Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 113
Filtrar
1.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 9: 629, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26635582

RESUMO

UNLABELLED: Mental schemas exert top-down control on information processing, for instance by facilitating the storage of schema-related information. However, given capacity-limits and competition in neural network processing, schemas may additionally exert their effects by suppressing information with low momentary relevance. In particular, when existing schemas suffice to guide goal-directed behavior, this may actually reduce encoding of the redundant sensory input, in favor of gaining efficiency in task performance. The present experiment set out to test this schema-induced shallow encoding hypothesis. Our approach involved a memory task in which faces had to be coupled to homes. For half of the faces the responses could be guided by a pre-learned schema, for the other half of the faces such a schema was not available. Memory storage was compared between schema-congruent and schema-incongruent items. To characterize putative schema effects, memory was assessed both with regard to visual details and contextual aspects of each item. The depth of encoding was also assessed through an objective neural measure: the parietal old/new ERP effect. This ERP effect, observed between 500-800 ms post-stimulus onset, is thought to reflect the extent of recollection: the retrieval of a vivid memory, including various contextual details from the learning episode. We found that schema-congruency induced substantial impairments in item memory and even larger ones in context memory. Furthermore, the parietal old/new ERP effect indicated higher recollection for the schema-incongruent than the schema-congruent memories. The combined findings indicate that, when goals can be achieved using existing schemas, this can hinder the in-depth processing of novel input, impairing the formation of perceptually detailed and contextually rich memory traces. Taking into account both current and previous findings, we suggest that schemas can both positively and negatively bias the processing of sensory input. An important determinant in this matter is likely related to momentary goals, such that mental schemas facilitate memory processing of goal-relevant input, but suppress processing of goal-irrelevant information. HIGHLIGHTS: - Schema-congruent information suffers from shallow encoding.- Schema congruency induces poor item and context memory.- The parietal old/new effect is less pronounced for schema-congruent items.- Schemas exert different influences on memory formation depending on current goals.

2.
Br J Anaesth ; 95(2): 197-206, 2005 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15980046

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Spontaneous EEG, mid-latency auditory evoked potentials (AEP) and somatosensory evoked potentials (SSEP) have been used to monitor anaesthesia. This poses the question as to whether or not EEG, AEP and SSEP vary in parallel with varying conditions during surgical anaesthesia. METHODS: A total of 81 variables (31 EEG, 22 SSEP, 28 AEP) were simultaneously recorded in 48 surgical patients during anaesthesia. A total of 307 cases of the 81 variables in stable anaesthetic states were recorded. A factor analysis was performed for this data set. RESULTS: Sixteen variables were excluded because of multicollinearity. We extracted 13 factors with eigenvalues >1, representing 78.3% of the total variance, from the remaining 65 x 307 matrix. The first three factors represented 12%, 11% and 10% of the total variance. Factor 1 had only significant loadings from EEG variables, factor 2 only significant loadings from AEP variables and factor 3 only significant loadings from SSEP variables. CONCLUSION: EEG, AEP and SSEP measure different aspects of neural processing during anaesthesia. This gives rise to the hypothesis that simultaneous monitoring of these quantities may give additional information compared with the monitoring of each quantity alone.


Assuntos
Anestésicos Intravenosos , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Monitorização Intraoperatória/métodos , Propofol , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Anestesia Geral , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Procedimentos Cirúrgicos Eletivos , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos , Potenciais Somatossensoriais Evocados , Análise Fatorial , Humanos , Midazolam , Pré-Medicação
4.
Fortschr Med Orig ; 121(1): 5-10, 2003.
Artigo em Alemão | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15117063

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND AIM: Over the past 25 years, numerous studies have confirmed the positive effect of the special ginkgo extract EGb 761 on the mental ability and emotional well-being of patients with cognitive disorders of vascular genesis, and Alzheimer-type dementia. The following study investigated the short-term effect of the special ginkgo extract EGb 761 on the subjective emotional well-being of healthy elderly subjects. STUDY POPULATION AND METHOD: The study was designed as a randomized double-blind, monocenter study with parallel groups. It included 66 healthy subjects of both sexes aged between 50 and 65 with no age-related cognitive impairments. For a period of 4 weeks, 34 subjects received a daily dose of 240 mg EGb 761, and 32 a placebo. Prior to starting medication and after 28 days of treatment, subjects completed the following scales and questionnaires to establish subjective emotional well-being: the Profile of Mood States (POMS), the Self Rating Depression Scale (SDS), three Visual Analog Scales to assess the quality of life (VAS-QoL), general health (VAS-GH) and mental health (VAS-MH), and a new instrument for assessing changes in general subjective well-being, the Subjective Intensity Score Mood (SIS Mood). Depending on the underlying distribution of the variables analyzed, parametric (t-tests) or nonparametric tests (U-tests) were performed to compare mean values and distributions both within and between the treatment groups. RESULTS: The final examination revealed a statistically significant difference between the two groups for the VAS mental health and quality of life, as also for SIS Mood at the telephone interview in week 2. A comparison of baseline with the final examination within the groups showed a statistically significant improvement in the EGb 761 group for the variables: depression, fatigue, anger and SDS. For none of the variables investigated was a worsening observed in the EGb 761 group. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest a positive effect of EGb 761 on the subjective emotional well-being of healthy elderly persons.


Assuntos
Atividades Cotidianas/psicologia , Afeto/efeitos dos fármacos , Envelhecimento/efeitos dos fármacos , Fitoterapia , Extratos Vegetais/farmacologia , Qualidade de Vida/psicologia , Idoso , Método Duplo-Cego , Feminino , Ginkgo biloba , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Psicometria
6.
Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) ; 61(3): 175-9, 2001.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11584450

RESUMO

The variability of simple actions with response to auditory stimuli was studied under different delay conditions. Subjects reacted as fast as possible or with a defined time delay (from 250 to 750 ms) to a tone switching off by pressing a response-key with the left index finger (controlled by the right hemisphere) or with the right one (left hemisphere). For short delays (requested response times below 350 ms) variability of responses was much larger then for longer delays (above 350 ms), especially for the right hand. Thus, precise temporal control on consciously mediated actions sets only in after a rather long delay (in some cases after half a second). Neuronal mechanisms underlying conscious temporal control of actions appear to be different for the two hemispheres.


Assuntos
Estado de Consciência/fisiologia , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Volição/fisiologia
9.
Zentralbl Chir ; 124(10): 876-83, 1999.
Artigo em Alemão | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10596044

RESUMO

On the basis of an account on elementary processes of spatio-temporal control of movements and on findings on motor learning, we propose in this article the employment of test and training procedures for motor skills in the training of surgeons. Elementary temporal and spatial factors of motor performance control underly the very precise complex motor behaviour of the activity of a surgeon. An established diagnosis of individual competence in this domain could help the doctors' decisions on whether to take up surgery. Training devices could improve the efficiency of the surgeons' motor functions. The inclusion of knowledge of motor abilities and learning processes could complement the present-day training of surgeons.


Assuntos
Cirurgia Geral/educação , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Competência Clínica , Currículo , Humanos , Psicofisiologia
10.
Cortex ; 35(1): 89-100, 1999 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10213536

RESUMO

Over a period of 24 hours, fusion thresholds (click durations 100 micros) were assessed in 7 subjects. Over the same period, order thresholds (click duration of 1 ms) were measured in 10 subjects (12 independent sessions). Auditory fusion thresholds showed a diurnal rhythm with a maximum performance (shortest intervals) around midnight. In contrast, order thresholds appear to be independent on the time of day. Sex specific differences in threshold levels were only observed in order thresholds but not in fusion thresholds.


Assuntos
Córtex Auditivo/fisiologia , Limiar Auditivo/fisiologia , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiologia , Acústica , Adulto , Vias Auditivas/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores Sexuais
11.
Neurosci Lett ; 257(2): 69-72, 1998 Nov 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9865929

RESUMO

We have previously shown that temporal integration in the domain of a few seconds may be studied using a subjective accentuation paradigm. Here we report developmental effects on the limits of this temporal integration in 9-10-year-olds in comparison with 13-14-year-olds. The task was to listen to a string of identical metronome beats and mentally bind the presented beats by subjectively accentuating every second, third or nth beat. The integration interval length was defined as the number of stimuli mentally connected multiplied by the temporal interval between two successive beats. For the lowest stimulus frequency integration intervals were approximately 3 s for the older and 2.2 s for the younger children. For higher frequencies integration intervals got systematically shorter, but being always longer for the older age group. It is suggested that the prefrontal region is responsible for this developmental effect. The expansion of temporal integration correlates with cognitive development in the investigated phase of ontogenesis.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Adolescente , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Periodicidade , Fatores de Tempo
12.
Neurosci Lett ; 235(1-2): 33-6, 1997 Oct 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9389589

RESUMO

We report an association between a language deficit following brain lesion and a new strategy in temporal integration. Patients with different brain lesions mentally grouped sequences of identical acoustic stimuli generated at various frequencies. They were asked while listening to the stimuli to accentuate every second, third or other stimulus to create an individual rhythmic pattern. After each sequence patients reported how many stimuli they had united into a perceptual unit. The integration interval was defined as the number of reported stimuli multiplied by the temporal interval between two successive stimuli. Results indicate different integration strategies depending on the lesion site, i.e. Broca's aphasics behaving differently than all other patient groups. At lower frequencies they showed longer, at higher frequencies they displayed shorter integration. From this observation we conclude that the Broca's patients acquired a new strategy because of the lesion; they relied on mental counting and less on automatic temporal integration, which is usually the case.


Assuntos
Afasia de Broca/fisiopatologia , Encefalopatias/fisiopatologia , Periodicidade , Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia , Adulto , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Afasia de Wernicke/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Percepção do Tempo
14.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 1(2): 56-61, 1997 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21223864

RESUMO

Temporal perception comprises subjective phenomena such as simultaneity, successiveness, temporal order, subjective present, temporal continuity and subjective duration. These elementary temporal experiences are hierarchically related to each other. Functional system states with a duration of 30 ms are implemented by neuronal oscillations and they provide a mechanism to define successiveness. These system states are also responsible for the identification of basic events. For a sequential representation of several events time tags are allocated, resulting in an ordinal representation of such events. A mechanism of temporal integration binds successive events into perceptual units of 3 s duration. Such temporal integration, which is automatic and presemantic, is also operative in movement control and other cognitive activities. Because of the omnipresence of this integration mechanism it is used for a pragmatic definition of the subjective present. Temporal continuity is the result of a semantic connection between successive integration intervals. Subjective duration is known to depend on mental load and attentional demand, high load resulting in long time estimates. In the hierarchical model proposed, system states of 30 ms and integration intervals of 3 s, together with a memory store, provide an explanatory neuro-cognitive machinery for differential subjective duration.

15.
Acta Anaesthesiol Scand ; 40(2): 171-6, 1996 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8848915

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Under general anaesthesia with the volatile anaesthetics halothane, enflurane and isoflurane, midlatency auditory evoked potentials (MLAEP) are suppressed dose-dependently. Therefore, MLAEP have been used to measure depth of anaesthesia and to indicate intraoperative awareness. Desflurane is a new volatile anaesthetic and its effect on MLAEP have not been studied previously. METHODS: We have studied MLAEP during general anaesthesia with increasing endexpiratory concentrations of desflurane in 12 patients scheduled for elective gynaecological surgery. Auditory evoked potentials were recorded in the awake state and during anaesthesia with endexpiratory steady state concentrations of 1.5, 3.0, 4.5 and 6.0 vol % of desflurane on vertex (positive) and mastoids on both sides (negative). Latencies of the peaks V, Na, Pa, Nb, Pl (ms) and amplitudes Na/Pa, Pa/Nb and Nb/Pl (micro V) were measured. RESULTS: In the awake state, MLAEP had high peak-to-peak amplitudes and a periodic waveform. During general anaesthesia with increasing endexpiratory concentration of desflurane, the latency of the brainstem response V increased only slightly. In contrast, MLAEP showed a marked dose-dependent and statistically significant increase in latencies of Na, Pa, Nb and Pl and decrease in amplitudes of Na/Pa, Pa/Nb and Nb/Pl. Under 6.0 vol % of desflurane MLAEP were severely attenuated or even abolished. CONCLUSION: Based on these observations, endexpiratory concentrations of > or = 4.5 vol % desflurane should suppress awareness phenomena such as auditory perceptions during anaesthesia.


Assuntos
Anestesia Geral , Anestésicos Inalatórios/farmacologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/efeitos dos fármacos , Isoflurano/análogos & derivados , Adulto , Desflurano , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos do Tronco Encefálico/efeitos dos fármacos , Feminino , Humanos , Isoflurano/farmacologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Respiração
16.
Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) ; 56(1): 215-25, 1996.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8787177

RESUMO

This study investigates the effect of a mental content of presented stimuli, normal aging and individual differences in cognitive abilities on temporal limits of an integration mechanism. Younger and older subject grouped together the beats generated by a metronome. Subjects were asked to listen to the beats of a metronome and to accentuate mentally every second, third, fourth...etc. beat, to create a subjective rhythm. This rhythm exists, in fact, only in subjects' mind and not objectively. Subjects reported verbally how many clicks they were able to integrate into a perceptual unit. On this basis, the time interval during which subjects were able to integrate temporally separated stimuli was calculated (number of beats reported as being integrated x time distance between beats) for different metronome frequencies. The results show, firstly, that the length of integration periods significantly depends on the frequency of presented metronome beats. When the frequency of metronome beats is high, the time interval during which the subjects integrate beats into a single perceptual unit is shorter. Secondly, older adults integrate information during a longer time interval than younger ones. Thirdly, the length of an integration period is related to a subjects' level of cognitive ability. These results suggest that the length of an integration period is not a constant, stable feature, but varies across the life span depending on the mental content of the information presented and individual factors.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Idoso , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
17.
Anaesthesist ; 44(11): 743-54, 1995 Nov.
Artigo em Alemão | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8678265

RESUMO

The possibility that a patient during general anaesthesia is aware of the operation going on and aware of severe pain that might be remembered postoperatively must be very alarming to patients and anaesthetists alike. Furthermore, there is experimental evidence showing that conscious recall of intraoperative events is only the tip of an iceberg; it seems very probable that there is even a higher incidence of unconscious perception during general anaesthesia. Therefore, the following stages of intraoperative awareness must be distinguished: (1) conscious awareness with explicit recall and with severe pain; (2) conscious awareness with explicit recall but no complaints of pains; (3) conscious awareness without explicit recall and possible implicit recall; (4) subconscious awareness without explicit recall and possible implicit recall; (5) no awareness. The incidence of conscious awareness with explicit recall and severe pain has been estimated at less frequent than 1/3000 general anaesthetics. Conscious awareness with explicit recall but no complaints of pain has been reported in the literature with an incidence of 05-2%. With 7-72%, conscious awareness without explicit recall and possible implicit recall shows a very wide range of variation and its occurrence probably depends on the anaesthetic drugs used. Subconscious awareness with possible implicit recall has an incidence of up to 80%, but there are many methodological problems in demonstrating implicit memory of intraoperative events. Reports of intraoperative awareness do not come exclusively from cardiac surgery and obstetrics, but also from all other operative specialties. Postoperatively, patients who experience intraoperative awareness may develop a so-called post-traumatic stress syndrome. Symptoms involve re-experiencing the event awake or in dreams, sleep disturbances, depression, avoidance of stimuli associated with the event. The probability of the development of the post-traumatic stress syndrome seems to coincide with the experience of severe pain. When a patient complains of intraoperative awareness postoperatively the anaesthesiologist should discuss the event frankly with the patient. When the symptoms of the post-traumatic stress syndrome persist a psychotherapy should follow. Causes for intraoperative awareness may be: equipment failure, too-light anaesthesia, e.g. for a caesarean section or for emergency surgery in severely injured or polytraumatized patients, during cardiac surgery, bronchoscopy of difficult intubation. There is interindividual variability in anaesthetic effect; for example, chronic drug or alcohol abuse or overweight may make increased anaesthetic doses necessary. They are at risk for intraoperative awareness. Some general anaesthetics or anaesthetic procedures, e.g. the combination of a relaxant and N2O, opioid mono-anaesthetics, or opioids combined with benzodiazepines, seem to involve a higher risk of intraoperative awareness than do volatile anaesthetics. The bases of litigation are medical malpractice, breach of contract by the anaesthesiologist or lack of informed consent from the patient. Therefore, patients who are at risk of intraoperative awareness should be given detailed information on this special risk before the operation.


Assuntos
Anestesia Geral/psicologia , Estado de Consciência , Complicações Intraoperatórias/psicologia , Anestesia Geral/efeitos adversos , Alemanha/epidemiologia , Humanos , Complicações Intraoperatórias/epidemiologia , Complicações Intraoperatórias/prevenção & controle , Período Intraoperatório , Legislação Médica , Terminologia como Assunto
18.
Anesth Analg ; 81(4): 817-22, 1995 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7574016

RESUMO

We studied midlatency auditory evoked potentials (MLAEP) during general anesthesia with increasing end-expiratory concentrations of sevoflurane in 12 patients scheduled for elective gynecologic surgery. After oral premedication with 20 mg clorazepate dipotassium, anesthesia was induced with etomidate (0.2 mg/kg intravenously [IV]). Vecuronium (0.1 mg/kg) was given for neuromuscular block, and controlled ventilation with sevoflurane in 100% O2 was instituted. Auditory evoked potentials were recorded in the awake state and during anesthesia with end-expiratory steady-state concentrations of 0.5, 1.0, 1.5, and 2.0 vol% of sevoflurane on vertex (positive) and mastoids on both sides (negative). Latencies of peaks V, Na, Pa, Nb, and P1 (ms) and amplitudes of Na/Pa, Pa/Nb, and Nb/P1 (microV) were measured. In the awake state, MLAEP had high peak-to-peak amplitudes and a periodic waveform. During general anesthesia with increasing end-expiratory concentrations of sevoflurane, the latency of the brainstem response V increased slightly. In contrast, MLAEP showed marked dose-dependent, statistically significant increases in the latencies of Na, Pa, Nb, and P1 and decreases in the amplitudes of Na/Pa, Pa/Nb, and Nb/P1. Under 2 vol% of sevoflurane, MLAEPs were severely attenuated or abolished. Based on these observations, > or = 1.5 vol% sevoflurane should suppress phenomena such as auditory perceptions, intraoperative wakefulness, and awareness.


Assuntos
Anestesia Geral , Anestésicos/farmacologia , Éteres/farmacologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos do Tronco Encefálico/efeitos dos fármacos , Éteres Metílicos , Adulto , Pressão Sanguínea/efeitos dos fármacos , Relação Dose-Resposta a Droga , Feminino , Ginecologia , Frequência Cardíaca/efeitos dos fármacos , Humanos , Sevoflurano
20.
Anesth Analg ; 80(3): 499-505, 1995 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7864414

RESUMO

Our interest focused on the question whether sufentanil differs from alfentanil, fentanyl, and morphine with regard on its effects on midlatency auditory evoked potentials (MLAEP). Therefore, we studied MLAEP during general anesthesia with increasing doses of sufentanil in 16 patients scheduled for elective major urologic surgery. Anesthesia was induced with sufentanil (1 microgram/kg every 7 min to a total dose of 3 micrograms/kg). In 8 of 16 patients, further incremental doses of sufentanil were given to a total dose of 5 micrograms/kg. Auditory evoked potentials were recorded before and 5 min after every sufentanil dose on vertex (positive) and mastoids on both sides (negative). Latencies of the peaks V, Na, Pa, Nb, and P1 (ms), and amplitudes Na/Pa, Pa/Nb, and Nb/P1 (microV) were measured. In the awake state, MLAEP had high peak-to-peak amplitudes and a periodic waveform. During general anesthesia the brainstem response V was stable to increasing doses of sufentanil. There was a marked statistically significant increase in latency and decrease in amplitude of Nb and P1 after 1-2 micrograms/kg sufentanil, which remained stable under further sufentanil application. In contrast, the early cortical potentials Na and Pa increased only slightly in latencies. This increase was statistically significant at 4 micrograms/kg for Na and at 3 and 4 micrograms/kg for Pa. For the amplitudes Na/Pa and Pa/Nb there was only a slight and statistically insignificant reduction. After the largest dose of sufentanil (3-5 micrograms/kg) Na and Pa showed a similar pattern as in awake patients. We conclude that sufentanil does not differ essentially from alfentanil, fentanyl, and morphine with regard on its effects on MLAEP.


Assuntos
Anestesia Geral , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/efeitos dos fármacos , Sufentanil/farmacologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação/efeitos dos fármacos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA