and picture targets with respect to all behavioral measures examined. We conclude that the assumption that the same processes underlie verbal interference in color and picture naming is warranted.


Assuntos
Atenção , Teste de Stroop , Percepção Visual , Adolescente , Percepção de Cores , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Semântica , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adulto Jovem
3.
Percept Mot Skills ; 116(1): 78-90, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23829136

RESUMO

Children 5 to 8 years of age find it harder to name the color of a meaningful picture (e.g., of a table) than to name the color of an abstract form. Previous research suggests that this color-object interference effect is most likely due to competition between the task set of color-naming and the children's prepotent tendency to name the picture's identity. The present article examined whether color-object interference diminishes when cues are provided that have been shown to facilitate the segregation of the relevant (color) attribute and the irrelevant (form) attribute of the stimulus. Experiment 1 examined the effect of a spatial separation, and Experiment 2 the effect of a temporal separation between color and picture. Both manipulations resulted in the elimination of color-object interference, suggesting that children 5 to 8 years old were able to use these cues to overcome task competition.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fatores de Tempo
4.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 66(10): 2023-38, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23510000

RESUMO

English words with an inconsistent grapheme-to-phoneme conversion or with more than one pronunciation ("homographic heterophones"; e.g., "lead"-/lεd/, /lid/) are read aloud more slowly than matched controls, presumably due to competition processes. In Japanese kanji, the majority of the characters have multiple readings for the same orthographic unit: the native Japanese reading (KUN) and the derived Chinese reading (ON). This leads to the question of whether reading these characters also shows processing costs. Studies examining this issue have provided mixed evidence. The current study addressed the question of whether processing of these kanji characters leads to the simultaneous activation of their KUN and ON reading, This was measured in a direct way in a masked priming paradigm. In addition, we assessed whether the relative frequencies of the KUN and ON pronunciations ("dominance ratio", measured in compound words) affect the amount of priming. The results of two experiments showed that: (a) a single kanji, presented as a masked prime, facilitates the reading of the (katakana transcriptions of) their KUN and ON pronunciations; however, (b) this was most consistently found when the dominance ratio was around 50% (no strong dominance towards either pronunciation) and when the dominance was towards the ON reading (high-ON group). When the dominance was towards the KUN reading (high-KUN group), no significant priming for the ON reading was observed. Implications for models of kanji processing are discussed.


Assuntos
Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Fonética , Psicolinguística , Leitura , Estimulação Acústica , Adolescente , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
5.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 65(11): 2155-68, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22630114

RESUMO

In naming a picture at the basic level, a semantically related distractor word induces interference in comparison to an unrelated word. When the task is changed from basic-level naming to categorization, however, this effect reverses to semantic facilitation. In previous studies, this semantic facilitation effect was attributed to "message congruency" at the conceptual level. The present study examines the nature of this message congruency effect: Is it due to competition between two activated category concepts in the incongruent condition or is it due to convergence of activity on a single category concept in the congruent condition? Two experiments show that neither the strength with which the context stimulus activates an incongruent category concept nor the semantic distance between the category concepts activated by target and distractor affect target categorization speed. We conclude that the message congruency effect is most likely due to convergence on a single category concept in the category-congruent condition.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Semântica , Fala/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Estudantes , Universidades , Vocabulário
6.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 139(2): 272-80, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22305348

RESUMO

Picture naming is facilitated when a target picture (e.g. of a cat) is accompanied by a form-related context word (e.g. CAP) relative to an unrelated word (e.g. PEN). Because in alphabetic languages phonological and orthographic similarity are confounded, Chinese, a logographic language, has been employed to study these two effects in isolation. The results obtained suggest that the orthographic facilitation effect is localized at an earlier processing level than the phonological facilitation effect. In the present study we examine this issue again, using an experimental design in which the context words in the related and unrelated conditions are optimally matched. In contrast to the earlier studies Experiments 1 and 2 fail to show differences in the time course of the two context effects. Moreover, Experiment 3 provides direct evidence against an early, conceptual locus of orthographic facilitation. Our findings indicate that in Chinese language production both orthographically and phonologically related context words have their effect at the rather late level of word-form encoding.


Assuntos
Linguística , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Fonética , Fala , China , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Semântica
7.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 64(9): 1836-49, 2011 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21722063

RESUMO

The current study investigated the effects of phonologically related context pictures on the naming latencies of target words in Japanese and Chinese. Reading bare words in alphabetic languages has been shown to be rather immune to effects of context stimuli, even when these stimuli are presented in advance of the target word (e.g., Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984 ; Roelofs, 2003 ). However, recently, semantic context effects of distractor pictures on the naming latencies of Japanese kanji (but not Chinese hànzì) words have been observed (Verdonschot, La Heij, & Schiller, 2010 ). In the present study, we further investigated this issue using phonologically related (i.e., homophonic) context pictures when naming target words in either Chinese or Japanese. We found that pronouncing bare nouns in Japanese is sensitive to phonologically related context pictures, whereas this is not the case in Chinese. The difference between these two languages is attributed to processing costs caused by multiple pronunciations for Japanese kanji.


Assuntos
Nomes , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Fonética , Semântica , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Povo Asiático , Atenção/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 108(1): 156-69, 2011 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20869723

RESUMO

Young children are slower in naming the color of a meaningful picture than in naming the color of an abstract form (Stroop-like color-object interference). The current experiments tested an executive control account of this phenomenon. First, color-object interference was observed in 6- and 8-year-olds but not in 12- and 16-year-olds (Experiment 1). Second, meaningful pictures did not interfere in 5- to 7-year-olds' manual sorting of objects on the basis of color (Experiment 2) or in their naming the number of colored objects in the display, that is, subitizing (Experiment 3). These findings provide support for the view that color-object interference results from the children's immature inhibition of the prepotent but irrelevant task of object naming.


Assuntos
Função Executiva , Adolescente , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Percepção de Cores , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Teste de Stroop
9.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 34(2): 302-12, 2008 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18315407

RESUMO

It has been claimed that bilingualism enhances inhibitory control, but the available evidence is equivocal. The authors evaluated several possible versions of the inhibition hypothesis by comparing monolinguals and bilinguals with regard to stop signal performance, inhibition of return, and the attentional blink. These three phenomena, it can be argued, tap into different aspects of inhibition. Monolinguals and bilinguals did not differ in stop signal reaction time and thus were comparable in terms of active-inhibitory efficiency. However, bilinguals showed no facilitation from spatial cues, showed a strong inhibition of return effect, and exhibited a more pronounced attentional blink. These results suggest that bilinguals do not differ from monolinguals in terms of active inhibition but have acquired a better ability to maintain action goals and to use them to bias goal-related information. Under some circumstances, this ability may indirectly lead to more pronounced reactive inhibition of irrelevant information.


Assuntos
Atenção , Inibição Psicológica , Multilinguismo , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto , Percepção de Cores , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Tempo de Reação
10.
Psychol Rep ; 100(3 Pt 1): 838-46, 2007 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17688103

RESUMO

For bilingual persons, comprehension of a word in a second language (L2 word) could be achieved via an indirect route, in which the L2 word is first translated into the first language (L1) before meaning is accessed, or via a direct route, in which an L2 word directly activates its meaning. To test these two accounts, proficient Dutch-English bilinguals were asked to translate and to categorize L2 words of high and low familiarity. These L2 words were accompanied by a Dutch context word that was either phonologically related or unrelated to its Dutch translation equivalent. The results showed a clear phonological facilitation effect in the translation task but no phonological facilitation in the categorization task. This result was taken as evidence for a "direct route" from the L2 word to its meaning.


Assuntos
Cognição , Semântica , Percepção da Fala , Vocabulário , Humanos , Multilinguismo , Tempo de Reação
11.
Cortex ; 42(7): 1028-31; discussion 1032-6, 2006 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17172183

RESUMO

On the basis of two empirical observations, Finkbeiner and Caramazza (2006) take issue with the generally accepted interpretation of semantic interference in the picture-word interference task in terms of lexical competition. As an alternative, they propose a response-selection account, in which semantic interference is attributed to the time needed to remove the inappropriate (word-reading) response from an output buffer. In this comment we argue that the empirical work discussed provides an interesting challenge for current models of language production, but that the authors' alternative account is at variance with at least three robust empirical findings in the language production literature.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fala/fisiologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Humanos , Aprendizagem por Associação de Pares/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Semântica , Medida da Produção da Fala
12.
Psychol Res ; 67(1): 30-42, 2003 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12589448

RESUMO

Stroop dilution refers to the observation that the impact of a color word on the naming of a color bar is reduced if another word-like object is displayed simultaneously. Recently, Brown, Roos-Gilbert, and Carr (1995) concluded that Stroop dilution is due to early-visual interference. This conclusion was evaluated in three experiments. Experiment 1 showed that, contrary to the predictions of an early-visual interference account, (a) diluters that are similar in terms of visual complexity induced different amounts of dilution and (b) the size of the dilution effect is proportional to the size of the Stroop interference effect when the diluters are used as single distractors. Experiments 2 and 3 revealed that when the position of the color bar is pre-cued, Stroop dilution disappears. We argue that these findings support Van der Heijden's (1992) attention-capture account of Stroop dilution.


Assuntos
Atenção , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Percepção de Cores , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Países Baixos , Tempo de Reação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA