Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 61
Filtrar
1.
Ear Hear ; 2024 Feb 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38334698

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The Children's English and Spanish Speech Recognition (ChEgSS) test is a computer-based tool for assessing closed-set word recognition in English and in Spanish, with a masker that is either speech-shaped noise or competing speech. The present study was conducted to (1) characterize the psychometric properties of the ChEgSS test, (2) evaluate feasibility and reliability for a large cohort of Spanish/English bilingual children with normal hearing, and (3) establish normative data. DESIGN: Three experiments were conducted to evaluate speech perception in children (4-17 years) and adults (19-40 years) with normal hearing using the ChEgSS test. In Experiment 1, data were collected from Spanish/English bilingual and English monolingual adults at multiple, fixed signal-to-noise ratios. Psychometric functions were fitted to the word-level data to characterize variability across target words in each language and in each masker condition. In Experiment 2, Spanish/English bilingual adults were tested using an adaptive tracking procedure to evaluate the influence of different target-word normalization approaches on the reliability of estimates of masked-speech recognition thresholds corresponding to 70.7% correct word recognition and to determine the optimal number of reversals needed to obtain reliable estimates. In Experiment 3, Spanish/English bilingual and English monolingual children completed speech perception testing using the ChEgSS test to (1) characterize feasibility across age and language group, (2) evaluate test-retest reliability, and (3) establish normative data. RESULTS: Experiments 1 and 2 yielded data that are essential for stimulus normalization, optimizing threshold estimation procedures, and interpreting threshold data across test language and masker type. Findings obtained from Spanish/English bilingual and English monolingual children with normal hearing in Experiment 3 support feasibility and demonstrate reliability for use with children as young as 4 years of age. Equivalent results for testing in English and Spanish were observed for Spanish/English bilingual children, contingent on adequate proficiency in the target language. Regression-based threshold norms were established for Spanish/English bilingual and English monolingual children between 4 and 17 years of age. CONCLUSIONS: The present findings indicate the ChEgSS test is appropriate for testing a wide age range of children with normal hearing in either Spanish, English, or both languages. The ChEgSS test is currently being evaluated in a large cohort of patients with hearing loss at pediatric audiology clinics across the United States. Results will be compared with normative data established in the present study and with established clinical measures used to evaluate English- and Spanish-speaking children. Questionnaire data from parents and clinician feedback will be used to further improve test procedures.

2.
Ear Hear ; 45(1): 81-93, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37415268

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The purpose of this study was to evaluate effects of masker type and hearing group on the relationship between school-age children's speech recognition and age, vocabulary, working memory, and selective attention. This study also explored effects of masker type and hearing group on the time course of maturation of masked speech recognition. DESIGN: Participants included 31 children with normal hearing (CNH) and 41 children with mild to severe bilateral sensorineural hearing loss (CHL), between 6.7 and 13 years of age. Children with hearing aids used their personal hearing aids throughout testing. Audiometric thresholds and standardized measures of vocabulary, working memory, and selective attention were obtained from each child, along with masked sentence recognition thresholds in a steady state, speech-spectrum noise (SSN) and in a two-talker speech masker (TTS). Aided audibility through children's hearing aids was calculated based on the Speech Intelligibility Index (SII) for all children wearing hearing aids. Linear mixed effects models were used to examine the contribution of group, age, vocabulary, working memory, and attention to individual differences in speech recognition thresholds in each masker. Additional models were constructed to examine the role of aided audibility on masked speech recognition in CHL. Finally, to explore the time course of maturation of masked speech perception, linear mixed effects models were used to examine interactions between age, masker type, and hearing group as predictors of masked speech recognition. RESULTS: Children's thresholds were higher in TTS than in SSN. There was no interaction of hearing group and masker type. CHL had higher thresholds than CNH in both maskers. In both hearing groups and masker types, children with better vocabularies had lower thresholds. An interaction of hearing group and attention was observed only in the TTS. Among CNH, attention predicted thresholds in TTS. Among CHL, vocabulary and aided audibility predicted thresholds in TTS. In both maskers, thresholds decreased as a function of age at a similar rate in CNH and CHL. CONCLUSIONS: The factors contributing to individual differences in speech recognition differed as a function of masker type. In TTS, the factors contributing to individual difference in speech recognition further differed as a function of hearing group. Whereas attention predicted variance for CNH in TTS, vocabulary and aided audibility predicted variance in CHL. CHL required a more favorable signal to noise ratio (SNR) to recognize speech in TTS than in SSN (mean = +1 dB in TTS, -3 dB in SSN). We posit that failures in auditory stream segregation limit the extent to which CHL can recognize speech in a speech masker. Larger sample sizes or longitudinal data are needed to characterize the time course of maturation of masked speech perception in CHL.


Assuntos
Surdez , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial , Perda Auditiva , Percepção da Fala , Criança , Humanos , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Audição , Ruído , Inteligibilidade da Fala
3.
Front Pediatr ; 11: 1252452, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38078311

RESUMO

Introduction: This study evaluated the ability of children (8-12 years) with mild bilateral or unilateral hearing loss (MBHL/UHL) listening unaided, or normal hearing (NH) to locate and understand talkers in varying auditory/visual acoustic environments. Potential differences across hearing status were examined. Methods: Participants heard sentences presented by female talkers from five surrounding locations in varying acoustic environments. A localization-only task included two conditions (auditory only, visually guided auditory) in three acoustic environments (favorable, typical, poor). Participants were asked to locate each talker. A speech perception task included four conditions [auditory-only, visually guided auditory, audiovisual, auditory-only from 0° azimuth (baseline)] in a single acoustic environment. Participants were asked to locate talkers, then repeat what was said. Results: In the localization-only task, participants were better able to locate talkers and looking times were shorter with visual guidance to talker location. Correct looking was poorest and looking times longest in the poor acoustic environment. There were no significant effects of hearing status/age. In the speech perception task, performance was highest in the audiovisual condition and was better in the visually guided and auditory-only conditions than in the baseline condition. Although audiovisual performance was best overall, children with MBHL or UHL performed more poorly than peers with NH. Better-ear pure-tone averages for children with MBHL had a greater effect on keyword understanding than did poorer-ear pure-tone averages for children with UHL. Conclusion: Although children could locate talkers more easily and quickly with visual information, finding locations alone did not improve speech perception. Best speech perception occurred in the audiovisual condition; however, poorer performance by children with MBHL or UHL suggested that being able to see talkers did not overcome reduced auditory access. Children with UHL exhibited better speech perception than children with MBHL, supporting benefits of NH in at least one ear.

4.
Int J Audiol ; : 1-12, 2023 Dec 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38147879

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine if a stricter criterion for paediatric hearing aid fitting for proximity of fit-to-target of <3 dB root-mean-square (RMS) error produces better audibility and outcomes compared to the current <5 dB criterion, and to examine the relationship between aided audibility and RMS error by degree of hearing loss. DESIGN: We evaluated the influence of unaided hearing level on the relationship between RMS error and aided audibility. We assessed the effect of RMS error category (<3, 3-5, >5 dB) on aided audibility, speech recognition, expressive vocabulary, and morphosyntax. STUDY SAMPLE: The study included 2314 hearing aid verification measurements from 307 children with hearing aids. RESULTS: Children who met a <3 dB criterion had higher aided audibility than children who met no criterion (>5 dB error). Results showed no differences in speech recognition or vocabulary by error category, but children with <3 dB error demonstrated better morphosyntax than children with 3-5 and >5 dB RMS error. CONCLUSIONS: Fittings that are close to prescriptive targets provide a more positive outcome for children with hearing aids. Using probe microphone measures to adjust hearing aids to within 3 dB may benefit language abilities in children.

5.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 66(9): 3550-3573, 2023 09 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37591235

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The primary goal for this study was to compare story generation and narrative retell performance in children who are hard of hearing (CHH) and hearing children. A secondary goal was to determine factors that influence narrative performance. Research on this topic is important because narrative language competency is an essential communication function. METHOD: Participants included 86 CHH and 53 seven-year-old hearing children who had completed a test battery composed of vocabulary, grammar, phonological processing, story generation, and narrative retell tasks. Coders who were blind to hearing status used a scoring rubric to judge the quality of narrative macrostructure in story generation and narrative retells. Data were analyzed using analysis of variance to determine group differences and correlational models to examine individual differences. RESULTS: At 7 years of age, CHH demonstrated significant deficits in narrative macrostructure compared to hearing children, with larger delays in narrative retell than story generation. Vocabulary, grammar, and phonological memory acted as mediators in the relationship between hearing status and story generation; grammar acted as a mediator between hearing status and narrative retell. Auditory access variables accounted for a significant proportion of shared variance in story generation skills for CHH. CONCLUSIONS: School-age CHH are at risk for delays in narrative production, particularly with retelling stories. The results of this study highlight a narrative coding approach and task procedures that are sensitive to differences in language levels and may be clinically useful for professionals working with early school-age children.


Assuntos
Perda Auditiva , Audição , Criança , Humanos , Idioma , Linguística , Individualidade
6.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 154(2): 991-1002, 2023 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37581511

RESUMO

Individual differences in ear-canal acoustics introduce variability into hearing aid output that can affect speech audibility. Measuring ear-canal acoustics in young children can be challenging, and relying on normative real-ear-to-coupler difference (RECD) transforms can lead to large fitting errors. Acoustic immittance measures characterize the impedance of the ear and are more easily measured than RECD. Using 226 Hz tympanometry to predict the RECD is more accurate than using age-based average RECD values. The current study sought to determine whether wideband acoustic immittance measurements could improve predictions of wideband real-ear-to-coupler difference (wRECD). 150 children ages 2-10 years with intact tympanic membranes underwent wRECD and wideband acoustic immittance measures in each ear. Three models were constructed to predict each child's measured wRECD: the age-based average wRECD, 226 Hz admittance wRECD, and wideband absorbance wRECD. The average age-based wRECD model predicted the child's measured wRECD within 3 dB in 62% of cases, but both the 226 Hz admittance and wideband absorbance wRECD were within 3 dB in 90% of cases. Using individual 226 Hz or wideband absorbance to predict wRECD improved the accuracy and precision of transforms used for pediatric hearing aid fitting.


Assuntos
Testes de Impedância Acústica , Membrana Timpânica , Humanos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Audição , Meato Acústico Externo , Acústica , Orelha Média
7.
Semin Hear ; 44(Suppl 1): S17-S28, 2023 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36970647

RESUMO

This article reviews the research of Pat Stelmachowicz on traditional and novel measures for quantifying speech audibility (i.e., pure-tone average [PTA], the articulation/audibility index [AI], the speech intelligibility index, and auditory dosage) as predictors of speech perception and language outcomes in children. We discuss the limitations of using audiometric PTA as a predictor of perceptual outcomes in children and how Pat's research shed light on the importance of measures that characterize high-frequency audibility. We also discuss the AI, Pat's work on the calculation of the AI as a hearing aid outcome measure, and how this work led to the application of the speech intelligibility index as a clinically utilized measure of unaided and aided audibility. Finally, we describe a novel measure of audibility-auditory dosage-that was developed based on Pat's work on audibility and hearing aid use for children who are hard of hearing.

8.
Int J Audiol ; 62(5): 462-471, 2023 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36752672

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Paediatric hearing-aid verification relies on measures of output obtained from the ear canal or in a coupler with the child's real-ear-to-coupler difference (RECD). Measured RECD cannot always be completed in children, leading to fitting inaccuracies. Audiologists often have tympanometry data that characterises the child's ear-canal acoustics. The goal of this study was to determine if tympanometry can be used to improve predictions of measured RECD. DESIGN: A retrospective analysis of RECD and admittance, tympanometric peak pressure, and equivalent ear-canal volume from 226 Hz tympanometry collected as part of a longitudinal study of children with hearing loss were modelled with Bayesian hierarchical regression. STUDY SAMPLE: Two-hundred sixty-six children with mild-to-severe hearing loss contributed data. RESULTS: Age-based average RECD models were within 3 dB of measured RECD values in 54% of cases with normal middle ear status and 50.6% of cases with abnormal middle ear status. Immittance-predicted RECD were within 3 dB in 69.6% of cases with normal middle ear status and 74.4% of cases with abnormal middle ear status. CONCLUSION: Immittance-predicted RECD was more accurate than age-based average RECD, particularly in children with abnormal middle ear status. The findings suggest that 226 Hz tympanometry could be used clinically to improve predictions of measured RECD when it cannot be measured.


Assuntos
Testes de Impedância Acústica , Perda Auditiva , Criança , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Estudos Retrospectivos , Teorema de Bayes
9.
Ear Hear ; 44(4): 787-802, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36627755

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: The purpose of this study was to determine if traditional audiologic measures (e.g., pure-tone average, speech recognition) and audibility-based measures predict risk for spoken language delay in children who are hard of hearing (CHH) who use hearing aids (HAs). Audibility-based measures included the Speech Intelligibility Index (SII), HA use, and auditory dosage, a measure of auditory access that weighs each child's unaided and aided audibility by the average hours of HA use per day. The authors also sought to estimate values of these measures at which CHH would be at greater risk for delayed outcomes compared with a group of children with typical hearing (CTH) matched for age and socioeconomic status, potentially signaling a need to make changes to a child's hearing technology or intervention plan. DESIGN: The authors compared spoken language outcomes of 182 CHH and 78 CTH and evaluated relationships between language and audiologic measures (e.g., aided SII) in CHH using generalized additive models. They used these models to identify values associated with falling below CTH (by > 1.5 SDs from the mean) on language assessments, putting CHH at risk for language delay. RESULTS: Risk for language delay was associated with aided speech recognition in noise performance (<59% phonemes correct, 95% confidence interval [55%, 62%]), aided Speech Intelligibility Index (SII < 0.61, 95% confidence internal [.53,.68]), and auditory dosage (dosage < 6.0, 95% confidence internal [5.3, 6.7]) in CHH. The level of speech recognition in quiet, unaided pure-tone average, and unaided SII that placed children at risk for language delay could not be determined due to imprecise estimates with broad confidence intervals. CONCLUSIONS: Results support using aided SII, aided speech recognition in noise measures, and auditory dosage as tools to facilitate clinical decision-making, such as deciding whether changes to a child's hearing technology are warranted. Values identified in this article can complement other metrics (e.g., unaided hearing thresholds, aided speech recognition testing, language assessment) when considering changes to intervention, such as adding language supports, making HA adjustments, or referring for cochlear implant candidacy evaluation.


Assuntos
Auxiliares de Audição , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial , Perda Auditiva , Percepção da Fala , Criança , Humanos , Perda Auditiva/reabilitação , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Audição , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/reabilitação , Limiar Auditivo
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(9): 5228-5237, 2023 04 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36310092

RESUMO

Sensory gating is a process by which the brain filters out redundant information to preserve neural resources for behaviorally relevant stimuli. Although studies have shown alterations in auditory and visual processing in children who are hard-of-hearing (CHH) relative to children with normal hearing (CNH), it is unclear whether these alterations extend to the somatosensory domain, and how aberrations in sensory processing affect sensory gating. In this study, CHH and CNH were presented with a paired-pulse median nerve stimulation during magnetoencephalography. Stimulus-related gamma neural activity was imaged and virtual time series from peak somatosensory responses were extracted. We found significant effects of both stimulus and group, as well as a significant group-by-stimulus interaction. CHH showed a larger response to stimulation overall, as well as greater differences in gamma power from the first to the second stimulus. However, when looking at the ratio rather than the absolute difference in power, CHH showed comparable gating to CNH. In addition, smaller gating ratios were correlated with better classroom behavior and verbal ability in CHH, but not CNH. Taken together, these data underscore the importance of considering how CHH experience their multisensory environment when interpreting outcomes and designing interventions.


Assuntos
Perda Auditiva , Humanos , Criança , Cognição , Magnetoencefalografia/métodos , Encéfalo , Nervo Mediano , Filtro Sensorial , Córtex Somatossensorial/fisiologia
11.
Int J Audiol ; 62(3): 261-268, 2023 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35184649

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The purpose of this study was to 1) characterise word recognition in a speech masker for preschoolers tested using closed-set, forced-choice procedures and 2) better understand the stimulus and listener factors affecting performance. DESIGN: Speech recognition thresholds (SRTs) in a two-talker masker were evaluated using a picture-pointing response with two sets of disyllabic target words. ChEgSS words were previously developed for children ≥5 years of age, and simple words were developed for preschoolers. Familiarisation ensured accurate identification of target words before testing. STUDY SAMPLE: Participants were 3- and 4-year olds (n = 21) and young adults (n = 10) with normal hearing. RESULTS: Preschoolers and adults had significantly lower SRTs for the simple words than the ChEgSS words, and lower SRTs for early-acquired than later-acquired ChEgSS words. For both word sets, SRTs were approximately 11-dB higher for preschoolers than adults, and child age was associated with SRTs. Preschoolers' receptive vocabulary size predicted performance for ChEgSS words but not simple words. CONCLUSIONS: Preschoolers were more susceptible to speech-in-speech masking than adults, with a similar child-adult difference for the ChEgSS and simple words. Effects of receptive vocabulary in preschoolers' recognition of ChEgSS words indicate that vocabulary size is an important consideration, even when using closed-set methods.


Assuntos
Mascaramento Perceptivo , Percepção da Fala , Adulto Jovem , Humanos , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Fala , Percepção da Fala/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Vocabulário
12.
J Am Acad Audiol ; 2022 Dec 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36577441

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Remote-microphone (RM) systems are designed to reduce the impact of poor acoustics on speech understanding. However, there is limited research examining the effects of adding reverberation to noise on speech understanding when using hearing aids (HAs) and RM systems. Given the significant challenges posed by environments with poor acoustics for children who are hard of hearing, we evaluated the ability of a novel RM system to address the effects of noise and reverberation. PURPOSE: We assessed the effect of a recently developed RM system on aided speech perception of children who were hard of hearing in noise and reverberation and how their performance compared to peers with "normal" hearing. The effect of aided speech audibility on sentence recognition when using an RM system also was assessed. STUDY SAMPLE: Twenty-two children with mild to severe hearing loss and 17 children with normal "hearing" (7-18 years) participated. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS: An adaptive procedure was used to determine the signal-to-noise ratio for 50 and 95% correct sentence recognition in noise and noise plus reverberation (RT 300 ms). Linear mixed models were used to examine the effect of listening conditions on speech recognition with RMs for children who were hard of hearing compared to children with "normal" hearing and the effects of aided audibility on performance across all listening conditions for children who were hard of hearing. RESULTS: Children who were hard of hearing had poorer speech recognition for HAs alone than for HAs plus RM. Regardless of hearing status, children had poorer speech recognition in noise plus reverberation than in noise alone. Children who were hard of hearing had poorer speech recognition than peers with "normal" hearing when using HAs alone but comparable or better speech recognition with HAs plus RM. Children with better-aided audibility with the HAs showed better speech recognition with the HAs alone and with HAs plus RM. CONCLUSIONS: Providing HAs that maximize speech audibility and coupling them with RM systems has the potential to improve communication access and outcomes for children who are hard of hearing in environments with noise and reverberation.

13.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 31(5): 2092-2107, 2022 09 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36037481

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Children who are hard of hearing (CHH) are at risk for literacy difficulties. Speech sound production is related to literacy skills in typically hearing children. Speech sound production abilities can also be markedly weak in CHH due to inconsistent access to the speech signal. We longitudinally examined relations between auditory experience, speech sound production abilities, and literacy (i.e., nonword reading and spelling in second grade) in CHH. METHOD: Participants included 166 CHH. All participants used hearing aids (HAs). Auditory experience is a weighted measure of the number of hours of daily HA use and the amount of audibility with and without their HAs. Children's speech sound production was tested 2-3 times between the ages of 3 and 9 years. At age 5 years and again in second grade, children were tested on a battery of language and literacy measures. RESULTS: Auditory experience was significantly correlated with speech sound production abilities, but age at HA fitting was not. Speech sound production abilities at age 5 years did not predict nonword reading in second grade but did predict spelling skills in second grade. CONCLUSIONS: Our results support the importance of auditory experience and speech sound production for later literacy abilities. Specifically, we found that speech sound production abilities and print knowledge at age 5 years are related to second grade spelling outcomes. Interestingly, we found that morphological knowledge at age 5 years was more predictive of decoding in second grade than was speech sound production. Although there are multiple factors that contribute to literacy success, our results underscore the unique contributions of speech sound production abilities in CHH.


Assuntos
Auxiliares de Audição , Perda Auditiva , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Audição , Perda Auditiva/diagnóstico , Humanos , Alfabetização , Fonética , Leitura
14.
Brain Commun ; 4(2): fcac093, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35480224

RESUMO

Children who are hard of hearing are at risk for developmental language and academic delays compared with children with normal hearing. Some work suggests that high-order cognitive function, including fluid intelligence, may relate to language and academic outcomes in children with hearing loss, but findings in these studies have been mixed and to date, there have been no studies of the whole-brain neural dynamics serving fluid intelligence in the context of hearing loss. To this end, this study sought to identify the impact of hearing loss and subsequent hearing aid use on the neural dynamics serving abstract reasoning in children who are hard of hearing relative to children with normal hearing using magnetoencephalography. We found significant elevations in occipital and parietal theta activity during early stimulus evaluation in children who are hard of hearing relative to normal-hearing peers. In addition, we found that greater hearing aid use was significantly related to reduced activity throughout the fronto-parietal network. Notably, there were no differences in alpha dynamics between groups during later-stage processing nor did alpha activity correlate with hearing aid use. These cross-sectional data suggest that differences in auditory experience lead to widespread alterations in the neural dynamics serving initial stimulus processing in fluid intelligence in children.

15.
PLoS One ; 17(3): e0264581, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35271608

RESUMO

Having a large receptive vocabulary benefits speech-in-noise recognition for young children, though this is not always the case for older children or adults. These observations could indicate that effects of receptive vocabulary size on speech-in-noise recognition differ depending on familiarity of the target words, with effects observed only for more recently acquired and less frequent words. Two experiments were conducted to evaluate effects of vocabulary size on open-set speech-in-noise recognition for adults with normal hearing. Targets were words acquired at 4, 9, 12 and 15 years of age, and they were presented at signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) of -5 and -7 dB. Percent correct scores tended to fall with increasing age of acquisition (AoA), with the caveat that performance at -7 dB SNR was better for words acquired at 9 years of age than earlier- or later-acquired words. Similar results were obtained whether the AoA of the target words was blocked or mixed across trials. Differences in word duration appear to account for nonmonotonic effects of AoA. For all conditions, a positive correlation was observed between recognition and vocabulary size irrespective of target word AoA, indicating that effects of vocabulary size are not limited to recently acquired words. This dataset does not support differential assessment of AoA, lexical frequency, and other stimulus features known to affect lexical access.


Assuntos
Percepção da Fala , Vocabulário , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Audição , Humanos , Ruído , Fala , Adulto Jovem
16.
J Am Acad Audiol ; 33(3): 170-180, 2022 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34695870

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Adults and children with sensorineural hearing loss (SNHL) have trouble understanding speech in rooms with reverberation when using hearing aid amplification. While the use of amplitude compression signal processing in hearing aids may contribute to this difficulty, there is conflicting evidence on the effects of amplitude compression settings on speech recognition. Less clear is the effect of a fast release time for adults and children with SNHL when using compression ratios derived from a prescriptive procedure. PURPOSE: The aim of the study is to determine whether release time impacts speech recognition in reverberation for children and adults with SNHL and to determine if these effects of release time and reverberation can be predicted using indices of audibility or temporal and spectral distortion. RESEARCH DESIGN: This is a quasi-experimental cohort study. Participants used a hearing aid simulator set to the Desired Sensation Level algorithm m[i/o] for three different amplitude compression release times. Reverberation was simulated using three different reverberation times. PARTICIPANTS: Participants were 20 children and 16 adults with SNHL. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSES: Participants were seated in a sound-attenuating booth and then nonsense syllable recognition was measured. Predictions of speech recognition were made using indices of audibility, temporal distortion, and spectral distortion and the effects of release time and reverberation were analyzed using linear mixed models. RESULTS: While nonsense syllable recognition decreased in reverberation release time did not significantly affect nonsense syllable recognition. Participants with lower audibility were more susceptible to the negative effect of reverberation on nonsense syllable recognition. CONCLUSION: We have extended previous work on the effects of reverberation on aided speech recognition to children with SNHL. Variations in release time did not impact the understanding of speech. An index of audibility best predicted nonsense syllable recognition in reverberation and, clinically, these results suggest that patients with less audibility are more susceptible to nonsense syllable recognition in reverberation.


Assuntos
Auxiliares de Audição , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial , Percepção da Fala , Adulto , Criança , Estudos de Coortes , Perda Auditiva Neurossensorial/reabilitação , Humanos , Fala
17.
Ear Hear ; 43(2): 347-360, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34288630

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Children who are hard of hearing (CHH) experience delays in spoken language and executive function, but the mechanisms for these deficits remain unresolved. Differences in auditory experience and language skills have been examined as contributing factors to deficits in executive function, primarily with children who are deaf and children with cochlear implants. The theoretical model of cumulative auditory experience quantifies auditory dosage as how much speech is audible and how often children wear their hearing aids. CHH with higher auditory dosage have better language outcomes than peers with less auditory dosage. However, the effects of auditory experience on executive function have not been studied in CHH. The goal of this study was to examine the influences of auditory experience and language skills on the development of executive function in CHH. DESIGN: We collected measures of aided speech audibility, hearing aid use, executive function, and receptive vocabulary in 177 CHH and 86 children with typical hearing who were 5- to 10 years old and matched for socioeconomic status and nonverbal intelligence. Auditory dosage was calculated by combining each child's average hours of hearing aid use with their audibility for speech to create a variable that quantifies individual differences in auditory access. RESULTS: CHH had lower receptive vocabulary and deficits in executive function related to working memory and selective attention compared to peers with typical hearing. CHH with greater auditory dosage had higher receptive vocabulary than CHH with lower auditory dosage. Better receptive vocabulary was associated with better scores on executive function measures related to working memory and attention. Auditory dosage was also directly associated with measures of verbal working memory. CONCLUSIONS: CHH have deficits in language and some, but not all, areas of executive function related to working memory and attention. Auditory dosage was associated with language abilities and verbal working memory. Language was associated with individual differences in executive function skills related to attention and working memory. These results provide support for systems theories regarding the development of executive function in CHH. Interventions that improve auditory access and language may be effective for improving executive function related to working memory and attention in CHH.


Assuntos
Implantes Cocleares , Surdez , Perda Auditiva , Percepção da Fala , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Surdez/reabilitação , Função Executiva , Audição , Humanos , Idioma , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Memória de Curto Prazo
18.
Ear Hear ; 43(2): 408-419, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34291759

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Children with hearing loss (CHL) may exhibit spoken language delays and may also experience deficits in other cognitive domains including working memory. Consistent hearing aid use (i.e., more than 10 hours per day) ameliorates these language delays; however, the impact of hearing aid intervention on the neural dynamics serving working memory remains unknown. The objective of this study was to examine the association between the amount of hearing aid use and neural oscillatory activity during verbal working memory processing in children with mild-to-severe hearing loss. DESIGN: Twenty-three CHL between 8 and 15 years-old performed a letter-based Sternberg working memory task during magnetoencephalography (MEG). Guardians also completed a questionnaire describing the participants' daily hearing aid use. Each participant's MEG data was coregistered to their structural MRI, epoched, and transformed into the time-frequency domain using complex demodulation. Significant oscillatory responses corresponding to working memory encoding and maintenance were independently imaged using beamforming. Finally, these whole-brain source images were correlated with the total number of hours of weekly hearing aid use, controlling for degree of hearing loss. RESULTS: During the encoding period, hearing aid use negatively correlated with alpha-beta oscillatory activity in the bilateral occipital cortices and right precentral gyrus. In the occipital cortices, this relationship suggested that with greater hearing aid use, there was a larger suppression of occipital activity (i.e., more negative relative to baseline). In the precentral gyrus, greater hearing aid use was related to less synchronous activity (i.e., less positive relative to baseline). During the maintenance period, hearing aid use significantly correlated with alpha activity in the right prefrontal cortex, such that with greater hearing aid use, there was less right prefrontal maintenance-related activity (i.e., less positive relative to baseline). CONCLUSIONS: This study is the first to investigate the impact of hearing aid use on the neural dynamics that underlie working memory function. These data show robust relationships between the amount of hearing aid use and phase-specific neural patterns during working memory encoding and maintenance after controlling for degree of hearing loss. Furthermore, our data demonstrate that wearing hearing aids for more than ~8.5 hours/day may serve to normalize these neural patterns. This study also demonstrates the potential for neuroimaging to help determine the locus of variability in outcomes in CHL.


Assuntos
Surdez , Auxiliares de Audição , Perda Auditiva , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Adolescente , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Criança , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografia/métodos , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia
19.
J Early Hear Detect Interv ; 7(1): 1-10, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38737914

RESUMO

Purpose: To evaluate the impact of hearing screening, diagnosis, and early intervention (EI) by 3 months or 6 months of age on language growth trajectories for children with hearing loss (HL) relative to children with normal hearing (NH). Method: We recruited 133 children with mild to severe HL through universal newborn hearing screening records and referrals from audiologists in the United States and 116 children with NH who served as a comparison group. Examiners administered a battery of developmentally appropriate language measures between 12 months and 8 years of age. We constructed latent growth curve models of global language, grammar, and vocabulary using Bayesian statistics. Results: Children with HL demonstrated no significant differences in initial language skills compared to children with NH. Children in the 1-3-6 group also showed no difference in language growth compared to children with NH. The slope for the 1-2-3 group was significantly steeper than children with NH for global language and grammar. Conclusions: This study documents the positive impact of EI on language outcomes in children with congenital HL. It is among the first to provide evidence to support the potential effects of very early intervention by 3 months of age.

20.
Neuroimage Clin ; 30: 102647, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33838545

RESUMO

Children with hearing loss (CHL) exhibit delays in language function relative to children with normal hearing (CNH). However, evidence on whether these delays extend into other cognitive domains such as working memory is mixed, with some studies showing decrements in CHL and others showing CHL performing at the level of CNH. Despite the growing literature investigating the impact of hearing loss on cognitive and language development, studies of the neural dynamics that underlie these cognitive processes are notably absent. This study sought to identify the oscillatory neural responses serving verbal working memory processing in CHL compared to CNH. To this end, participants with and without hearing loss performed a verbal working memory task during magnetoencephalography. Neural oscillatory responses associated with working memory encoding and maintenance were imaged separately, and these responses were statistically evaluated between CHL and CNH. While CHL performed as well on the task as CNH, CHL exhibited significantly elevated alpha-beta activity in the right frontal and precentral cortices during encoding relative to CNH. In contrast, CHL showed elevated alpha maintenance-related activity in the right precentral and parieto-occipital cortices. Crucially, right superior frontal encoding activity and right parieto-occipital maintenance activity correlated with language ability across groups. These data suggest that CHL may utilize compensatory right-hemispheric activity to achieve verbal working memory function at the level of CNH. Neural behavior in these regions may impact language function during crucial developmental ages.


Assuntos
Surdez , Perda Auditiva , Criança , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Magnetoencefalografia , Memória de Curto Prazo
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA