Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 39
Filter
1.
Neuroimage ; 59(1): 556-64, 2012 Jan 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21839178

ABSTRACT

Animal and human studies have shown that the parietal and the ventral premotor cortices constitute the neural substrate of the so-called mirror system. The word "mirror" originally referred to the discovery of neurons in non-human primates whose visual response echoes their motor response. This account proposes that action understanding and imitation depend on a mechanism which activates directly our own motor system as we observe the actions of other agents (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). Single unit recording experiments have also demonstrated that parietal neurons have predictive activity and discharge well ahead of a planned movement. Interestingly, patients with parietal damage can show impairments in their ability to imitate or understand an observed action, but they have also difficulties in monitoring early phases of their own movement planning, be it simple reaching movements or more complex object-directed actions. The fact that both deficits may co-occur after a parietal lesion raises the question whether this reflects the impairment of a common mechanism. To address this question we examined EEG activity in patients with selective lesions in the inferior parietal lobe (N=6) who were requested to watch passively a video showing an actor grasping a colored object. The object's color cued the subject that the actor was about to move. We recorded the Readiness Potential (RP), a marker of motor preparation which also arises when preparing to observe an action (Kilner et al., 2004). Parietal patients' performance was compared to that of neurologically normal subjects (n=9) and patients with a ventral premotor cortex lesion (N=4). We show that neurologically normal subjects and premotor patients exhibit a significant RP prior to the observed action, whereas no such RP is observed in parietal patients. Our results indicate that parietal cortex injury alters the ability to monitor the early planning phases not only of one's own actions but those of other agents as well. We speculate that parietal activity during action observation does not only or essentially reflect a mirroring process, as recently proposed by mirror neurons' account, but involve instead an anticipatory process which arises through prior learning and predictive mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Contingent Negative Variation/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Aged , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Mirror Neurons/physiology , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted
2.
Psychol Med ; 40(9): 1531-9, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19917144

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) overestimate their size despite being severely underweight. Whether this misperception echoes an underlying emotional disturbance or also reflects a genuine body-representation deficit is debatable. Current measures inquire directly about subjective perception of body image, thus distinguishing poorly between top-down effects of emotions/attitudes towards the body and disturbances due to proprioceptive disorders/distorted body schema. Disorders of body representation also emerge following damage to the right parietal lobe. The possibility that parietal dysfunction might contribute to AN is suspected, based on the demonstrated association of spatial impairments, comparable to those found after parietal lesion, with this syndrome. METHOD: We used a behavioral task to compare body knowledge in severe anorexics (n=8), healthy volunteers (n=11) and stroke patients with focal damage to the left/right parietal lobe (n=4). We applied a psychophysical procedure based on the perception, in the dark, of an approaching visual stimulus that was turned off before reaching the observer. Participants had to predict whether the stimulus would have hit/missed their body, had it continued its linear motion. RESULTS: Healthy volunteers and left parietal patients estimated body boundaries very close to the real ones. Conversely, anorexics and right parietal patients underestimated eccentricity of their left body boundary. CONCLUSIONS: These findings are in line with the role the parietal cortex plays in developing and maintaining body representation, and support the possibility for a neuropsychological component in the pathogenesis of anorexia, offering alternative approaches to treatment of the disorder.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/pathology , Body Dysmorphic Disorders/pathology , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Adult , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Basal Ganglia/pathology , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Somatosensory Cortex/pathology
3.
Science ; 273(5281): 1564-8, 1996 Sep 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8703221

ABSTRACT

Recent neuroimagery findings showed that the patterns of cerebral activation during the mental rehearsal of a motor act are similar to those produced by its actual execution. This concurs with the notion that part of the distributed neural activity taking place during movement involves internal simulations, but it is not yet clear what specific contribution the different brain areas involved bring to this process. Here, patients with lesions restricted to the parietal cortex were found to be impaired selectively at predicting, through mental imagery, the time necessary to perform differentiated finger movements and visually guided pointing gestures, in comparison to normal individuals and to a patient with damage to the primary motor area. These results suggest that the parietal cortex is important for the ability to generate mental movement representations.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Movement/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Aged , Apraxias/physiopathology , Fingers/physiology , Humans , Middle Aged , Motor Cortex/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance
4.
Cereb Cortex ; 18(2): 272-7, 2008 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17517681

ABSTRACT

Several studies have shown a cortico-spinal facilitation during motor imagery. This facilitation effect is weaker when the actual hand posture is incompatible with the imagined movement. To determine whether the source of this interference effect arises from online proprioceptive information, we examined transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)-induced motor-evoked potentials during motor imagery in the deafferented subject G.L. The patient and 7 control subjects were asked to close their eyes and imagine joining the tips of the thumb and the little finger while maintaining a hand posture compatible or incompatible with the imagined movement. Contrary to control subjects' performance, G.L.'s results show that the facilitation observed during motor imagery was independent of the hand posture. To examine how vision of the hand interacts with the imagery process, G.L. and control subjects performed the same task with the eyes open. Like control subjects, when G.L. looked at her hand, a greater facilitation was observed when her hand posture was compatible with the imagined movement than when it was incompatible. These results suggest that in the absence of proprioception, vision may facilitate or inhibit motor representations and support the idea that limb position in the brain is organized around multisensory representations.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Hand/physiology , Imagination/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Spinal Cord/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Orientation/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis
5.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 629: 559-75, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19227521

ABSTRACT

Individuated finger movements of the human hand require selective activation of particular sets of muscles. Such selective activation is controlled primarily by the motor cortex via the corticospinal tract. Is this selectivity therefore lost when lesions damage the corticospinal tract? Or when the motor cortex reorganizes after amputation? We studied finger movements in normal human subjects and in patients who had recovered substantially from pure motor hemiparesis caused by lacunar strokes, which damage the corticospinal tract without affecting other pathways. Even after substantial recovery from these strokes, individuation of finger movements remained reduced-both for flexion/extension and for adduction/ abduction motion of the fingers. Stroke subjects regained the ability to move the instructed digit through a normal range, but unintentional motion of other digits was increased. This increase did not result from a change in the passive biomechanical coupling of the fingers. Rather, voluntary contractions of muscles that move the intended digit were accompanied by inappropriate contractions in muscles acting on additional digits. These observations suggest that the normal corticospinal system produces individuated finger movements not only by selectively activating certain muscles, but also by suppressing activation of other muscles during voluntary effort to move a given digit. In a separate experiment, reversible amputation of the hand was produced in normal subjects by ischemic nerve block at the wrist. Motor output to the intrinsic muscles and sensory input both become blocked under these conditions, effectively amputating the hand from the nervous system. But the long extrinsic muscles that flex and extend the digits remain normally innervated, and thus flexion forces still can be generated at the fingertips. During reversible amputation of the hand produced by ischemic nerve block, the ability of subjects to activate subdivisions of extrinsic muscles and to exert flexion force at individual fingertips continued to show essentially normal selectivity. Voluntary activation of the remaining muscles thus continues to be selective after amputation, in spite of both the loss of sensory input from the amputated hand, and reorganization within the primary motor cortex. During cortical reorganization after amputation, then, voluntary patterns of motor output intended for finger muscles may not be lost. We therefore examined activity in the stump muscles of above-elbow amputees, who have no remaining hand muscles. Different movements of the phantom hand were accompanied by different patterns of EMG in remaining proximal muscles, distinct from the EMG patterns associated with movement of the phantom elbow. We infer that voluntary motor output patterns that normally control finger movements after amputation may become diverted to remaining proximal muscles.


Subject(s)
Amputation, Traumatic/physiopathology , Fingers/physiopathology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiopathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Electromyography , Humans , Motor Cortex/physiopathology , Movement/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Nerve Block , Pyramidal Tracts/physiopathology
6.
Commun Agric Appl Biol Sci ; 74(3): 809-19, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20222567

ABSTRACT

In Sardinia (Italy) Tomato yellow leaf curl disease (TYLCD) re-emerged in 2003, after a period of decline, producing severe yield losses in protected tomato crops. This epidemic outbreak highlighted the inadequacy of the approach to TYLCD management based chiefly on the chemical control of its vector, the whitefly Bemisia tabaci, and the use of 40-mesh nets for greenhouse screening. To evaluate the reliability of alternative practices for implementation within IP&DM programmes, in 2007 and 2008 we carried out two field experiments on greenhouse tomato crops planted in summer. In both trials we tested the use of UV-reflective mulches (UVRM) or floating row covers (NWRC), against an uncovered control on clear mulch. Furthermore, we assessed the effectiveness of acibenzolar-S-methyl, a SAR elicitor, as subplot factor in a split plot design. In 2007 because of the low incidence of the disease it was not possible to assess differences among the treatments. However a significant increase in plant growth, and production was observed in UVRM plots. By contrast, in 2008 the study crop was seriously affected by TYLCD. In the plots not treated with acibenzolar-S-methyl, the disease progression was initially slowed down in NWRC and, to a lesser extent, in UVRM plots compared to control plots but, during the second month of the cropping period, the differences among treatments became statistically not significant. In the plots treated with acibenzolar-S-methyl the dynamics of TYLCD infection during the first five weeks exhibited the same trend as in the untreated plots. Thereafter, the combination of UVRM and, to a lesser extent, of NWRC with the SAR elicitor resulted in a reduction of disease incidence with respect to the control. In conclusion, all the control measures tested in the present work showed the potential for TYLCD management in greenhouse tomatoes. However, the most promising results were achieved using UV-reflective mulch and non-woven row cover in combination with acibenzolar-S-methyl. In view of its positive impact on plant growth and yield in summer planted tomato crops, as well as on TYLCD infection, the use of UV-reflective mulch warrants further investigation.


Subject(s)
Imidazoles/pharmacology , Insect Control/methods , Insecticides/pharmacology , Nitro Compounds/pharmacology , Plant Diseases/prevention & control , Crops, Agricultural/growth & development , Italy , Solanum lycopersicum/drug effects , Solanum lycopersicum/growth & development , Solanum lycopersicum/radiation effects , Neonicotinoids , Plant Diseases/statistics & numerical data , Soil , Temperature , Ultraviolet Rays
7.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 162(6-7): 703-12, 2006 Jun.
Article in French | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16840978

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: What are the nature and the neural substrate of voluntary force perception? STATE OF ART: Experimental findings demonstrate that efferent signals related to motor command play a dominant role in perceiving voluntary muscular force. This suggests that voluntary force perception is provided through a sense of effort and not through a sense of intramuscular tension. Nevertheless, experimental data show that the contribution of sensory input to effort awareness must not be dismissed. Sensory signals are not involved in generating a signal of effort but rather in calibrating and modulating its magnitude. Neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies revealed that many cortical structures are activated during tasks of voluntary muscular force perception. PERSPECTIVES AND CONCLUSION: In such tasks, the basal ganglia might support the coherence of cortical activity.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Physical Exertion , Sensation/physiology , Basal Ganglia/physiology , Humans , Motor Cortex/physiology , Muscle Fatigue/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Neurons, Efferent/physiology , Perception/physiology
8.
Neurochirurgie ; 61(2-3): 101-5, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24975204

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The cerebellum is a fundamental structure of the central nervous system. However, in humans, its anatomo-functional organization and the processes through which this organization adapts in response to injuries remain widely unknown. METHODS: Motor and somatosensory evoked potentials were used to map functional representations in the posterior cerebellum of patients with extra- and intracellebellar injuries. Extracerebellar patients had injuries outside the cerebellum (e.g. pineal region, quadrigeminal plate) while intracerebellar patients had injuries within the cerebellum. Data were collected in 20 extracerebellar patients for motor representations. Only preliminary data were gathered for somatosensory representations and intracerebellar patients. RESULTS: In extracerebellar patients, electrical stimulation induced muscle contractions in the ipsilateral hemibody. These representations were somatotopically organized with large overlaps between the face and upper limb in the superior posterior cerebellum and the upper and lower limb in the inferior posterior cerebellum. Neck muscles were represented in the oculomotor vermis. In intracerebellar patients, preliminary data seem to indicate that motor plasticity is achieved by recruiting the contralesional (healthy) cerebellar hemisphere. CONCLUSIONS: Although still ongoing, this project could eventually lead to an improvement of the surgical treatment of patients with lesions of the posterior fossa, by improving our knowledge of cerebellar organization and the process of post-lesional plasticity.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebellum/surgery , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Pineal Gland/surgery , Sensorimotor Cortex/surgery , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Child , Electric Stimulation/methods , Humans , Middle Aged , Young Adult
9.
Neurology ; 45(2): 387-9, 1995 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7854546

ABSTRACT

Autobiographic memory is reported to be impaired in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). To determine if cholinergic blockade fully reproduces the amnestic disorder found in dementia, we evaluated aspects of autobiographic and episodic memory in six healthy elderly controls after both scopolamine and placebo administration compared with untreated age- and education-matched patients with AD. The performance of patients with AD was significantly worse than that of controls after both treatment conditions. Scopolamine impaired episodic but not autobiographic memory. Thus, even though the cholinergic system is severely affected in patients with AD, muscarinic blockade alone does not seem to be a good model of this disorder.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Memory/drug effects , Muscarinic Antagonists , Scopolamine/pharmacology , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Middle Aged , Placebos , Reference Values , Time Factors
10.
Neurology ; 45(8): 1477-83, 1995 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7644044

ABSTRACT

The pattern of cortical and subcortical neuropathologic lesions in corticobasal degeneration (CBD) should predict a specific cognitive profile in this disease. To characterize this profile and to determine its specificity by comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) and senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type (SDAT), we used an extensive neuropsychological battery assessing global efficiency, executive functions, various tests of encoding and retrieval, dynamic motor organization, and upper limb praxis. We compared the performance of patients with CBD (n = 15) with that of controls (n = 19) matched for age and education, and with that of patients with PSP and SDAT (15 in each group), matched for severity of dementia and depression. Patients with CBD showed: (1) a moderate global deterioration; (2) a dysexecutive syndrome similar to that of patients with PSP and more severe than in SDAT; (3) explicit learning deficits, without retention difficulties and easily compensated by using the same semantic cues at encoding and retrieval as in PSP; this was in contrast with SDAT where cued recall and recognition were also impaired; (4) disorders of dynamic motor execution (temporal organization, bimanual coordination, control, and inhibition) similar to those of patients with PSP and not in SDAT; (5) asymmetric praxis disorders (posture imitation, symbolic gesture execution, and object utilization) that were not observed in PSP or SDAT. Patients with CBD show a specific neuropsychological pattern associating a dysexecutive syndrome, likely due to degeneration of the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex, and asymmetric praxis disorders, which might be related to premotor and parietal lobe lesions. This neuropsychological profile may help to distinguish this condition clinically from other neurodegenerative diseases.


Subject(s)
Basal Ganglia , Brain Diseases/pathology , Cerebral Cortex , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Basal Ganglia/pathology , Brain Diseases/physiopathology , Brain Diseases/psychology , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Functional Laterality , Gestures , Humans , Middle Aged , Motor Activity , Neuropsychological Tests , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/pathology , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/physiopathology , Supranuclear Palsy, Progressive/psychology
11.
Neurology ; 42(8): 1493-6, 1992 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1641142

ABSTRACT

We compared the performance of 12 patients with cerebellar atrophy (CA) and 12 normal controls matched for age and education on the Tower of Hanoi, a nine-problem task that requires cognitive planning. CA patients performed significantly worse than controls on this task despite no difference in planning and between-move pause times. A reanalysis of the data using just the subgroup of patients with pure cerebellar cortical atrophy (CCA) (N = 9) replicated the above results and also showed that CCA patients had significantly increased planning times compared with controls. Neither age, sex, education level, severity of dementia, word fluency, response time, memory, nor visuomotor procedural learning predicted CA or CCA performance. This deficit in cognitive planning suggests a functional link between the cerebellum, basal ganglia, and the frontal lobe concerning specific cognitive processes. However, the exact role of the cerebellum in cognitive planning remains undetermined.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Diseases/psychology , Cognition , Adult , Atrophy , Cerebellar Cortex/pathology , Cerebellar Diseases/pathology , Female , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 39(8): 759-70, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11369400

ABSTRACT

Patients with frontal lobe lesions are known to encounter severe problems in the organisation of their behaviour in everyday life. Script generation tasks assess the subject's conceptual ability to formulate and evaluate a coherent and structured plan of action. In the present study, we investigated to what extent neuropsychological deficits observed at the conceptual level of action knowledge lead to impairments in action execution. We examined seven patients with prefrontal cortex damage and sixteen normal subjects. Subjects were first asked to verbally formulate a plan of action and then to use this knowledge for 'executing' the actions in a virtual 3-dimensional interactive apartment presented on a computer screen. The results indicated that the presence of the realistic context improved patients' performance. However, specific impairments were observed in patients in the execution condition, namely actions slips, omissions, failure in initiating actions and purposeless displacements. Moreover, an analysis of planning time showed that, differently of the patients group, normal subjects spent more time during plan execution as compared to plan generation. These results suggest that after a frontal lobe lesion a defective formulation of a routine plan might affect the execution of the corresponding course of actions.


Subject(s)
Prefrontal Cortex/injuries , Reading , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 37(13): 1469-76, 1999 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10617267

ABSTRACT

Neuropsychological studies have shown that the prefrontal cortex is important in planning and monitoring everyday behaviour. In this study, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we investigated whether specific prefrontal regions are involved in processing a sequence of actions. Subjects were required to perform two different tasks: Script-event order and Sentence-word order. Script sequence and word sequence processing were found to activate partially overlapping areas which are known to be implicated in language processing. In addition, the Script-task activated a large area in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 6 and 8, BA 6 and 8), in both the left and right hemispheres, as well as the left supplementary motor area and left angular gyrus (BA 39). Our results suggest that these prefrontal areas may be more specifically involved in the process of analysing sequential links in the action domain.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Reading , Semantics , Adult , Brain Mapping , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Problem Solving/physiology
14.
Neuroreport ; 6(7): 997-1001, 1995 May 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7632907

ABSTRACT

The chronometry of imagined and actual movements was investigated in a patient with a unilateral lesion of the motor cortex. Motor imagery generated highly accurate estimates of motor performance in a variety of situations, reflecting the hypokinesia of the contralesional hand. There were parallel increases in mental and actual movement times from proximal to distal limb segments. Bimanual movements adopted the slower speed of the impaired hand in both conditions. Imagined motor sequences to the beat of a metronome predicted the maximum speed reached in actual performance. Finally, visually guided pointing showed the same target-size effects in the imagery and movement conditions. The results are in agreement with the hypothesis that common cerebral motor representations are activated when imaging and planning voluntary movements.


Subject(s)
Brain Diseases/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Hand/physiology , Imagination/physiology , Motor Cortex , Movement Disorders/physiopathology , Movement/physiology , Aged , Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Brain Diseases/pathology , Brain Diseases/psychology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Middle Aged , Movement Disorders/pathology , Movement Disorders/psychology , Tomography, Emission-Computed
15.
Cortex ; 32(1): 83-95, 1996 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8697754

ABSTRACT

We report the case of a patient (T.R.) who developed a severe and selective amnesia for names and dates associated with events. His amnesia was temporally limited, affecting only the last two to three decades of his life. When recalling an event he was able to evoke both its content and place, while he could not provide any information about people (names or their physical features) and the time (date/period) of its occurrence. His performance on event-memory tests was consistent across the type of material used (personal and public events) or the period of life investigated. These results suggest that knowledge of an episode is specified across multiple representations.


Subject(s)
Amnesia/physiopathology , Memory/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
16.
Cortex ; 23(1): 73-85, 1987 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3568707

ABSTRACT

Four patients showing the syndrome of "topographical disorientation" are reported. Patients became unable to find their way, especially in unfamiliar surroundings, following a single lesion in the territory of the right posterior cerebral artery, as evidenced on CT-scan. Associated disturbances included: left hemianopia, mild face recognition problems, and various degree of impairment in face-learning and visual maze-learning tasks. Language, visuo-perceptive and constructional abilities, object and picture recognition were intact. Memory tests only showed a mild, generally non-significant, impairment of visual memory. As inferred from the lesion located in the 4 patients, this syndrome seems to be strongly related to damage to the right parahippocampal gyrus, a structure that thus appears crucial for specifically storing and/or retrieving visual information necessary to achieve orientation in the locomotor environment.


Subject(s)
Brain Damage, Chronic/psychology , Orientation , Social Environment , Adult , Brain/pathology , Brain Damage, Chronic/pathology , Brain Mapping , Cerebrovascular Disorders/psychology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Thalamic Diseases/psychology , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
17.
Cortex ; 36(2): 163-79, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10815704

ABSTRACT

Several studies have pointed out that basal ganglia are involved in adaptive control of action at both motor and cognitive level. This study aimed to investigate how basal ganglia retrieve and manage script event knowledge required in planning behavior. Script event knowledge was investigated in patients with Parkinson's Disease using three kinds of activity that differed in familiarity. Unlike patients with prefrontal lesions, patients with Parkinson's Disease were able to order events in a typical sequence and obeyed the boundaries and hierarchies between events. In contrast, patients with Parkinson's Disease were impaired in evaluating how important each script event was within the context of goal-oriented planning activity. Our findings indicate that the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia are differentially involved in planning. The role of the basal ganglia might consist in providing a feedback about the goodness of each action while building up meaningful sequences of events during learning.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Knowledge , Mental Recall , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychological Tests , Reference Values
18.
Cortex ; 34(5): 771-8, 1998 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9872379

ABSTRACT

Time is a fundamental dimension of cognition. It is expressed in the sequential ordering of individual elements in a wide variety of activities such as language, motor control or in the broader domain of long range goal-directed actions. Several studies have shown the importance of the frontal lobes in sequencing information. The question addressed in this study is whether this brain region hosts a single supramodal sequence processor, or whether separate mechanisms are required for different kinds of temporally organised knowledge structures such as syntax and action knowledge. Here we show that so-called agrammatic patients, with lesions in Broca's area, ordered word groups correctly to form a logical sequence of actions but they were severely impaired when similar word groups had to be ordered as a syntactically well-formed sentence. The opposite performance was observed in patients with dorsolateral prefrontal lesions, that is, while their syntactic processing was intact at the sentence level, they demonstrated a pronounced deficit in producing temporally coherent sequences of actions. Anatomical reconstruction of lesions from brain scans revealed that the sentence and action grammar deficits involved distinct, non-overlapping sites within the frontal lobes. Finally, in a third group of patients whose lesions encompassed both Broca's area and the prefrontal cortex, the two types of deficits were found. We conclude that sequence processing is specific to knowledge domains and involves different networks within the frontal lobes.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiology , Language Disorders/physiopathology , Analysis of Variance , Aphasia, Broca/etiology , Aphasia, Broca/physiopathology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/complications , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Humans , Language Disorders/etiology , Language Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Task Performance and Analysis , Word Association Tests
19.
Cortex ; 34(4): 621-7, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9800095

ABSTRACT

The purpose of the present study was to assess how the striato-frontal system contributes to the manipulation of goal-directed actions. We studied a group of ten patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) in order to investigate which aspects of action knowledge processing are impaired and to define the conditions under which the deficits may occur. PD patients committed errors of sequence and inserted distractors in tasks that required them to order pre-determined events belonging to a given script in a typical sequence. Rather than attributing errors of event sequencing to a deficit of script "syntax" knowledge, we suggest that the difficulties manifested by PD patients were due to an impairment of a switching mechanism that is necessary for processing information in parallel.


Subject(s)
Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Cognition/physiology , Corpus Striatum/physiopathology , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Higher Nervous Activity/physiology , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time/physiology
20.
Cortex ; 31(2): 301-16, 1995 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7555008

ABSTRACT

Script generation was investigated in patients with lesions in the prefrontal (n = 9) and posterior (n = 8) cortical regions and in normal subjects (n = 16). Three different activities ranging in degree of familiarity were studied. Frontal patients did not differ from patients with posterior lesion and Normal subjects in the number of actions evoked, mean evocation time, or centrality. Impairments in script information processing were observed only in patients with prefrontal lesions, and for the three types of scripts. Specifically these patients made errors in ordering actions in the correct temporal sequence, failed to close scripts and to remain within the stated boundaries, and made deviant estimates of action importance. The results suggest that pre-frontal cortical lesions provoke a selective impairment in managerial knowledge (Grafman, 1989) that may contribute to difficulties in the formulation and execution of plans.


Subject(s)
Brain Damage, Chronic/physiopathology , Brain Damage, Chronic/psychology , Decision Making , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Reaction Time , Task Performance and Analysis
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL