Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 209
Filtrar
1.
Front Psychol ; 15: 1425389, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39315047

RESUMO

The sexual misperception bias is a cognitive bias in which men tend to overestimate sexual interest from women, potentially shaped by evolutionary mating strategies. Testosterone, often linked to mating behaviors, might play a role in sustaining sexual overperceptions. To explore this possibility, we conducted a placebo-controlled study with 190 heterosexual men, administering either 11 mg of testosterone or a placebo. Participants interacted with an attractive female confederate, while naïve raters assessed the confederate's affiliative behaviors. Our findings suggest that exogenous testosterone did not broadly impact sexual overperception. However, we found that affiliative behavior from the confederate was positively correlated with perceived sexual interest among testosterone-treated, but not placebo-treated men. In addition, we found that this effect among testosterone-treated men was contingent on their self-perceived attractiveness. Specifically, the confederate's affiliative behaviors were positively correlated with perceived sexual interest, but only for testosterone-treated men with average or above average self-perceived attractiveness. Furthermore, our data revealed that men's tendency to project their own short-term and long-term mating interests increases as a function of self-perceived attractiveness, and this coupling is enhanced by testosterone for long-term interest. Taken together, these results suggest that testosterone may potentiate existing biases, particularly when sexual motivation is high, and bias perceptions of friendly behavior when engaging in cross-sex mindreading. This study adds to the understanding of the neuroendocrine bases of social cognition, suggesting that testosterone can affect men's perceptions of potential mates.

2.
Prev Med Rep ; 46: 102856, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39253723

RESUMO

Background: The U.S. Food and Drug Administration authorized the sale and marketing of two very low nicotine cigarettes (VLNC) as modified risk tobacco products. The misperception that VLNC are healthier than regular cigarettes is common. This study explores effective message strategies to inform the public about health risks associated with VLNC use, encourage cigarette smokers to try VLNC, and prevent other tobacco users and non-users from product initiation. Methods: Following the Reasoned Action approach, a VLNC educational message was developed based on the salient beliefs associated with behavioral intention. The message was tested in an online survey conducted in 2018, where 410 participants were randomly assigned to one of the two message conditions (no-message, VLNC message). Message effects were assessed across four tobacco-use groups (non-tobacco users, cigarette-only smokers, cigarette dual/poly smokers, other tobacco users). Results: Compared to the no-message control, the VLNC message condition showed lower nicotine risk perception for all participants, lower misbelief in VLNC safety for non-users and cigarette-only smokers, higher belief in VLNC carcinogenicity for other tobacco users, stronger belief in second-hand smoke harm for cigarette dual/poly smokers and other tobacco users, and higher VLNC intention for cigarette-only smokers. Conclusions: Different messages are needed for different types of tobacco users. Both cigarette smokers and other tobacco users could benefit from messages that acknowledge the non-addictiveness but emphasize the health risks of VLNC. Regulators could consider making physical harm statements a requirement for VLNC packaging and marketing. New strategies need to be explored to inform cigarette dual/poly smokers.

3.
PNAS Nexus ; 3(9): pgae333, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39290440

RESUMO

The misperception of income inequality is often touted as a critical barrier to more widespread support of redistributive policies. Here, we examine to what extent and why (mis)perceptions vary systematically across the income distribution. Drawing on data from four studies (N = 2,744)-including a representative sample and preregistered incentive-compatible experiments-we offer converging evidence that people specifically underestimate the amount of income held by the top of the income distribution. While this selective underestimation is likely driven by multiple mechanisms, including systemic factors, we find that cognitive biases contribute to the observed pattern of results. The rise of inequality in many developed countries has been documented before, and the fact that this growing inequality is largely driven by the outsized gains of the richest individuals may pose new challenges previously underappreciated: our theory and findings highlight that cognitive biases pose a key obstacle to people's recognition of the concentration of income among the richest individuals, and may potentially distort their preferences for redistribution. We conclude by discussing future directions for research and the importance of incorporating behavioral and cognitive limitations into the design of redistributive public policy.

4.
Public Underst Sci ; : 9636625241266150, 2024 Aug 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39104361

RESUMO

A meta-analysis was conducted to quantify the overall effect of health misinformation exposure on shaping misbelief. Aggregation of results from 28 individual randomized controlled trial studies (n = 8752) reveals a positive but small average effect, d = 0.28. Moderation analyses suggest that adults who are younger and female tend to develop higher misbelief if exposed to health misinformation. Furthermore, media platform, message falsity, and misbelief measurements also contribute to the exposure effect. These findings offer nuanced but crucial insights into existing misinformation literature, and development of more effective strategies to mitigate the adverse impacts of health misinformation.

5.
Neuroimage ; 298: 120782, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39128660

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Sleep State Misperception (SSM) is described as the tendency of Insomnia Disorder (ID) patients to overestimate Sleep Latency (SL) and underestimate Total Sleep Time (TST). Literature exploring topographical components in ID with SSM is scarce and does not allow us to fully understand the potential mechanisms underlying this phenomenon. This study aims to evaluate the existence of sleep EEG topography alterations in ID patients associated with SSM compared to Healthy Controls (HC), focusing on two distinct periods: the Sleep Onset (SO) and the whole night. METHODS: Twenty ID patients (mean age: 43.5 ± 12.7; 7 M/13F) and 18 HCs (mean age: 41.6 ± 11.9; 8 M/10F) underwent a night of Polysomnography (PSG) and completed sleep diaries the following morning upon awakening. Two SSM indices, referring to the misperception of SL (SLm) and TST (TSTm), were calculated by comparing objective and subjective sleep indices extracted by PSG and sleep diary. According to these indices, the entire sample was split into 4 sub-groups: ID +SLm vs HC -SLm; ID +TSTm vs HC -TSTm. RESULTS: Considering the SO, the two-way mixed-design ANOVA showed a significant main effect of Groups pointing to a decreased delta/beta ratio in the whole scalp topography. Moreover, we found a significant interaction effect for the sigma and beta bands. Post Hoc tests showed higher sigma and beta power in anterior and temporo-parietal sites during the SO period in IDs +SLm compared to HC -SLm. Considering the whole night, the unpaired t-test revealed in IDs +TSTm significantly lower delta power during NREM, and lower delta/beta ratio index during NREM and REM sleep compared to HCs -TSTm. Finally, we found diffuse significant negative correlations between SSM indices and the delta/beta ratio during SO, NREM, and REM sleep. CONCLUSION: The main finding of the present study suggests that higher SL overestimation and TST underestimation are both phenomena related to diffuse cortical hyperarousal interpreted as a sleep state-independent electrophysiological correlate of the SSM, both during the SO and the whole night.


Assuntos
Ritmo Delta , Polissonografia , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono , Humanos , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/fisiopatologia , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Ritmo Delta/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Ritmo beta/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Sono/fisiologia , Latência do Sono/fisiologia
6.
J Clin Sleep Med ; 2024 Aug 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39172085

RESUMO

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Temporomandibular disorders (TMD) were linked to poor sleep on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), whereas polysomnography (PSG) revealed no major sleep disturbances, implying sleep state misperception (SSM). This study investigates SSM in TMD and control participants; correlates SSM with objective short sleep duration (SSD), depression symptoms, daytime sleepiness, and orofacial pain; and compares objective SSD between the groups. METHODS: General linear models were used to compare second-night PSG total sleep time (TST), sleep latency (SL), sleep efficiency (SE) and wake after sleep onset (WASO) with homologous PSQI-derived variables in 124 women with myofascial TMD and 46 age and BMI matched controls. PSQI variables were regressed onto objective SSD, depression symptoms, daytime sleepiness, and pain. Lastly, objective SSD was related to TMD presence. RESULTS: Compared to controls, TMD cases misperceived SE (p = 0.02); depression symptoms explained PSQI-derived SE (p = 0.002) and mediated the effect of pain (p <.001). PSQI variables were unrelated to respective PSG measures or objective SSD, except a significant subjective-objective correlation in SE among controls only (p = 0.002). Objective SSD was more frequent in TMD cases (p = 0.02, OR = 2.95), but it was unrelated to depression symptoms, daytime sleepiness or pre-PSG pain. CONCLUSIONS: The study demonstrates misperception of SE among TMD cases, which was accounted for by depression symptoms. Objective SSD nearly tripled in TMD cases; however, it was unrelated to PSQI variables, depression, daytime sleepiness, or pain, suggesting that SSM and objective SSD are two independent sleep features in TMD.

7.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 2024 Jul 17.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39016125

RESUMO

While the mechanisms underlying polarization are complex, scholars have consistently found a pervasive overestimation of perceptions of polarization to be a contributing factor. We argue that one mitigation strategy that can work at scale to address such misperceptions might be relatively straightforward: better data visualizations of cross-party attitudes on key issues. In a large-scale (N = 6603), international replication, we find that mode of presentation-or the manner in which data are visually presented-plays a significant role in moderating perceptions of polarization, even for longstanding, divisive issues for which partisans would likely hold strong prior beliefs. Additionally, we find the effects that different modes of presentation have on issue-specific polarization also extend to participant beliefs about overall interparty polarization, with certain modes proving capable of not only promoting less polarized views but also enabling more accurate estimates of the extent to which political groups agree. Finally, our findings also suggest that the manner in which intergroup data are visualized may also exert influence over the degree to which political groups are essentialized-a finding with implications for not only political perception but also for apolitical social psychological phenomena such as dehumanization.

8.
J Clin Med ; 13(13)2024 Jul 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38999538

RESUMO

Background/Objectives: Despite the availability of effective pharmacotherapy and evidence-based treatments, a substantial proportion of smokers do not seek treatment. This study aims to explore the cognitive distortions associated with not seeking evidence-based smoking cessation treatment and to identify cognitive barriers. Methods: The research conducted in Istanbul between October and December 2017 employs a cross-sectional design and includes two groups: a treatment-seeking group comprising 156 patients diagnosed with tobacco use disorder and a non-treatment seeking group of 78 patients with tobacco use disorder who had never sought professional help for smoking cessation. A comprehensive data collection process was used, including sociodemographic information, cognitive distortion assessment using the cognitive distortions scale, a smoking-related cognitive distortions interview and the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence. Results: While no significant sociodemographic differences were observed between the treatment-seeking and non-treatment-seeking groups, the study found that higher nicotine dependence was associated with a higher likelihood of seeking treatment. The treatment-seeking group displayed significantly higher levels of "all-or-nothing thinking" cognitive distortions related to smoking and smoking cessation. Conversely, the non-treatment-seeking group exhibited elevated levels of cognitive distortions such as "labeling", "mental filtering", "should statements" and "minimizing the positive" regarding receiving smoking cessation treatment. Conclusions: Understanding the cognitive distortions associated with treatment-seeking behavior for tobacco use disorder is crucial for developing targeted public-based interventions, public service announcements for tobacco use prevention and encouraging individuals to seek evidence-based treatment. Addressing these cognitive distortions can also potentially enhance the effectiveness of smoking cessation programs and reduce the global burden of tobacco-related diseases and mortality.

9.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38679960

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: This study examined associations between various cognitive domains and sleep discrepancy (self-reported vs objectively measured sleep), and evaluated interactive associations with insomnia status (non-insomnia vs insomnia). METHODS: Older adults (N = 65, Mage = 68.72, SD = 5.06, 43 insomnia/22 non-insomnia) aged 60+ reported subjective sleep (7 days of sleep diaries), objective sleep assessment (one-night polysomnography, PSG, via Sleep Profiler during the 7-day period), and completed cognitive tasks (National Institutes of Health Toolbox-Cognition Battery) measuring attention and processing speed, working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and episodic memory. The sleep diary variable corresponding to the same one night of PSG was used to calculate the sleep discrepancy (diary minus PSG parameter) variables for total sleep time (TST), sleep onset latency, wake after sleep onset, and sleep efficiency. Regression analyses determined independent and interactive (with insomnia status) associations between cognition and sleep discrepancy, controlling for age, sex, apnea-hypopnea index, and sleep medication usage. RESULTS: Working memory interacted with insomnia status in associations with sleep discrepancy related to TST and sleep efficiency. In those with insomnia, worse working memory was associated with shorter self-reported TST (p = .008) and lower sleep efficiency (p = .04) than PSG measured. DISCUSSION: In older adults with insomnia, worse working memory may be a contributing factor to sleep discrepancy. Future investigations of underlying neurophysiological factors and consideration of other objective sleep measures (actigraphy) are warranted. Prospective findings may help determine whether sleep discrepancy is a potential marker of future cognitive decline.


Assuntos
Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono , Humanos , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/epidemiologia , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/psicologia , Idoso , Masculino , Feminino , Polissonografia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Autorrelato , Cognição/fisiologia , População Branca/estatística & dados numéricos , Escolaridade , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Sono/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Brancos
10.
Cureus ; 16(3): e55436, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38567204

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Pareidolias, or visual misperceptions, are a non-motor symptom of Parkinson's disease (PD) with unclear pathophysiology. The noise pareidolia test (NPT) is a tool for screening pareidolias. The usefulness of the NPT in differentiating PD from atypical parkinsonian syndromes (APS) is also unknown. METHODS: We retrospectively investigated 74 patients with PD and 18 patients with APS who took the NPT. Correlations between the number of pareidolic responses, gray matter volume, and cerebral blood flow were also examined in the patients with PD. RESULTS: The median number of pareidolic responses in patients with PD and patients with APS was 0 (interquartile range (IQR): 0-3) and 0 (IQR: 0-1), respectively, and tended to be higher in patients with PD than in those with APS (p = 0.077). It was significantly higher in patients with PD who had hallucinations (2; IQR: 0-9) (p = 0.016). The area under the receiver operating characteristic curve for the number of pareidolic responses in the NPT was 0.62 when used to differentiate PD and APS, and the optimal cutoff number of pareidolic responses was 2/3. Sensitivity and specificity were 25.7% and 100%, respectively. In the PD group, the number of pareidolic responses was correlated with age (r = 0.27; p = 0.021) and the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) score (r = -0.34; p = 0.0099). Magnetic resonance imaging showed no significant correlation between the number of pareidolic responses and the volume of focal gray matter. On cerebral hypoperfusion mapping, the left parietal lobe had a significant correlation with the number of pareidolic responses (r = 0.35; p = 0.027). CONCLUSION: The number of pareidolic responses in NPT was suggested to be useful as a red flag to rule out APS in differentiating PD from APS. In PD without dementia, the number of pareidolic responses was associated with reduced blood flow in the left parietal lobe.

11.
Sleep Med ; 115: 21-29, 2024 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38325157

RESUMO

STUDY OBJECTIVE: To provide a comprehensive assessment of sleep state misperception in insomnia disorder (INS) and good sleepers (GS) by comparing recordings performed for one night in-lab (PSG and night review) and during several nights at-home (actigraphy and sleep diaries). METHODS: Fifty-seven INS and 29 GS wore an actigraphy device and filled a sleep diary for two weeks at-home. They subsequently completed a PSG recording and filled a night review in-lab. Sleep perception index (subjective/objective × 100) of sleep onset latency (SOL), sleep duration (TST) and wake duration (TST) were computed and compared between methods and groups. RESULTS: GS displayed a tendency to overestimate TST and WASO but correctly perceived SOL. The degree of misperception was similar across methods within the GS group. In contrast, INS underestimated their TST and overestimated their SOL both in-lab and at-home, yet the severity of misperception of SOL was larger at-home than in-lab. Finally, INS overestimated WASO only in-lab while correctly perceiving it at-home. While only the degree of TST misperception was stable across methods in INS, misperception of SOL and WASO were dependent on the method used. CONCLUSIONS: We found that GS and INS exhibit opposite patterns and severity of sleep misperception. While the degree of misperception in GS was similar across methods, only sleep duration misperception was reliably detected by both in-lab and at-home methods in INS. Our results highlight that, when assessing sleep misperception in insomnia disorder, the environment and method of data collection should be carefully considered.


Assuntos
Actigrafia , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono , Humanos , Polissonografia/métodos , Actigrafia/métodos , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/diagnóstico , Sono , Latência do Sono
12.
J Sleep Res ; 33(5): e14122, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38221712

RESUMO

The present study examined the relationship between subjective sleep onset latency (SOL), sleep structure, changes in skin and body temperature, and subjective evaluation of sleep in healthy young adults to elucidate the pathophysiological mechanisms of insomnia. A total of 28 participants (age 21.54 [0.50] years) with no sleep problems participated in a 1-h polysomnographic recording that obtained objective sleep parameters during the daytime while skin and body temperatures were recorded. The distal-proximal skin temperature gradient (DPG) was calculated. Subjective parameters, such as subjective SOL, sleep time, and restorative sleepiness, were evaluated before and after sleep. Most participants estimated their sleep latency as being longer than their actual SOL (13.7 versus 7.6 min). Objective SOL was significantly correlated with each sleep stage parameter whereas subjective SOL was negatively correlated with Stage N2 sleep duration (Rho = -0.454, p = 0.020), slow-wave activity and delta power (Rho = -0.500, p = 0.011 and Rho = -0.432, p = 0.031, respectively), and ΔDPG (the degree of reduction of heat loss before and after lights-off). Stepwise regression analysis showed that ΔDPG was the strongest predictive factor in explaining the length of subjective SOL. The degree of heat dissipation before and after lights-off contributed most to the sensation of falling asleep in healthy young adults. This finding may be helpful for elucidating the physiological mechanisms of insomnia and its treatment.


Assuntos
Polissonografia , Temperatura Cutânea , Latência do Sono , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto Jovem , Latência do Sono/fisiologia , Temperatura Cutânea/fisiologia , Fases do Sono/fisiologia , Temperatura Corporal/fisiologia , Regulação da Temperatura Corporal/fisiologia , Distúrbios do Início e da Manutenção do Sono/fisiopatologia , Sono/fisiologia , Adulto
13.
Brain Sci ; 14(1)2024 Jan 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38275517

RESUMO

Sleep state misperception (SSM) is a common issue in insomnia disorder (ID), causing a discrepancy between objective and subjective sleep/wake time estimation and increased daytime impairments. In this context, the hyperarousal theory assumes that sustained central nervous system activation contributes to the SSM. This study investigates factors influencing SSM during sleep latency (SL) and total sleep time (TST). Objective polysomnographic sleep variables (the alpha density index, latency-to-sleep stages and the first K-complex, and Rapid Eye Movement (REM) arousal density) and subjective sleep indices, taken from sleep diaries, were analyzed in 16 ID patients. Correlation analyses revealed a positive association between the degree of SL misperception (SLm) and the percentage of epochs that contained a visually scored stereotyped alpha rhythm during objective SL. A regression analysis showed that the REM arousal density and alpha density index significantly predicted TST misperception (TSTm). Furthermore, the degree of SLm was associated with an increased probability of transitioning from stage 1 of non-REM sleep to wakefulness during subjective SL. These findings support the role of hyperarousal in SSM and highlight the importance of alpha activity in unravelling the heterogeneous underpinnings of SSM.

14.
Child Obes ; 20(6): 434-441, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38241489

RESUMO

Background: Few longitudinal studies have investigated the role of weight-loss attempts or weight-related stress on body image during childhood. We examined whether weight-loss attempts and weight-related stress are associated with weight misperception and body dissatisfaction across childhood and adolescence. Methods: Data were drawn from the Quebec Adipose and Lifestyle InvesTigation in Youth (QUALITY) cohort of Canadian children with parental obesity (8-10 years: n = 630; 10-12 years: n = 564; 15-17 years: n = 377). We assessed weight-loss attempts and weight-related stress at baseline and first follow-up, and perceived and desired silhouettes at first and second follow-up with questionnaires. Weight misperception consisted of the difference in BMI z-score (zBMI) from the perceived silhouette and the measured zBMI. Body dissatisfaction consisted of the discordance between perceived and desired silhouettes. We estimated multivariable mixed-effects regression models adjusting for age, sex, pubertal stage, parental BMI and education, and sport-based teasing. Results: Weight loss attempts were associated with a higher weight misperception score (ever tried, beta [95% confidence intervals; CI]: 0.13 [0.01-0.24]) and with 2.13 times higher desire to be thinner (95% CI: 1.39-3.26) at the subsequent follow-up. Similarly, children stressed by their weight had a higher misperception score (beta [95% CI]: 0.15 [0.02-0.27]) and greater desire to be thinner at the next follow-up (odds ratio [95% CI]: 1.73 [0.999-3.00]). Conclusions: Weight-loss attempts and weight-related stress in children and adolescents are associated with weight misperception and body dissatisfaction, supporting empowerment and counseling focusing on healthy eating behaviors and a positive body image. Clinical Trial Registration Number: NCT03356262.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal , Obesidade Infantil , Estresse Psicológico , Redução de Peso , Humanos , Criança , Adolescente , Feminino , Masculino , Imagem Corporal/psicologia , Quebeque/epidemiologia , Estresse Psicológico/epidemiologia , Obesidade Infantil/psicologia , Obesidade Infantil/epidemiologia , Pais/psicologia , Índice de Massa Corporal , Estudos Longitudinais , Insatisfação Corporal/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários
15.
J Soc Psychol ; : 1-14, 2024 Jan 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38194434

RESUMO

In this study, we gather empirical evidence supporting the assumption that political trust is related to the ignorance of political performance problems, which ignorance, in turn, has a palliative function as it protects subjective well-being. We hypothesized that political trust has a stronger positive effect on the evaluation of political performance (and indirectly on subjective well-being) if actual performance is more questionable. These hypotheses were tested by multilevel structural equation modeling. Our multilevel moderated mediation models supporting this hypothesis were based on the 2018 European Social Survey dataset (N = 49,519) extended with county-level indices indicating actual performance in healthcare, education, and economy. The results also imply that the bigger the actual problem is, the stronger ignorance is driven by political trust.

16.
Trends Mol Med ; 30(4): 327-329, 2024 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37981534

RESUMO

In recent years, researchers have increasingly used the embodiment illusion paradigm in subclinical and clinical eating disorder (ED) populations. This has important implications for understanding and ultimately improving bodily misperception. Here, we provide a brief overview of the current 'state of the art' of these implications while highlighting challenges and future directions.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Alimentação e da Ingestão de Alimentos , Ilusões , Humanos , Imagem Corporal
17.
J Med Internet Res ; 25: e51336, 2023 12 13.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38090797

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Sleep disturbances are core symptoms of psychiatric disorders. Although various sleep measures have been developed to assess sleep patterns and quality of sleep, the concordance of these measures in patients with psychiatric disorders remains relatively elusive. OBJECTIVE: This study aims to examine the degree of agreement among 3 sleep recording methods and the consistency between subjective and objective sleep measures, with a specific focus on recently developed devices in a population of individuals with psychiatric disorders. METHODS: We analyzed 62 participants for this cross-sectional study, all having data for polysomnography (PSG), Zmachine, Fitbit, and sleep logs. Participants completed questionnaires on their symptoms and estimated sleep duration the morning after the overnight sleep assessment. The interclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) were calculated to evaluate the consistency between sleep parameters obtained from each instrument. Additionally, Bland-Altman plots were used to visually show differences and limits of agreement for sleep parameters measured by PSG, Zmachine, Fitbit, and sleep logs. RESULTS: The findings indicated a moderate agreement between PSG and Zmachine data for total sleep time (ICC=0.46; P<.001), wake after sleep onset (ICC=0.39; P=.002), and sleep efficiency (ICC=0.40; P=.006). In contrast, Fitbit demonstrated notable disagreement with PSG (total sleep time: ICC=0.08; wake after sleep onset: ICC=0.18; sleep efficiency: ICC=0.10) and exhibited particularly large discrepancies from the sleep logs (total sleep time: ICC=-0.01; wake after sleep onset: ICC=0.05; sleep efficiency: ICC=-0.02). Furthermore, subjective and objective concordance among PSG, Zmachine, and sleep logs appeared to be influenced by the severity of the depressive symptoms and obstructive sleep apnea, while these associations were not observed between the Fitbit and other sleep instruments. CONCLUSIONS: Our study results suggest that Fitbit accuracy is reduced in the presence of comorbid clinical symptoms. Although user-friendly, Fitbit has limitations that should be considered when assessing sleep in patients with psychiatric disorders.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Sono-Vigília , Sono , Humanos , Polissonografia/métodos , Estudos Transversais , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Transtornos do Sono-Vigília/diagnóstico , Eletroencefalografia , Actigrafia/métodos
18.
Cureus ; 15(10): e48074, 2023 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38046502

RESUMO

The most common and challenging chief complaint in the emergency department is abdominal pain. Intussusception, although rare in adults, is an important etiology to consider. The diagnosis is often delayed because of the nonspecific symptoms, especially in adults. This case highlights a rare case of intussusception in a middle-aged male with a colonic lipoma as a leading point. Endo-loop was applied to the colonic lipoma, leading to the resolution of intussusception. Therefore, this can be an effective alternative to surgery in select cases.

19.
BMC Public Health ; 23(1): 2550, 2023 12 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38129799

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Unhealthy snacking behaviors and body weight misperception are both significant concerns in adolescent health. Weight misperception are common among youth and may influence their motivation to engage in health-related behaviors, however, the effect on snacking patterns choice remains unclear. Our study aimed to examine the relationship between body weight misperception and snacking pattern choice among school adolescents. METHODS: A cross-sectional study was conducted using an online survey platform. Body weight misperception was defined based on perceived body weight and true weight. Snack intake was measured using a qualitative food-frequency questionnaire. Factor analysis was used to identify snacking patterns, and multiple linear regression was employed to examine the association between body weight misperception and snacking patterns. RESULTS: 190,296 students with the average age of 13.3 ± 1.0 years was included, and 44.5% of students misperceived their weight. Overestimation was more prevalent than underestimation. Two snacking patterns, namely a high-calorie snacking pattern and a healthy snacking pattern, were identified with eigenvalues > 1. Weight underestimation was positively linked to high-calorie snacking pattern scores for both normal weight students (ß: 0.16, 95% CI: 0.11, 0.21) and students with overweight/obesity (ß: 0.44, 95% CI: 0.35, 0.52), and to healthy snacking scores for students with overweight/obesity (ß: 0.28, 95% CI: 0.22, 0.33), but negatively linked to healthy snacking pattern scores for normal weight students (ß: -0.12, 95% CI: -0.15, -0.09). Conversely, weight overestimation was negatively linked to both high-calorie and healthy snacking pattern scores for normal weight students (ß: -0.07, 95% CI: -0.11, -0.04 and ß: -0.13, 95% CI: -0.15, -0.10), but positively linked to healthy snacking scores for underweight students (ß: 0.15, 95% CI: 0.08, 0.21). Interactions were found between sex, grade, accommodation, only child, primary guardians, parental education level and weight misperception to snacking patterns. CONCLUSIONS: Adolescents with normal weight and overweight/obesity who misperceived their weight exhibited less healthy snacking patterns, whereas underweight students who misperceived their weight displayed healthier snacking patterns. Comprehensive programs are crucial to educate and guide adolescents in understanding their weight status and making healthier snack choices, involving families, schools, and society.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal , Comportamento Alimentar , Obesidade , Sobrepeso , Magreza , Adolescente , Criança , Humanos , Índice de Massa Corporal , Peso Corporal , Estudos Transversais , Obesidade/epidemiologia , Sobrepeso/epidemiologia , Lanches
20.
Curr Opin Psychol ; 54: 101717, 2023 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37972526

RESUMO

Research on political misperceptions is flourishing across disciplines. Literature on misinformation susceptibility and political group meta-perceptions have arisen independently, both seeking to understand how inaccurate social beliefs of the first and second order respectively contribute to political polarization. Here we review these literatures and argue for greater integration. We highlight four domains where these two literatures intersect: how inaccurate group meta-perceptions may increase misinformation susceptibility, how misinformation may itself convey inaccurate second-order information, how second-order perceptions of misinformation belief may increase misinformation susceptibility, and how reputational concerns may affect misinformation engagement. Our hope is to illuminate fruitful avenues of future research and inspire scholars of political misperceptions to pursue unified theoretical models of how misperceptions drive negative political outcomes.


Assuntos
Desinformação , Percepção , Política , Humanos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA