Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
1.
J Circadian Rhythms ; 21: 2, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37842163

RESUMO

The circadian clock is adjusted by light inputs via the retinohypothalamic tract. Because environmental light is controllable for modern humans at the individual's preference although under social schedules, individual differences in time-related psychology and behavior may be associated with morningness-eveningness preference (M-E preference). To examine this hypothesis, we used the Time Management Scale and Time Anxiety Scale to quantify time-related psychology and behavior. These scales aim to evaluate "awareness of effective time management and utilization" and "anxiety about uncontrollable time schedule and unexpected time-related outcome", respectively. According to our correlation analysis using mid-sleep time as a marker for M-E preference, we obtained results supporting our hypothesis in the correlation between the M-E preference values and the Time Management Scale scores, with larger "time estimation" and "taking each moment as it comes" scores associated with more morningness and eveningness, respectively. Considering that modern humans likely become night owls under artificial light conditions, it appears plausible that lower awareness of time management leads to more eveningness.

3.
Curr Biol ; 31(2): 406-412.e3, 2021 01 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33157025

RESUMO

Identifying the plastic and stable components of the visual cortex after retinal loss is an important topic in visual neuroscience and neuro-ophthalmology.1-5 Humans with juvenile macular degeneration (JMD) show significant blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) responses in the primary visual area (V1) lesion projection zone (LPZ),6 despite the absence of the feedforward signals from the degenerated retina. Our previous study7 reported that V1 LPZ responds to full-field visual stimuli during the one-back task (OBT), not during passive viewing, suggesting the involvement of task-related feedback signals. Aiming to clarify whether visual inputs to the intact retina are necessary for the LPZ responses, here, we measured BOLD responses to tactile and auditory stimuli for both JMD patients and control participants with and without OBT. Participants were instructed to close their eyes during the experiment for the purpose of eliminating retinal inputs. Without OBT, no V1 responses were detected in both groups of participants. With OBT, to the contrary, both stimuli caused substantial V1 responses in JMD patients, but not controls. Furthermore, we also found that the task-dependent activity in V1 LPZ became less pronounced when JMD patients opened their eyes, suggesting that task-related feedback signals can be partially suppressed by residual feedforward signals. Modality-independent V1 LPZ responses only in the task condition suggest that V1 LPZ responses reflect task-related feedback signals rather than reorganized feedforward visual inputs.


Assuntos
Doença de Stargardt/fisiopatologia , Córtex Visual/fisiopatologia , Vias Visuais/fisiopatologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Adulto , Idade de Início , Idoso , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Retroalimentação Fisiológica , Feminino , Voluntários Saudáveis , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estimulação Luminosa , Retina/patologia , Doença de Stargardt/patologia , Tato , Córtex Visual/diagnóstico por imagem , Vias Visuais/diagnóstico por imagem
4.
Jpn J Ophthalmol ; 65(1): 133-142, 2021 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33185792

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Leber's hereditary optic neuropathy (LHON) is a mitochondrial neuropathy that causes acute vision loss. Idebenone, a short-chain ubiquinone analog that preserves mitochondrial function is thought to suppress disease progression in early-onset LHON patients. We investigated the effects of idebenone in Japanese LHON patients. STUDY DESIGN: Prospective, interventional, non-comparative study in patients with definite LHON diagnosis, under trial registration number UMIN000017939. METHODS: Fifty-seven patients received 900 mg/day idebenone for 24 weeks. We measured baseline best-corrected visual acuity, visual fields, critical fusion frequency and retinal ganglion cell layer complex thickness; we assessed efficacy at 24 and 48 weeks, and safety throughout. RESULTS: Patients were predominantly male (91.2%) and most had an mt.11778G>A mutation (94.7%). All patients tolerated idebenone therapy well. Data from the 51 mt.11778 patients were compared with their baseline data. At 48 weeks, significant improvement in best-corrected visual acuity was observed in 17 patients (33.3%). Furthermore, 25.5% of patients showed improvements in visual fields and 33.3% in critical fusion frequency. However, retinal ganglion cell layer complex thickness was significantly reduced. Among patients who started idebenone >1 year after disease onset, visual improvement was found in 12 (38.7%). Among patients who developed LHON before 19 years of age, visual improvement was found in 11 (42.3%). CONCLUSION: Idebenone's potential and favorable safety profile were confirmed in Japanese LHON patients. However, this study had no placebo group; therefore, we need to undertake a prospective intervention study to further investigate the therapeutic effects of Idebenone in Japanese LHON patients.


Assuntos
Atrofia Óptica Hereditária de Leber , Ubiquinona , Humanos , Japão/epidemiologia , Masculino , Atrofia Óptica Hereditária de Leber/diagnóstico , Atrofia Óptica Hereditária de Leber/tratamento farmacológico , Atrofia Óptica Hereditária de Leber/genética , Estudos Prospectivos , Ubiquinona/análogos & derivados , Acuidade Visual
5.
Iperception ; 11(3): 2041669520937320, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32647561

RESUMO

Many studies have investigated various effects of smooth pursuit on visual motion processing, especially the effects related to the additional retinal shifts produced by eye movement. In this article, we show that the perception of apparent motion during smooth pursuit is determined by the interelement proximity in retinal coordinates and also by the proximity in objective world coordinates. In Experiment 1, we investigated the perceived direction of the two-frame apparent motion of a square-wave grating with various displacement sizes under fixation and pursuit viewing conditions. The retinal and objective displacements between the two frames agreed with each other under the fixation condition. However, the displacements differed by 180 degrees in terms of phase shift, under the pursuit condition. The proportions of the reported motion direction between the two viewing conditions did not coincide when they were plotted as a function of either the retinal displacement or of the objective displacement; however, they did coincide when plotted as a function of a mixture of the two. The result from Experiment 2 showed that the perceived jump size of the apparent motion was also dependent on both retinal and objective displacements. Our findings suggest that the detection of the apparent motion during smooth pursuit considers the retinal proximity and also the objective proximity. This mechanism may assist with the selection of a motion path that is more likely to occur in the real world and, therefore, be useful for ensuring perceptual stability during smooth pursuit.

6.
Brain Struct Funct ; 223(8): 3889-3900, 2018 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29951918

RESUMO

We investigated the impact of age-related macular degeneration (AMD) on visual acuity and the visual white matter. We combined an adaptive cortical atlas and diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (dMRI) and tractography to separate optic radiation (OR) projections to different retinal eccentricities in human primary visual cortex. We exploited the known anatomical organization of the OR and clinically relevant data to segment the OR into three primary components projecting to fovea, mid- and far-periphery. We measured white matter tissue properties-fractional anisotropy, linearity, planarity, sphericity-along the aforementioned three components of the optic radiation to compare AMD patients and controls. We found differences in white matter properties specific to OR white matter fascicles projecting to primary visual cortex locations corresponding to the location of retinal damage (fovea). Additionally, we show that the magnitude of white matter properties in AMD patients' correlates with visual acuity. In sum, we demonstrate a specific relation between visual loss, anatomical location of retinal damage and white matter damage in AMD patients. Importantly, we demonstrate that these changes are so profound that can be detected using magnetic resonance imaging data with clinical resolution. The conserved mapping between retinal and white matter damage suggests that retinal neurodegeneration might be a primary cause of white matter degeneration in AMD patients. The results highlight the impact of eye disease on brain tissue, a process that may become an important target to monitor during the course of treatment.


Assuntos
Degeneração Macular/patologia , Córtex Visual/patologia , Vias Visuais/patologia , Substância Branca/patologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Imagem de Difusão por Ressonância Magnética , Feminino , Humanos , Degeneração Macular/fisiopatologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Acuidade Visual
7.
J Vis ; 16(14): 2, 2016 11 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27802510

RESUMO

Although it is known that a moving stimulus appears to dilate in duration compared to a stationary stimulus, whether subjective motion devoid of stimulus motion is sufficient remains unknown. To elucidate this, we used a motion illusion in which an actually static stimulus clearly appears to move, a useful dissociation between actual and subjective motions. We used the jitter aftereffect resulting from adaptation to dynamic noise as such a tool and measured subjective durations of a static random-dot pattern in which illusory jitter was seen, an actually oscillating pattern mimicking the illusory jitter, and a static pattern without illusory jitter. Pattern oscillation as tiny as fixational eye movements robustly evoked time dilation, and time dilation to a similar extent was also induced by an actually static but subjectively jittering pattern. Taken together with the previous knowledge that this subjective jitter is related to a visually based compensation of spurious retinal image motions due to fixational eye movements, these findings demonstrate that visual duration computation is influenced by a representation at a high-level motion processing stage at which a stable visual world despite jittery retinal inputs has been established.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Pós-Efeito de Figura/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Adaptação Ocular/fisiologia , Dilatação , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Retina/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
8.
J Vis ; 15(13): 2, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26381833

RESUMO

When eyes track a moving target, a stationary background environment moves in the direction opposite to the eye movement on the observer's retina. Here, we report a novel effect in which smooth pursuit can enhance the retinal motion in the direction opposite to eye movement, under certain conditions. While performing smooth pursuit, the observers were presented with a counterphase grating on the retina. The counterphase grating consisted of two drifting component gratings: one drifting in the direction opposite to the eye movement and the other drifting in the same direction as the pursuit. Although the overall perceived motion direction should be ambiguous if only retinal information is considered, our results indicated that the stimulus almost always appeared to be moving in the direction opposite to the pursuit direction. This effect was ascribable to the perceptual dominance of the environmentally stationary component over the other. The effect was robust at suprathreshold contrasts, but it disappeared at lower overall contrasts. The effect was not associated with motion capture by a reference frame served by peripheral moving images. Our findings also indicate that the brain exploits eye-movement information not only for eye-contingent image motion suppression but also to develop an ecologically plausible interpretation of ambiguous retinal motion signals. Based on this biological assumption, we argue that visual processing has the functional consequence of reducing the apparent motion blur of a stationary background pattern during eye movements and that it does so through integration of the trajectories of pattern and color signals.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos
9.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 855, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25404904

RESUMO

The central edge of an opposing pair of luminance gradients (COC edge) makes adjoining regions with identical luminance appear to be different. This brightness illusion, called the Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet effect (COCe), can be explained by low-level spatial filtering mechanisms (Dakin and Bex, 2003). Also, the COCe is greatly reduced when the stimulus lacks a frame element surrounding the COC edge (Purves et al., 1999). This indicates that the COCe can be modulated by extra contextual cues that are related to ideas about lighting priors. In this study, we examined whether processing for contextual modulation could be independent of the main COCe processing mediated by the filtering mechanism. We displayed the COC edge and frame element at physically different times. Then, while varying the onset asynchrony between them and changing the luminance contrast of the frame element, we measured the size of the COCe. We found that the COCe was observed in the temporal range of around 600-800 ms centered at the 0 ms (from around -400 to 400 ms in stimulus onset asynchrony), which was much larger than the range of typical visual persistency. More importantly, this temporal range did not change significantly regardless of differences in the luminance contrast of the frame element (5-100%), in the durations of COC edge and/or the frame element (50 or 200 ms), in the display condition (interocular or binocular), and in the type of lines constituting the frame element (solid or illusory lines). Results suggest that the visual system can bind the COC edge and frame element with a temporal window of ~1 s to estimate surface brightness. Information from the basic filtering mechanism and information of contextual cue are separately processed and are linked afterwards.

10.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 55(10): 6976-86, 2014 Sep 25.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25257055

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Patients with Leber hereditary optic neuropathy (LHON) and cone-rod dystrophy (CRD) have central vision loss; but CRD damages the retinal photoreceptor layer, and LHON damages the retinal ganglion cell (RGC) layer. Using diffusion MRI, we measured how these two types of retinal damage affect the optic tract (ganglion cell axons) and optic radiation (geniculo-striate axons). METHODS: Adult onset CRD (n = 5), LHON (n = 6), and healthy controls (n = 14) participated in the study. We used probabilistic fiber tractography to identify the optic tract and the optic radiation. We compared axial and radial diffusivity at many positions along the optic tract and the optic radiation. RESULTS: In both types of patients, diffusion measures within the optic tract and the optic radiation differ from controls. The optic tract change is principally a decrease in axial diffusivity; the optic radiation change is principally an increase in radial diffusivity. CONCLUSIONS: Both photoreceptor layer (CRD) and retinal ganglion cell (LHON) retinal disease causes substantial change in the visual white matter. These changes can be measured using diffusion MRI. The diffusion changes measured in the optic tract and the optic radiation differ, suggesting that they are caused by different biological mechanisms.


Assuntos
Morte Celular , Fibras Nervosas/patologia , Atrofia Óptica Hereditária de Leber/diagnóstico , Células Ganglionares da Retina/patologia , Vias Visuais/patologia , Substância Branca/patologia , Adulto , Idoso , Imagem de Difusão por Ressonância Magnética , Progressão da Doença , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tomografia de Coerência Óptica , Adulto Jovem
11.
J Vis ; 13(12): 21, 2013 Oct 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24172609

RESUMO

The static envelope of a Gabor patch with a moving carrier appears to shift in the direction of the carrier motion; this phenomenon is known as the motion-induced position shift (De Valois & De Valois, 1991; Ramachandran & Anstis, 1990). This conventional stimulus configuration contains at least three covarying factors: the retinal carrier velocity, the environmental carrier velocity, and the carrier velocity relative to the envelope velocity, which happens to be zero. We manipulated these velocities independently to identify which is critical, and we measured the perceived position of the moving Gabor patch relative to a reference stimulus moving in the same direction at the same speed. In the first experiment, the position of the moving envelope observed with fixation appeared to shift in the direction of the carrier velocity relative to the envelope velocity. Furthermore, the illusion was more pronounced when the carrier moved in a direction opposite to that of the envelope. In the second and third experiments, we measured the illusion during smooth-pursuit eye movement in which the envelope was either static or moving, thereby dissociating retinal and environmental velocities. Under all conditions, the illusion occurred according to the envelope-relative velocity of the carrier. Additionally, the illusion was more pronounced when the carrier and envelope moved in opposite directions. We conclude that the carrier's envelope-relative velocity is the primary determinant of the motion-induced position shift.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Ilusões/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Retina/fisiologia
12.
J Vis ; 12(4)2012 Apr 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22539865

RESUMO

This study explored the orientation connectivity in a contrast modulation processing mechanism, modeled as two filtering stages with nonlinear processing in between, by investigating how a negative aftereffect of a contrast-defined spatial offset is influenced by carrier orientations in the adapting stimulus. After adaptation to multiple, globally presented pairs of Gabor patches with a specific horizontal offset, subjects perceived a vertically aligned test pair of patches as offset in the orientation opposite to that of the adaptor. Although the orientations of the carrier gratings in the adaptor pairs were irrelevant to the task, the aftereffect magnitude depended on them. A large aftereffect was observed when the carrier orientations were parallel and/or perpendicular to the contrast-defined orientation, supporting the notion that second-stage filters receive strong inputs from first-stage filters with parallel and perpendicular orientation preferences. Furthermore, the aftereffect was also large when the carrier for only one patch was parallel or perpendicular, and no significant difference in the aftereffect magnitude was observed whether the adaptor pair contained one or two such patches. These results suggest that connectivity is not strictly selective to parallel and perpendicular relationships. Spatially heterogeneous connectivity might explain the observed effect.


Assuntos
Pós-Efeito de Figura/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Psicofísica/métodos , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
13.
J Neurophysiol ; 107(12): 3493-508, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22442570

RESUMO

Early visual motion signals are local and one-dimensional (1-D). For specification of global two-dimensional (2-D) motion vectors, the visual system should appropriately integrate these signals across orientation and space. Previous neurophysiological studies have suggested that this integration process consists of two computational steps (estimation of local 2-D motion vectors, followed by their spatial pooling), both being identified in the area MT. Psychophysical findings, however, suggest that under certain stimulus conditions, the human visual system can also compute mathematically correct global motion vectors from direct pooling of spatially distributed 1-D motion signals. To study the neural mechanisms responsible for this novel 1-D motion pooling, we conducted human magnetoencephalography (MEG) and functional MRI experiments using a global motion stimulus comprising multiple moving Gabors (global-Gabor motion). In the first experiment, we measured MEG and blood oxygen level-dependent responses while changing motion coherence of global-Gabor motion. In the second experiment, we investigated cortical responses correlated with direction-selective adaptation to the global 2-D motion, not to local 1-D motions. We found that human MT complex (hMT+) responses show both coherence dependency and direction selectivity to global motion based on 1-D pooling. The results provide the first evidence that hMT+ is the locus of 1-D motion pooling, as well as that of conventional 2-D motion pooling.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Córtex Visual/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Magnetoencefalografia , Masculino , Orientação/fisiologia , Oxigênio/sangue , Oxigênio/fisiologia
14.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 5: 125, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22059072

RESUMO

The Craik-O'Brien-Cornsweet (COC) effect demonstrates that perceived lightness depends not only on the retinal input at corresponding visual areas but also on distal retinal inputs. In the COC effect, the central edge of an opposing pair of luminance gradients (COC edge) makes adjoining regions with identical luminance appear to be different. To investigate the underlying mechanisms of the effect, we examined whether the subjective awareness of the COC edge is necessary for the generation of the effect. We manipulated the visibility of the COC edge using visual backward masking and continuous flash suppression while monitoring subjective reports regarding online percepts and aftereffects of adaptation. Psychophysical results showed that the online percept of the COC effect nearly vanishes in conditions where the COC edge is rendered invisible. On the other hand, the results of adaptation experiments showed that the COC edge is still processed at the early stage even under the perceptual suppression. These results suggest that processing of the COC edge at the early stage is not sufficient for generating the COC effect, and that subjective awareness of the COC edge is necessary.

15.
J Vis ; 11(6)2011 May 20.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21602556

RESUMO

Motion perception is compromised at equiluminance. Because previous investigations have been primarily carried out under fixation conditions, it remains unknown whether and how equiluminant color motion comes into play in the velocity compensation for retinal image motion due to smooth pursuit eye movement. We measured the retinal image velocity required to reach subjective stationarity for a horizontally drifting sinusoidal grating in the presence of horizontal smooth pursuit. The grating was defined by luminance or chromatic modulation. When the subjective stationarity of the color motion was shifted toward environmental stationarity, compared with the subjective stationarity of luminance motion, that of color motion was farther from retinal stationarity, indicating that a slowing of color motion occurred before this factor contributed to the process by which retinal motion was integrated with a biological estimate of eye velocity during pursuit. The gain in the estimate of eye velocity per se was unchanged irrespective of whether the stimulus was defined by luminance or by color. Indeed, the subjective reduction in the speed of color motion during fixation was accounted for by the same amount of deterioration in speed. From these results, we conclude that the motion deterioration at equiluminance takes place prior to the velocity comparison.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Luminescência , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Adulto , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Retina/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
16.
PLoS One ; 5(6): e11214, 2010 Jun 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20574511

RESUMO

Human observers see a single mixed color (yellow) when different colors (red and green) rapidly alternate. Accumulating evidence suggests that the critical temporal frequency beyond which chromatic fusion occurs does not simply reflect the temporal limit of peripheral encoding. However, it remains poorly understood how the central processing controls the fusion frequency. Here we show that the fusion frequency can be elevated by extra-retinal signals during smooth pursuit. This eye movement can keep the image of a moving target in the fovea, but it also introduces a backward retinal sweep of the stationary background pattern. We found that the fusion frequency was higher when retinal color changes were generated by pursuit-induced background motions than when the same retinal color changes were generated by object motions during eye fixation. This temporal improvement cannot be ascribed to a general increase in contrast gain of specific neural mechanisms during pursuit, since the improvement was not observed with a pattern flickering without changing position on the retina or with a pattern moving in the direction opposite to the background motion during pursuit. Our findings indicate that chromatic fusion is controlled by a cortical mechanism that suppresses motion blur. A plausible mechanism is that eye-movement signals change spatiotemporal trajectories along which color signals are integrated so as to reduce chromatic integration at the same locations (i.e., along stationary trajectories) on the retina that normally causes retinal blur during fixation.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cor , Humanos , Retina/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
17.
PLoS One ; 4(7): e6383, 2009 Jul 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19633711

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Observers misperceive the location of points within a scene as compressed towards the goal of a saccade. However, recent studies suggest that saccadic compression does not occur for discrete elements such as dots when they are perceived as unified objects like a rectangle. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: We investigated the magnitude of horizontal vs. vertical compression for Kanizsa figure (a collection of discrete elements unified into single perceptual objects by illusory contours) and control rectangle figures. Participants were presented with Kanizsa and control figures and had to decide whether the horizontal or vertical length of stimulus was longer using the two-alternative force choice method. Our findings show that large but not small Kanizsa figures are perceived as compressed, that such compression is large in the horizontal dimension and small or nil in the vertical dimension. In contrast to recent findings, we found no saccadic compression for control rectangles. CONCLUSIONS: Our data suggest that compression of Kanizsa figure has been overestimated in previous research due to methodological artifacts, and highlight the importance of studying perceptual phenomena by multiple methods.


Assuntos
Movimentos Sacádicos , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
18.
Nat Neurosci ; 11(5): 541-2, 2008 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18408716

RESUMO

The neural mechanisms underlying visual estimation of subsecond durations remain unknown, but perisaccadic underestimation of interflash intervals may provide a clue as to the nature of these mechanisms. Here we found that simply reducing the flash visibility, particularly the visibility of transient signals, induced similar time underestimation by human observers. Our results suggest that weak transient responses fail to trigger the proper detection of temporal asynchrony, leading to increased perception of simultaneity and apparent time compression.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Humanos , Ilusões/fisiologia , Processos Mentais/fisiologia , Inibição Neural/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo
19.
Spat Vis ; 18(3): 297-316, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16060229

RESUMO

When subjects made a saccade across a single-flashed dot, a flickering dot or a continuous dot, they perceived a dot, an array (phantom array), or a line (phantom line), respectively. We asked subjects to localize both endpoints of the phantom array or line and calculated the perceived lengths. Based on the findings of Matsumiya and Uchikawa (2001), we predicted that the apparent length of the phantom line would be larger than that of the phantom array. In Experiment 1 of the current study, contrary to the prediction, the phantom line was found to be shorter than the phantom array. In Experiment 2, we investigated whether the function underlying the filled-unfilled space illusion (Lewis, 1912) instead of the function underlying the saccadic compression could explain the results. Subjects were asked to localize both endpoints of a line or an array while keeping their eyes fixated. Although the results of Experiment 2 showed that the perceived length of a line was shorter than that of an array, the function underlying the filled-unfilled illusion could not fully account for the results of Experiment 1. To explain the present results, we proposed a model for the localization process and discussed its validity.


Assuntos
Movimento (Física) , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Feminino , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Estimulação Luminosa , Valores de Referência
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA