Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38582939

RESUMO

Chronic psychostimulant use causes long-lasting changes to neural and cognitive function that persist after long periods of abstinence. As cocaine users transition from drug use to abstinence, a parallel transition from hyperactivity to hypoactivity has been found in orbitofrontal-striatal glucose metabolism and striatal D2/D3-receptor activity. Targeting these changes pharmacologically, using highly selective dopamine D3-receptor (D3R) antagonists and partial agonists, has shown promise in reducing drug-taking, and attenuating relapse in animal models of cocaine and opioid use disorder. However, much less attention has been paid to treating the loss of insight, operationalized as the inability to infer likely outcomes, associated with chronic psychostimulant use. Here we tested the selective D3R antagonist VK4-116 as a treatment for this loss in rats with a prior history of cocaine use. Male and female rats were first trained to self-administer cocaine or a sucrose liquid for 2 weeks. After 4 weeks of abstinence, performance was assessed using a sensory preconditioning (SPC) learning paradigm. Rats were given VK4-116 (15 mg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle 30 min prior to each SPC training session, thus creating four drug-treatment groups: sucrose-vehicle, sucrose-VK4-116, cocaine-vehicle, cocaine-VK4-116. The control groups (sucrose-vehicle, sucrose-VK4-116) showed normal sensory preconditioning, whereas cocaine use (cocaine-vehicle) selectively disrupted responding to the preconditioned cue, an effect that was reversed in the cocaine-VK4-116 group, which demonstrating responding to the preconditioned cue at levels comparable to controls. These preclinical findings demonstrate that highly selective dopamine D3R antagonists, particularly VK4-116, can reverse the long-term negative behavioral consequences of cocaine use.

2.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Sep 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37732238

RESUMO

Chronic psychostimulant use can cause long lasting changes to neural and cognitive function that persist even after long periods of abstinence. As cocaine users transition from drug use to abstinence, a parallel transition from hyperactivity to hypoactivity has been found in orbitofrontal-striatal glucose metabolism, and striatal D2/D3 receptor activity. Targeting these changes pharmacologically, using highly selective dopamine D3 receptor (D3R) antagonists and partial agonists, has shown significant promise in reducing drug-taking, and attenuating relapse in animal models of cocaine and opioid use disorder. However, much less attention has been focused on treating inflexible and potentially maladaptive non-drug behaviors following chronic psychostimulant use. Here we tested the selective D3R antagonist VK4-116 as a treatment for the long-term behavioral inflexibility in abstinent male and female rats with a prior history of chronic cocaine use. Rats were first trained to self-administer cocaine (0.75 mg/kg/reinforcer) or a sucrose liquid (10%, .04 mL/reinforcer) for 2 weeks (FR1 schedule, max 60 reinforcers in 3 hrs/ day), followed by 4 weeks of abstinence. Cognitive and behavioral flexibilities were then assessed using a sensory preconditioning (SPC) learning paradigm. Rats were given an VK4-116 (15 mg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle 30 mins prior to each SPC training session, thus creating four drug-treatment groups: sucrose-vehicle, sucrose-VK4-116, cocaine-vehicle, cocaine-VK4-116. The control groups (sucrose-vehicle, sucrose-VK4-116) demonstrated significant evidence of flexible SPC behavior, whereas cocaine use (cocaine-vehicle) disrupted SPC behavior. Remarkably, the D3R antagonist VK4-116 mitigated this cocaine deficit in the cocaine-VK4-116 group, demonstrating flexible SPC to levels comparable to the control groups. These preclinical findings demonstrate that highly selective dopamine D3R antagonists, particularly VK4-116, show significant promise as a pharmacological treatment for the long-term negative behavioral consequences of cocaine use disorder.

3.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Apr 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37090565

RESUMO

In some individuals, drug-associated cues subsume potent control of behavior, such as the elicitation of drug craving1-3 and automatized drug use4. The intensity of this cue reactivity is highly predictive of relapse and other clinical outcomes in substance use disorders5,6. It has been postulated that this cue reactivity is driven by augmentation of dopamine release over the course of chronic drug use7. Here we carried out longitudinal recording and manipulation of cue-evoked dopamine signaling across phases of substance-use related behavior in rats. We observed a subset of individuals that exhibited increased cue reactivity and escalated drug consumption, two cardinal features of substance use disorders. In these individuals, cue-evoked phasic dopamine release underwent diametrically opposed changes in amplitude, determined by the context in which the cue is presented. Dopamine evoked by non-contingent cue presentation increased over drug use, producing greater cue reactivity; whereas dopamine evoked by contingent cue presentation decreased over drug use, producing escalation of drug consumption. Therefore, despite being in opposite directions, these dopamine trajectories each promote core symptoms of substance use disorders.

4.
Nat Neurosci ; 26(1): 64-78, 2023 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36510112

RESUMO

Extended wakefulness is associated with reduced performance and the build-up of sleep pressure. In the cortex, this manifests as changes in network activity. These changes show local variation depending on the waking experience, and their underlying mechanisms represent targets for overcoming the effects of tiredness. Here, we reveal a central role for intracellular chloride regulation, which sets the strength of postsynaptic inhibition via GABAA receptors in cortical pyramidal neurons. Wakefulness results in depolarizing shifts in the equilibrium potential for GABAA receptors, reflecting local activity-dependent processes during waking and involving changes in chloride cotransporter activity. These changes underlie electrophysiological and behavioral markers of local sleep pressure within the cortex, including the levels of slow-wave activity during non-rapid eye movement sleep and low-frequency oscillatory activity and reduced performance levels in the sleep-deprived awake state. These findings identify chloride regulation as a crucial link between sleep-wake history, cortical activity and behavior.


Assuntos
Cloretos , Sono , Cloretos/farmacologia , Sono/fisiologia , Vigília/fisiologia , Fenômenos Eletrofisiológicos , Ácido gama-Aminobutírico/farmacologia , Eletroencefalografia
5.
Mol Psychiatry ; 28(2): 579-587, 2023 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36460723

RESUMO

Psychosis in disorders like schizophrenia is commonly associated with aberrant salience and elevated striatal dopamine. However, the underlying cause(s) of this hyper-dopaminergic state remain elusive. Various lines of evidence point to glutamatergic dysfunction and impairments in synaptic plasticity in the etiology of schizophrenia, including deficits associated with the GluA1 AMPAR subunit. GluA1 knockout (Gria1-/-) mice provide a model of impaired synaptic plasticity in schizophrenia and exhibit a selective deficit in a form of short-term memory which underlies short-term habituation. As such, these mice are unable to reduce attention to recently presented stimuli. In this study we used fast-scan cyclic voltammetry to measure phasic dopamine responses in the nucleus accumbens of Gria1-/- mice to determine whether this behavioral phenotype might be a key driver of a hyper-dopaminergic state. There was no effect of GluA1 deletion on electrically-evoked dopamine responses in anaesthetized mice, demonstrating normal endogenous release properties of dopamine neurons in Gria1-/- mice. Furthermore, dopamine signals were initially similar in Gria1-/- mice compared to controls in response to both sucrose rewards and neutral light stimuli. They were also equally sensitive to changes in the magnitude of delivered rewards. In contrast, however, these stimulus-evoked dopamine signals failed to habituate with repeated presentations in Gria1-/- mice, resulting in a task-relevant, hyper-dopaminergic phenotype. Thus, here we show that GluA1 dysfunction, resulting in impaired short-term habituation, is a key driver of enhanced striatal dopamine responses, which may be an important contributor to aberrant salience and psychosis in psychiatric disorders like schizophrenia.


Assuntos
Dopamina , Habituação Psicofisiológica , Camundongos , Animais , Camundongos Knockout , Memória de Curto Prazo , Fenótipo
6.
Curr Biol ; 32(24): 5364-5373.e4, 2022 12 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36368324

RESUMO

Recording action potentials extracellularly during behavior has led to fundamental discoveries regarding neural function-hippocampal neurons respond to locations in space,1 motor cortex neurons encode movement direction,2 and dopamine neurons signal reward prediction errors3-observations undergirding current theories of cognition,4 movement,5 and learning.6 Recently it has become possible to measure calcium flux, an internal cellular signal related to spiking. The ability to image calcium flux in anatomically7,8 or genetically9 identified neurons can extend our knowledge of neural circuit function by allowing activity to be monitored in specific cell types or projections, or in the same neurons across many days. However, while initial studies were grounded in prior unit recording work, it has become fashionable to assume that calcium is identical to spiking, even though the spike-to-fluorescence transformation is nonlinear, noisy, and unpredictable under real-world conditions.10 It remains an open question whether calcium provides a high-fidelity representation of single-unit activity in awake, behaving subjects. Here, we have addressed this question by recording both signals in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) of rats during olfactory discrimination learning. Activity in the OFC during olfactory learning has been well-studied in humans,11,12,13,14 nonhuman primates,15,16 and rats,17,18,19,20,21 where it has been shown to signal information about both the sensory properties of odor cues and the rewards they predict. Our single-unit results replicated prior findings, whereas the calcium signal provided only a degraded estimate of the information available in the single-unit spiking, reflecting primarily reward value.


Assuntos
Cálcio , Aprendizagem , Ratos , Humanos , Animais , Ratos Long-Evans , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Neurônios Dopaminérgicos , Recompensa
7.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 16: 983480, 2022.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36439968

RESUMO

Reward predictive cues can selectively motivate instrumental behaviors that predict the same rewarding outcomes, an effect known as specific Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (PIT). This selective effect is thought to be mediated by a representation of the sensory specific properties of an outcome, that has become associated with both the Pavlovian cue and the instrumental response during initial learning. Specific satiety is a common method of outcome devaluation that reduces an outcome's value but might also lead to the habituation of the outcome's sensory properties. Previous research has demonstrated that specific PIT is insensitive to changes in specific outcome value following taste aversion devaluation, as well as general satiety manipulations, and therefore specific satiety should not disrupt specific PIT by reducing outcome value. The present rodent experiments used a specific satiety devaluation procedure immediately prior to a specific PIT test to show that habituation of these outcome specific sensory representations can disrupt its efficacy as a stimulus and abolish the specific PIT effect. Experiment 1 employed a two-lever choice test to show that a non-devalued stimulus supports specific PIT, whereas a devalued stimulus abolished the specific PIT effect. Experiment 2 replicated this procedure while controlling for response competition by using a single-lever test to confirm that a devalued stimulus abolishes the specific PIT effect. These findings demonstrate that specific satiety can disrupt the ability of an outcome specific representation to support specific PIT. Given previous findings that specific PIT is insensitive to changes in outcome value by general satiety and taste aversion devaluation, this suggests that specific satiety devaluation might disrupt the use of sensory specific outcome representations to guide behavior via a mechanism that is independent of the outcome's current value.

9.
Neuropsychopharmacology ; 47(9): 1721-1731, 2022 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35478011

RESUMO

It is well established that dopamine transmission is integral in mediating the influence of reward expectations on reward-seeking actions. However, the precise causal role of dopamine transmission in moment-to-moment reward-motivated behavioral control remains contentious, particularly in contexts where it is necessary to refrain from responding to achieve a beneficial outcome. To examine this, we manipulated dopamine transmission pharmacologically as rats performed a Go/No-Go task that required them to either make or withhold action to gain either a small or large reward. D1R Stimulation potentiated cue-driven action initiation, including fast impulsive actions on No-Go trials. By contrast, D1R blockade primarily disrupted the successful completion of Go trial sequences. Surprisingly, while after global D1R blockade this was characterized by a general retardation of reward-seeking actions, nucleus accumbens core (NAcC) D1R blockade had no effect on the speed of action initiation or impulsive actions. Instead, fine-grained analyses showed that this manipulation decreased the precision of animals' goal-directed actions, even though they usually still followed the appropriate response sequence. Strikingly, such "unfocused" responding could also be observed off-drug, particularly when only a small reward was on offer. These findings suggest that the balance of activity at NAcC D1Rs plays a key role in enabling the rapid activation of a focused, reward-seeking state to enable animals to efficiently and accurately achieve their goal.


Assuntos
Dopamina , Núcleo Accumbens , Animais , Dopamina/fisiologia , Motivação , Ratos , Receptores de Dopamina D1 , Recompensa
10.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 239(1): 123-140, 2022 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34762147

RESUMO

The serotonin (5-HT) system, particularly the 5-HT2C receptor, has consistently been implicated in behavioural control. However, while some studies have focused on the role 5-HT2C receptors play in regulating motivation to work for reward, others have highlighted its importance in response restraint. To date, it is unclear how 5-HT transmission at this receptor regulates the balance of response invigoration and restraint in anticipation of future reward. In addition, it remains to be established how 5-HT2C receptors gate the influence of internal versus cue-driven processes over reward-guided actions. To elucidate these issues, we investigated the effects of administering the 5-HT2C receptor antagonist SB242084, both systemically and directly into the nucleus accumbens core (NAcC), in rats performing a Go/No-Go task for small or large rewards. The results were compared to the administration of d-amphetamine into the NAcC, which has previously been shown to promote behavioural activation. Systemic perturbation of 5-HT2C receptors-but crucially not intra-NAcC infusions-consistently boosted rats' performance and instrumental vigour on Go trials when they were required to act. Concomitantly, systemic administration also reduced their ability to withhold responding for rewards on No-Go trials, particularly late in the holding period. Notably, these effects were often apparent only when the reward on offer was small. By contrast, inducing a hyperdopaminergic state in the NAcC with d-amphetamine strongly impaired response restraint on No-Go trials both early and late in the holding period, as well as speeding action initiation. Together, these findings suggest that 5-HT2C receptor transmission, outside the NAcC, shapes the vigour of ongoing goal-directed action as well as the likelihood of responding as a function of expected reward.


Assuntos
Receptor 5-HT2C de Serotonina , Serotonina , Animais , Motivação , Núcleo Accumbens , Ratos , Recompensa
11.
Cereb Cortex Commun ; 2(1): tgab010, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34296155

RESUMO

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is a critical structure in the flexible control of value-based behaviors. OFC dysfunction is typically only detected when task or environmental contingencies change, against a backdrop of apparently intact initial acquisition and behavior. While intact acquisition following OFC lesions in simple Pavlovian cue-outcome conditioning is often predicted by models of OFC function, this predicted null effect has not been thoroughly investigated. Here, we test the effects of lesions and temporary muscimol inactivation of the rodent lateral OFC on the acquisition of a simple single cue-outcome relationship. Surprisingly, pretraining lesions significantly enhanced acquisition after overtraining, whereas post-training lesions and inactivation significantly impaired acquisition. This impaired acquisition to the cue reflects a disruption of behavioral control and not learning since the cue could also act as an effective blocking stimulus in an associative blocking procedure. These findings suggest that even simple cue-outcome representations acquired in the absence of OFC function are impoverished. Therefore, while OFC function is often associated with flexible behavioral control in complex environments, it is also involved in very simple Pavlovian acquisition where complex cue-outcome relationships are irrelevant to task performance.

12.
Behav Neurosci ; 135(2): 165-173, 2021 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34060873

RESUMO

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) plays a critical role in the flexible control of behaviors and has been the focus of increasing research interest. However, there have been a number of controversies around the exact theoretical role of the OFC. One potential source of these issues is the comparison of evidence from different studies, particularly across species, which focus on different specific sub-regions within the OFC. Furthermore, there is emerging evidence that there may be functional diversity across the OFC which may account for these theoretical differences. Therefore, in this review we consider evidence supporting functional heterogeneity within the OFC and how it relates to underlying anatomical heterogeneity. We highlight the importance of anatomical and functional distinctions within the traditionally defined OFC subregions across the medial-lateral axis, which are often not differentiated for practical and historical reasons. We then consider emerging evidence of even finer-grained distinctions within these defined subregions along the anterior-posterior axis. These fine-grained anatomical considerations reveal a pattern of dissociable, but often complementary functions within the OFC. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Córtex Pré-Frontal
13.
Behav Neurosci ; 135(2): 226-244, 2021 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34060876

RESUMO

Our understanding of orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) function has progressed remarkably over the past decades in part due to theoretical advances in associative and reinforcement learning theories. These theoretical accounts of OFC function have implicated the region in progressively more psychologically refined processes from the value and sensory-specific properties of expected outcomes to the representation and inference over latent state representations in cognitive maps of task space. While these accounts have been successful at modeling many of the effects of causal manipulation of OFC function in both rodents and primates, recent findings suggest that further refinement of our current models are still required. Here, we briefly review how our understanding of OFC function has developed to understand two cardinal deficits following OFC dysfunction: Reversal learning and outcome devaluation. We then consider recent findings that OFC dysfunction also significantly affects initial acquisition learning, often assumed to be intact. To account for these findings, we consider a possible role for the OFC in the arbitration and exploration between model-free (MF) and model-based (MB) learning systems, offline updating of MB representations. While the function of the OFC as a whole is still likely to be integral to the formation and use of a cognitive map of task space, these refinements suggest a way in which distinct orbital subregions, such as the rodent lateral OFC, might contribute to this overall function. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Córtex Pré-Frontal , Roedores , Animais , Reforço Psicológico , Reversão de Aprendizagem
14.
Neuroscience ; 460: 53-68, 2021 04 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33609638

RESUMO

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has been anatomically divided into a number of subregions along its medial-lateral axis, which behavioral research suggests have distinct functions. Recently, evidence has emerged suggesting functional diversity is also present along the anterior-posterior axis of the rodent OFC. However, the patterns of anatomical connections that underlie these differences have not been well characterized. Here, we use the retrograde tracer cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) to simultaneously label the projections into the anterior lateral (ALO), posterior lateral (PLO), and posterior ventral (PVO) portions of the rat OFC. Our methodological approach allowed us to simultaneously compare the density and input patterns into these OFC subdivisions. We observed distinct and topographically organized projection patterns into ALO, PLO, and PVO from the mediodorsal and the submedius nuclei of the thalamus. We also observed different levels of connectivity strength into these OFC subdivisions from the amygdala, motor cortex, sensory cortices and medial prefrontal cortical structures, including medial OFC, infralimbic and prelimbic cortices. Interestingly, while labelling in some of these input regions revealed only a gradient in connectivity strength, other regions seem to project almost exclusively to specific OFC subdivisions. Moreover, differences in input patterns between ALO and PLO were as pronounced as those between PLO and PVO. Together, our results support the existence of distinct anatomical circuits within lateral OFC along its anterior-posterior axis.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo , Córtex Pré-Frontal , Animais , Córtex Cerebral , Vias Neurais , Lobo Parietal , Ratos , Tálamo
15.
Addict Biol ; 24(5): 849-859, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29920857

RESUMO

Restricting when and where smoking can occur is a major focus of public health policies in Western countries. In conjunction with increased taxation, these approaches have contributed to a reduction in smoking uptake among adolescents, yet the consequences for established smokers are less clear. In order to further explore this relationship, we developed a novel animal model of restricted nicotine self-administration. Rats were trained to choose between three doses of nicotine (15, 30 and 60 µg/kg/infusion) under conditions where nicotine was (1) freely available at a low cost (20-second post-infusion time-out, fixed-ratio 1 [FR1]), (2) available under restricted access at a low cost (300-second post-infusion time-out, FR1), or (3) freely available at a high cost (20-second post-infusion time-out, FR5). We demonstrate that as access to nicotine is restricted or when cost increases, rats compensate for these changes by increasing their intake of the highest dose of nicotine available. This preference was impervious to treatment with the smoking cessation medication varenicline, but was reduced when the cost of the highest dose only was increased, or when nicotine was again made freely available at a low cost. These results provide the first evidence in rats that nicotine availability and cost influence nicotine choice independently of variations in nicotine and context exposure. They imply that established smokers may compensate for changes in the availability and cost of tobacco by increasing their rate of smoking when they are free to do so.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Nicotina/administração & dosagem , Agonistas Nicotínicos/administração & dosagem , Autoadministração , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Comportamento de Escolha/efeitos dos fármacos , Infusões Intravenosas , Política Pública , Ratos , Política Antifumo , Agentes de Cessação do Hábito de Fumar/farmacologia , Produtos do Tabaco , Vareniclina/farmacologia
16.
Elife ; 72018 07 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30044220

RESUMO

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is critical for updating reward-directed behaviours flexibly when outcomes are devalued or when task contingencies are reversed. Failure to update behaviour in outcome devaluation and reversal learning procedures are considered canonical deficits following OFC lesions in non-human primates and rodents. We examined the generality of these findings in rodents using lesions of the rodent lateral OFC (LO) in instrumental action-outcome and Pavlovian cue-outcome devaluation procedures. LO lesions disrupted outcome devaluation in Pavlovian but not instrumental procedures. Furthermore, although both anterior and posterior LO lesions disrupted Pavlovian outcome devaluation, only posterior LO lesions were found to disrupt reversal learning. Posterior but not anterior LO lesions were also found to disrupt the attribution of motivational value to Pavlovian cues in sign-tracking. These novel dissociable task- and subregion-specific effects suggest a way to reconcile contradictory findings between rodent and non-human primate OFC research.


Assuntos
Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Reversão de Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Animais , Condicionamento Psicológico/fisiologia , Masculino , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Ratos Long-Evans , Paladar
17.
Learn Mem ; 22(5): 247-57, 2015 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25878137

RESUMO

The present study investigated how oxytocin (OT) signaling in the central (CeA) and basolateral (BLA) amygdala affects acquisition, expression, and extinction of context-conditioned fear (freezing) in rats. In the first set of experiments, acquisition of fear to a shocked context was impaired by a preconditioning infusion of synthetic OT into the CeA (Experiment 1) or BLA (Experiment 2). In the second set of experiments, expression of context fear was enhanced by a pre- or post-extinction CeA infusion of synthetic OT (Experiments 3-6) or a selective OT receptor agonist, TGOT (Experiment 4). This enhancement of fear was blocked by coadministration of an OT receptor antagonist, OTA (Experiment 5) and context fear was suppressed by administration of the antagonist alone (Experiment 6). In the third set of experiments, expression of context fear was suppressed, not enhanced, by a preextinction BLA infusion of synthetic OT or a selective OT receptor agonist, TGOT (Experiments 7 and 8). This suppression of fear was blocked by coadministration of the OT receptor antagonist, OTA (Experiment 8). Taken together, these findings show that the involvement of the CeA and BLA in expression and extinction of context-conditioned fear is dissociable, and imply a critical role for oxytocin signaling in amygdala-based regulation of aversive learning.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/metabolismo , Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Extinção Psicológica/fisiologia , Medo/fisiologia , Ocitocina/metabolismo , Transdução de Sinais/fisiologia , Tonsila do Cerebelo/efeitos dos fármacos , Animais , Condicionamento Clássico/efeitos dos fármacos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Eletrochoque , Extinção Psicológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Medo/efeitos dos fármacos , Reação de Congelamento Cataléptica/efeitos dos fármacos , Reação de Congelamento Cataléptica/fisiologia , Masculino , Ocitocina/análogos & derivados , Ocitocina/farmacologia , Ratos , Receptores de Ocitocina/antagonistas & inibidores , Transdução de Sinais/efeitos dos fármacos
18.
Behav Res Ther ; 56: 22-9, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24755207

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Benzodiazepines reduce the effectiveness of fear extinction in rodents and of exposure therapy in people suffering from anxiety disorders if given concomitantly with the behavioral treatment from its onset. The present experiments used rats to examine whether benzodiazepines had the same detrimental effect when given after some initial extinction had been conducted drug-free. METHODS: Rats were trained to fear a context (Experiments 1 and 2) or discrete cue (Experiment 3) and were extinguished to the context or cue under a benzodiazepine (midazolam) or vehicle. Extinction occurred either continuously in one session, with the drug or vehicle administered prior to the onset, or divided into two sessions, with the drug or vehicle administered prior to the second session. Rats were then tested, drug-free, for fear of the context or CS. RESULTS: Midazolam disrupted context and cue extinction when administered prior to the initial session but failed to disrupt extinction when given prior to the second session. CONCLUSIONS: The results in an animal model confirm that the effectiveness of extinction can be reduced when combined with benzodiazepines. They also suggest that the effectiveness of extinction will not be reduced when combined with a benzodiazepine if the patient has undergone some initial extinction drug-free.


Assuntos
Benzodiazepinas/administração & dosagem , Benzodiazepinas/farmacologia , Extinção Psicológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Midazolam/administração & dosagem , Midazolam/farmacologia , Animais , Condicionamento Clássico/efeitos dos fármacos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Esquema de Medicação , Medo/psicologia , Masculino , Ratos
19.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 108: 78-87, 2014 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23954805

RESUMO

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is argued to be the neural locus of Pavlovian outcome expectancies. Reinforcement learning theories argue that extinction learning in Pavlovian procedures is caused by the discrepancy between the expected value of the outcome (US) that is elicited by a predictive stimulus (CS), and the lack of experienced US. If the OFC represents Pavlovian outcome expectancies that are necessary for extinction learning, then disrupting OFC function prior to extinction training should impair extinction learning. This was tested. In experiment 1, Long Evans rats received infusions of saline or muscimol targeting the lateral OFC prior to three appetitive Pavlovian extinction sessions. Muscimol infused into the OFC disrupted between-session but not within-session extinction behaviour. This finding was not due to muscimol infusions disrupting the memory consolidation process per se as there was no effect of muscimol infusion when administered immediately post session (experiment 2). These findings support a role for the OFC in representing outcome expectancies that are necessary for learning. A number of ways in which disrupting outcome expectancy information might block learning will be discussed in the context of traditional associative learning theories and the associative structures they depend on.


Assuntos
Extinção Psicológica/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Animais , Aprendizagem por Associação/efeitos dos fármacos , Aprendizagem por Associação/fisiologia , Condicionamento Clássico/efeitos dos fármacos , Condicionamento Clássico/fisiologia , Extinção Psicológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Agonistas de Receptores de GABA-A/farmacologia , Masculino , Memória/efeitos dos fármacos , Memória/fisiologia , Muscimol/farmacologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/efeitos dos fármacos , Ratos , Ratos Long-Evans
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA