Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 135
Filter
Add more filters

Publication year range
1.
Neuroimage ; 294: 120645, 2024 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38734156

ABSTRACT

Aggressive adolescents tend to exhibit abnormal fear acquisition and extinction, and reactive aggressive adolescents are often more anxious. However, the relationship between fear generalization and reactive aggression (RA) remains unknown. According to Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ) scores, 61 adolescents were divided into two groups, namely, a high RA group (N = 30) and a low aggression (LA) group (N = 31). All participants underwent three consecutive phases of the Pavlovian conditioning paradigm (i.e., habituation, acquisition, and generalization), and neural activation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) was assessed by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). The stimuli were ten circles with varying sizes, including two conditioned stimuli (CSs) and eight generalization stimuli (GSs). A scream at 85 dB served as the auditory unconditioned stimulus (US). The US expectancy ratings of both CSs and GSs were higher in the RA group than in the LA group. The fNIRS results showed that CSs and GSs evoked lower mPFC activation in the RA group compared to the LA group during fear generalization. These findings suggest that abnormalities in fear acquisition and generalization are prototypical dysregulations in adolescents with RA. They provide neurocognitive evidence for dysregulated fear learning in the mechanisms underlying adolescents with RA, highlighting the need to develop emotional regulation interventions for these individuals.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Generalization, Psychological , Prefrontal Cortex , Spectroscopy, Near-Infrared , Humans , Adolescent , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Fear/physiology , Male , Female , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Aggression/physiology
2.
J Neurochem ; 168(9): 2587-2600, 2024 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38705582

ABSTRACT

Fear overgeneralization is widely accepted as a pathogenic marker of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Recently, GABAergic interneurons have been regarded as key players in the regulation of fear memory. The role of hippocampal GABAergic interneurons in contextual fear generalization of PTSD remains incompletely understood. In the present study, we established a rat model of PTSD with inescapable foot shocks (IFS) and observed the loss of GABAergic interneuron phenotype in the hippocampal cornu ammonis-1 (CA1) subfield. To determine whether the loss of GABAergic interneuron phenotype was associated with fear generalization in PTSD rats, we used adeno-associated virus (AAV) to reduce the expression of GAD67 in CA1 and observed its effect on fear generalization. The results showed that the reduction of GAD67 in CA1 enhanced contextual fear generalization in rats. We investigated whether the PERK pathway was involved in the GABAergic interneuron injury. Increased expression of p-PERK, CHOP, and Caspase12 in GABAergic interneurons of PTSD rats was observed. Then, we used salubrinal, an endoplasmic reticulum stress inhibitor, to modulate the PERK pathway. The salubrinal treatment significantly protected the GABAergic interneurons and relieved fear generalization in PTSD rats. In addition, the results showed that salubrinal down-regulated the expression of CHOP and Caspase12 in GABAergic interneurons of PTSD rats. In conclusion, this study provided evidence that the loss of GABAergic interneuron phenotype in CA1 may contribute to contextual fear generalization in PTSD. The PERK pathway is involved in the GABAergic interneuron injury of PTSD rats and modulating it can protect GABAergic interneurons and constrain contextual fear generalization.


Subject(s)
CA1 Region, Hippocampal , Fear , GABAergic Neurons , Interneurons , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Animals , Rats , Interneurons/metabolism , Fear/physiology , Fear/psychology , Male , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/metabolism , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , CA1 Region, Hippocampal/metabolism , GABAergic Neurons/metabolism , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Glutamate Decarboxylase/metabolism
3.
Biochem Biophys Res Commun ; 722: 150074, 2024 08 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38805785

ABSTRACT

Genetic knockout and pharmaceutical inhibition of the NLRP3 inflammasome enhances the extinction of contextual fear memory, which is attributed to its role in neuronal and synaptic dysregulation, concurrent with neurotransmitter function disturbances. This study aimed to determine whether NLRP3 plays a role in generalizing fear via the inflammatory axis. We established the NLRP3 KO mice model, followed by behavioral and biochemical analyses. The NLRP3 KO mice displayed impaired fear generalization, lower neuroinflammation levels, and dysregulated neurotransmitter function. Additionally, α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptors, but not the inhibition of NMDA or 5-HT2C receptors, resulted in fear generalization in NLRP3 KO mice because TAT-GluA2 3Y, but not SB242084 and D-cycloserine, treated blocked NLRP3 deprivation effects on fear generalization. Thus, global knockout of NLRP3 is associated with aberrant fear generalization, possibly through AMPA receptor signaling.


Subject(s)
Fear , NLR Family, Pyrin Domain-Containing 3 Protein , Receptors, AMPA , Animals , Male , Mice , Fear/physiology , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Mice, Knockout , NLR Family, Pyrin Domain-Containing 3 Protein/metabolism , NLR Family, Pyrin Domain-Containing 3 Protein/genetics , NLR Family, Pyrin Domain-Containing 3 Protein/deficiency , Receptors, AMPA/metabolism , Receptors, AMPA/genetics
4.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 213: 107960, 2024 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39004160

ABSTRACT

Labilization-reconsolidation, which relies on retrieval, has been considered an opportunity to attenuate the negative aspects of traumatic memories. A therapeutic strategy based on reconsolidation blockade is deemed more effective than current therapies relying on memory extinction. Nevertheless, extremely stressful memories frequently prove resistant to this process. Here, after inducing robust fear memory in mice through strong fear conditioning, we examined the possibility of rendering it susceptible to pharmacological modulation based on the degree of generalized fear (GF). To achieve this, we established an ordered gradient of GF, determined by the perceptual similarity between the associated context (CA) and non-associated contexts (CB, CC, CD, and CE) to the aversive event. We observed that as the exposure context became less similar to CA, the defensive pattern shifted from passive to active behaviors in both male and female mice. Subsequently, in conditioned animals, we administered propranolol after exposure to the different contexts (CA, CB, CC, CD or CE). In males, propranolol treatment resulted in reduced freezing time and enhanced risk assessment behaviors when administered following exposure to CA or CB, but not after CC, CD, or CE, compared to the control group. In females, a similar change in behavioral pattern was observed with propranolol administered after exposure to CC, but not after the other contexts. These results highlight the possibility of indirectly manipulating a robust contextual fear memory by controlling the level of generalization during recall. Additionally, it was demonstrated that the effect of propranolol on reconsolidation would not lead to a reduction in fear memory per se, but rather to its reorganization resulting in greater behavioral flexibility (from passive to active behaviors). Finally, from a clinical viewpoint, this would be of considerable relevance since following this strategy could make the treatment of psychiatric disorders associated with traumatic memory formation more effective and less stressful.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Propranolol , Fear/drug effects , Fear/physiology , Animals , Male , Propranolol/pharmacology , Female , Mice , Conditioning, Classical/drug effects , Memory Consolidation/drug effects , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Memory/drug effects , Memory/physiology , Generalization, Psychological/drug effects , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/drug effects
5.
Learn Behav ; 51(3): 246-261, 2023 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36239878

ABSTRACT

Fear conditioning studies have occurred mostly in the laboratory, but recently researchers have started to adapt fear conditioning procedures for remote application. Standardization of aversive stimulus material not causing unnecessarily strong discomfort remains an issue especially relevant to research without experimental supervision. The present study introduces a novel semi-subjective method to calibrate aversive sounds in a remotely conducted fear conditioning paradigm. To demonstrate feasibility and proof of concept, 165 participants completed the paradigm, calibrating the loudness of an aversive sound without the guidance of an experimental instructor. This study also aimed to replicate existing findings of participant groups that differed in their early CS-UCS contingency awareness. Participants were classified as Accurate (UCS more likely after the CS+ than CS-), Poor (UCS more likely after the CS- than CS+, or UCS unlikely after either CS), and Threat Biased (UCS equally likely after the CS+ and CS-). Results indicated both the feasibility and efficacy of the paradigm, with participants showing typical patterns of fear learning. Threat Biased participants showed significantly higher uncertainty towards safety signals. There were no differences between the groups in terms of personality traits, thus questioning whether these attributes mediate differences in fear learning and the emergence of anxiety disorders. Using semi-subjective sound calibration appears to be functional, and future studies may consider implementing the new method when remotely administering fear conditioning paradigms.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Learning , Animals , Calibration , Feasibility Studies , Fear
6.
Mol Cell Neurosci ; 120: 103723, 2022 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35351607

ABSTRACT

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a devastating mental illness with high morbidity and major social and economic burden. Currently, there is no promising therapy available for the treatment of PTSD. Some clinical studies showed that ketamine could effectively alleviate PTSD symptoms. However, it is still unclear which brain region ketamine targets and how it attenuates the PTSD-like effects. In this study, we examined the effect of ketamine on fear generalization (a core symptom of PTSD) by using a mice model of fear generalization induced by fear conditioning procedure. Before retrieval, ketamine was locally infused into the nucleus accumbens (a brain region closely associated with PTSD). Fear generalization mice were subjected to behavioral testing and biochemical assessments, following ketamine infusion. The results showed that the foot shock strength-dependently induced fear generalization in mice with increased c-fos activity, and a lower level of GluR1(S845), GluR1(S831) protein, and a higher level of P-GluN2B protein in the nucleus accumbens (NAc). Local infusion of ketamine into NAc decreased the fear generalization together with an increased level of GluR1(S845), GluR1(S831) protein, and decreased level of P-GluN2B protein. Altogether, these results conclude that ketamine might affect the glutamatergic signaling in the NAc to attenuate the fear generalization in mice.


Subject(s)
Ketamine , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Animals , Fear/physiology , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Ketamine/pharmacology , Mice , Nucleus Accumbens , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/drug therapy
7.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37794273

ABSTRACT

Overgeneralization of conditioned fear is associated with anxiety disorders (AD). Most results stem from studies done in adult patients, but studies with children are rare, although the median onset of anxiety disorders lies already in childhood. Thus, the goal of the present study was to examine fear learning and generalization in youth participants, aged 10-17 years, with AD (n = 39) compared to healthy controls (HC) (n = 40). A discriminative fear conditioning and generalization paradigm was used. Ratings of arousal, valence, and US expectancy (the probability of an aversive noise following each stimulus) were measured, hypothesizing that children with AD compared to HC would show heightened ratings of arousal and US expectancy, and decreased positive valence ratings, respectively, as well as overgeneralization of fear. The results indicated that children with AD rated all stimuli as more arousing and less pleasant, and demonstrated higher US expectancy ratings to all stimuli when compared to HC. Thus, rather than displaying qualitatively different generalization patterns (e.g., a linear vs. quadratic slope of the gradient), differences between groups were more quantitative (similar, but parallel shifted gradient). Therefore, overgeneralization of conditioned fear does not seem to be a general marker of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents.

8.
Cereb Cortex ; 31(12): 5381-5395, 2021 10 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34145441

ABSTRACT

Traumatic memories may become less precise over time and lead to the development of fear responses to novel stimuli, a process referred to as time-dependent fear generalization. The conditions that cause the growth of fear generalization over time are poorly understood. Here, we found that, in male rats, the level of discrimination at the early time point contributes to determining whether fear generalization will develop with the passage of time or not, suggesting a link between the precision of recent memory and the stability of remote engrams. We also found that the expression of insulin-like growth factor 2 receptor in layer 2/3 of the auditory cortex is linked to the precision of recent memories and to the stability of remote engrams and the development of fear generalization over time. These findings provide new insights on the neural mechanisms that underlie the time-dependent development of fear generalization that may occur over time after a traumatic event.


Subject(s)
Auditory Cortex , Animals , Auditory Cortex/physiology , Fear/physiology , Male , Memory/physiology , Memory, Long-Term , Rats , Receptor, IGF Type 2
9.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 31(10): 1581-1590, 2022 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33983460

ABSTRACT

The aim of the study was to investigate age-related differences in fear learning and generalization in healthy children and adolescents (n = 133), aged 8-17 years, using an aversive discriminative fear conditioning and generalization paradigm adapted from Lau et al. (2008). In the current task, participants underwent 24 trials of discriminative conditioning of two female faces with neutral facial expressions, with (CS+) or without (CS-) a 95-dB loud female scream, presented simultaneously with a fearful facial expression (US). The discriminative conditioning was followed by 72 generalization trials (12 CS+, 12 GS1, 12 GS2, 12 GS3, 12 GS4, and 12 CS-): four generalization stimuli depicting gradual morphs from CS+ to CS- in 20%-steps were created for the generalization phases. We hypothesized that generalization in children and adolescents is negatively correlated with age. The subjective ratings of valence, arousal, and US expectancy (the probability of an aversive noise following each stimulus), as well as skin conductance responses (SCRs) were measured. Repeated-measures ANOVAs on ratings and SCR amplitudes were calculated with the within-subject factors stimulus type (CS+, CS-, GS1-4) and phase (Pre-Acquisition, Acquisition 1, Acquisition 2, Generalization 1, Generalization 2). To analyze the modulatory role of age, we additionally calculated ANCOVAs considering age as covariate. Results indicated that (1) subjective and physiological responses were generally lower with increasing age irrespective to the stimulus quality, and (2) stimulus discrimination improved with increasing age paralleled by reduced overgeneralization in older individuals. Longitudinal follow-up studies are required to analyze fear generalization with regard to brain maturational aspects and clarify whether overgeneralization of conditioned fear promotes the development of anxiety disorders or vice versa.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Generalization, Stimulus , Adolescent , Aged , Child , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Fear , Female , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Learning/physiology
10.
Neuroimage ; 239: 118308, 2021 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34175426

ABSTRACT

Fear generalization - the tendency to interpret ambiguous stimuli as threatening due to perceptual similarity to a learned threat - is an adaptive process. Overgeneralization, however, is maladaptive and has been implicated in a number of anxiety disorders. Neuroimaging research has indicated several regions sensitive to effects of generalization, including regions involved in fear excitation (e.g., amygdala, insula) and inhibition (e.g., ventromedial prefrontal cortex). Research has suggested several other small brain regions may play an important role in this process (e.g., hippocampal subfields, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis [BNST], habenula), but, to date, these regions have not been examined during fear generalization due to limited spatial resolution of standard human neuroimaging. To this end, we utilized the high spatial resolution of 7T fMRI to characterize the neural circuits involved in threat discrimination and generalization. Additionally, we examined potential modulating effects of trait anxiety and intolerance of uncertainty on neural activation during threat generalization. In a sample of 31 healthy undergraduate students, significant positive generalization effects (i.e., greater activation for stimuli with increasing perceptual similarity to a learned threat cue) were observed in the visual cortex, thalamus, habenula and BNST, while negative generalization effects were observed in the dentate gyrus, CA1, and CA3. Associations with individual differences were underpowered, though preliminary findings suggested greater generalization in the insula and primary somatosensory cortex may be correlated with self-reported anxiety. Overall, findings largely support previous neuroimaging work on fear generalization and provide additional insight into the contributions of several previously unexplored brain regions.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Fear/physiology , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Generalization, Stimulus/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Adolescent , Adult , Anxiety/physiopathology , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Female , Habenula/diagnostic imaging , Hippocampus/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/physiology , Septal Nuclei/diagnostic imaging , Somatosensory Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Thalamus/diagnostic imaging , Uncertainty , Visual Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
11.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 21(2): 372-389, 2021 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33629258

ABSTRACT

Because threatening situations often occur in a similar manner, the generalization of fear to similar situations is adaptive and can avoid harm to the organism. However, the overgeneralization of fear to harmless stimuli is maladaptive and assumed to contribute to anxiety disorders. Thus, elucidating factors that may modulate fear (over)generalization is important. Based on the known effects of acute stress on learning, which are at least partly due to noradrenergic arousal, we investigated whether stress may promote fear overgeneralization and whether we could counteract this effect by reducing noradrenergic arousal. In a placebo-controlled, double-blind, between-subjects design, 120 healthy participants underwent a fear-conditioning procedure on Day 1. Approximately 24 hours later, participants received orally either a placebo or the beta-adrenergic receptor antagonist propranolol and were exposed to a stress or control manipulation before they completed a test of fear generalization. Skin conductance responses as well as explicit rating data showed a successful acquisition of conditioned fear on Day 1 and a pronounced fear generalization 24 hours later. Although physiological data confirmed the successful stress manipulation and reduction of noradrenergic arousal, the extent of fear generalization remained unaffected by stress and propranolol. The absence of a stress effect on fear generalization was confirmed by a second study and a Bayesian analysis across both data sets. Our findings suggest that acute stress leaves fear generalization processes intact, at least in a sample of healthy, young individuals.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Bayes Theorem , Generalization, Psychological , Humans , Plant Leaves
12.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 21(5): 1054-1065, 2021 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34021495

ABSTRACT

Learned fear can be generalized through both perceptual and conceptual information. This study investigated how perceptual and conceptual similarities influence this generalization process. Twenty-three healthy volunteers completed a fear-generalization test as brain activity was recorded in the form of event-related potentials (ERPs). Participants were exposed to a de novo fear acquisition paradigm with four categories of conditioned stimuli (CS): two conceptual cues (animals and furniture); and two perceptual cues (blue and purple shapes). Animals (C+) and purple shapes (P+) were paired with the unconditioned stimulus (US), whereas furniture (C-) and blue shapes (P-) never were. The generalized stimuli were thus blue animals (C+P+, determined danger), blue furniture (C-P+, perceptual danger), purple animals (C+P-, conceptual danger), and purple furniture (C-P-, determined safe). We found that perceptual cues elicited larger fear responses and shorter reaction times than did conceptual cues during fear acquisition. This suggests that a perceptually related pathway might evoke greater fear than a conceptually based route. During generalization, participants were more afraid of C+ exemplars than of C- exemplars. Furthermore, C+ trials elicited greater N400 amplitudes. Thus, participants appear able to use conceptually based cues to infer the value of the current stimuli. Additionally, compared with C+ exemplars, we found an enhanced late positive potential effect in response to C- exemplars, which seems to reflect a late inhibitory process and might index safety learning. These findings may offer new insights into the pathological mechanism of anxiety disorders.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Female , Generalization, Psychological , Humans , Male
13.
Neuroimage ; 218: 116735, 2020 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32251834

ABSTRACT

Fear acquisition and generalization play key roles in promoting the survival of mammals and contribute to anxiety disorders. While previous research has provided much evidence for the repercussions of social exclusion on mental health, how social exclusion affects fear acquisition and generalization has received scant attention. In our study, participants were divided into two groups according to two Cyberball paradigm conditions (exclusion/inclusion). Both groups underwent a Pavlovian conditioning paradigm, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and skin conductance response (SCR) assessments. We aimed to determine the effects of social exclusion on fear acquisition and generalization and whether modulation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) mediates this relationship. Our results showed that socially excluded participants featured significantly higher and lower shock risk scores to safety stimuli (conditioned stimulus, CS-) and threatening stimuli (CS+), respectively, than did socially included subjects during fear acquisition. The exclusion group had increased skin conductance responses (SCRs) to CS and exhibited heightened shock risk and increased SCRs to generalized stimuli compared with the inclusion group. The fNIRS results demonstrated that the CS â€‹+ â€‹evoked larger oxy-Hb changes in the mPFC in the inclusion group than in the exclusion group during fear acquisition. Furthermore, the oxy-Hb of left mPFC of CS â€‹+ â€‹mediated the effect on the association between social exclusion and perceived risk of CS+ in the fear acquisition. Our results indicate that social exclusion impairs fear acquisition and generalization via the mediation of the mPFC and that social exclusion increases susceptibility to anxiety disorders through bias processing of fear discrimination in fear acquisition and generalization. By studying the role of social relationship in fear acquisition and generalization, our research provides new insights into the pathological mechanisms of anxiety disorder.


Subject(s)
Fear/psychology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Social Isolation/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Conditioning, Classical , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
14.
Neuroimage ; 205: 116302, 2020 01 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31639511

ABSTRACT

Acquired fear responses often generalize from conditioned stimuli (CS) towards perceptually similar, but harmless generalization stimuli (GS). Knowledge on similarities between CS and GS may be explicit or implicit. Employing behavioral measures and whole-head magnetoencephalography, we here investigated the neurocognitive mechanisms underpinning implicit fear generalization. Twenty-nine participants underwent a classical conditioning procedure in which 32 different faces were either paired with an aversive scream (16 CS+) or remained unpaired (16 CS-). CS+ and CS- faces systematically differed from each other regarding their ratio of eye distance and mouth width. High versus low values on this "threat-related feature (TF)" implicitly predicted the presence or absence of the aversive scream. In pre- and post-conditioning phases, all CS and 32 novel GS faces were presented. 16 GS+ â€‹faces shared the TF of the 16 CS+ â€‹faces, while 16 â€‹GS- faces shared the TF of the 16 CS- faces. Behavioral tests confirmed that participants were fully unaware of TF-US contingencies. CS+ â€‹compared to CS- faces revealed higher unpleasantness, arousal and US-expectancy ratings. A generalization of these behavioral fear responses to GS+ â€‹compared to GS- faces was observed by trend only. Source-estimations of event-related fields showed stronger neural responses to both CS+ and GS+ â€‹compared to CS- and GS- in anterior temporal (<100 â€‹ms) and temporo-occipital (<150 â€‹ms; 553-587 â€‹ms) ventral brain regions. Reverse effects were found in dorsal frontal areas (<100 â€‹ms; 173-203 â€‹ms; 257-290 â€‹ms). Neural data also revealed selectively enhanced responses to CS+ â€‹but not GS+ â€‹stimuli in occipital regions (110-167 â€‹ms; 330-413 â€‹ms), indicating perceptual discrimination. Our data suggest that the prioritized perceptual analysis of threat-associated conditioned faces in ventral networks rapidly generalizes to novel faces sharing threat-related features. This generalization process occurs in absence of contingency awareness and may thus contribute to implicit attentional biases. The coexisting perceptual discrimination suggests that fear generalization is not a mere consequence of insufficient stimulus discrimination but rather an active, integrative process.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Fear/physiology , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Magnetoencephalography , Motivation/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
15.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 167: 107099, 2020 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31698057

ABSTRACT

When fear is generalized, knowledge based on concepts is also retrieved. Concepts have two very different relations: thematic relations based on the co-occurrence of events or scenarios, and taxonomic relations based on similarity or shared features. However, it remains unclear whether thematic and taxonomic relationships differentially affect fear generalization. To clarify the underlying cognitive mechanisms of these relations, the current study combined the classical fear conditioning procedure with electroencephalography (EEG). Forty participants were conditioned to a neutral word by pairing the presentation of the word with an unpleasant electrical pulse. A different stimulus was not paired with the electrical pulse. Next, during generalization testing, thematically related or taxonomic-related words were presented. Behavioral responses (shock expectancy and response time) and brain responses (event-related potentials [ERP] and oscillation activity) were recorded. Behavioral results showed that taxonomic relations initiated higher shock expectancy compared with thematic relations, and that conceptual relations did not affect response times. Taxonomic relations induced larger P2 components than thematic relations, and danger generalization stimuli initiated smaller P600 components than safe generalization stimuli. In addition, the magnitudes of alpha and beta oscillations were larger for danger generalization stimuli. These results suggested that taxonomic stimuli generalize broader responses compared with thematic relations after fear conditioning. Therefore, we report a possible new electrophysiological evidence for the presentation of fear generalization. These findings aid our understanding of fear generalization at the concept level and have clinical implications for the cognitive treatment of anxiety disorders.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Concept Formation/physiology , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Evoked Potentials , Fear/physiology , Generalization, Stimulus/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Electroshock , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
16.
J Neurosci ; 38(39): 8313-8328, 2018 09 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30093537

ABSTRACT

The medial prefrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala (BLA) are essential for discriminating between harmful and safe stimuli. The primary auditory cortex (Te1) sends projections to both sites, but whether and how it interacts with these areas during fear discrimination are poorly understood. Here we show that in male rats that can differentiate between a new tone and a threatening one, the selective optogenetic inhibition of Te1 axon terminals into the prelimbic (PL) cortex shifted discrimination to fear generalization. Meanwhile, no effects were detected when Te1 terminals were inhibited in the BLA. Using a combination of local field potential and multiunit recordings, we show that in animals that discriminate successfully between a new tone and a harmful one, the activity of the Te1 and the PL cortex becomes immediately and tightly synchronized in the slow-gamma range (40-70 Hz) at the onset of the new tone. This enhanced synchronization was not present in other frequency ranges, such as the theta range. Critically, the level of gamma synchrony predicted the behavioral choice (i.e., no freezing or freezing) of the animals. Moreover, in the same rats, gamma synchrony was absent before the fear-learning trial and when animals should discriminate between an olfactory stimulus and the auditory harmful one. Thus, our findings reveal that the Te1 and the PL cortex dynamically establish a functional connection during auditory fear-discrimination processes, and that this corticocortical oscillatory mechanism drives the behavioral choice of the animals.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Identifying neural networks that infer safety versus danger is of great interest in the scientific field. Fear generalization reduces the chances of an animal's survival and leads to psychiatric diseases, such as post-traumatic stress disorders and phobias in humans. Here we demonstrate that animals able to differentiate a new tone from a previous threating tone showed synchronization between the prefrontal and primary auditory cortices. Critically, this connectivity precedes and predicts the behavioral outcome of the animal. Optogenetic inhibition of this functional connectivity leads to fear generalization. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to demonstrate that a corticocortical dialogue occurring between sensory and prefrontal areas is a key node for fear-discrimination processes.


Subject(s)
Auditory Cortex/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Fear/physiology , Gamma Rhythm , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Basolateral Nuclear Complex/physiology , Conditioning, Classical , Cortical Synchronization , Generalization, Psychological , Male , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Optogenetics , Rats, Wistar
17.
Neuroimage ; 188: 445-455, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30572112

ABSTRACT

The generalization of conditioned fear responses has been shown to decrease as a function of perceptual similarity. However, generalization may also extend beyond the perceptual discrimination threshold, ostensibly due to contributions from processes other than perception. Currently the neural mechanisms that mediate perceptual and non-perceptual aspects of fear generalization are unclear. To investigate this question, we conducted a Pavlovian fear conditioning and generalization experiment, collecting functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), skin conductance and explicit shock likelihood ratings, in 37 healthy subjects. Face stimuli were initially paired (CS+) or not paired (CS) with an electrical shock. During the generalization phase, responses were measured to the CS+, CS and a range of CS + -toCS morphs (generalization stimuli), selected for each participant based on that participant's discrimination ability. Across multiple measurements, we found that fear generalization responses were limited to stimuli that could not be distinguished from the CS + stimulus, thus following a gradient closely linked to perceptual discriminability. These measurements, which were correlated with one another, included skin conductance responses, behavioral ratings, and fMRI responses of anterior insula and superior frontal gyrus. In contrast, responses in areas of the default network, including the posterior cingulate gyrus, angular gyrus and hippocampus, showed a negative generalization function extending to stimuli that were more likely to be distinguished from the CS+. In addition, the generalization gradients of the anterior insula and the behavioral ratings showed some evidence for extension beyond perceptual limits. Taken together, these results suggest that distinct brain areas are involved in perceptual and non-perceptual components of fear generalization.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Fear/physiology , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Generalization, Response/physiology , Adult , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Facial Recognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
18.
Biochem Biophys Res Commun ; 513(1): 248-254, 2019 05 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30954227

ABSTRACT

A typical feature of the contextual fear memory is increased fear generalization with time. Though much attention has been given to the neural structures that underlie the long-term consolidation of a contextual fear memory, the molecular mechanisms regulating fear generalization remain unclear. We observed that retrieval of contextual fear in a novel context at a remote time point increased coupling of neuronal nitric oxide synthase (nNOS) with postsynaptic density-95 (PSD-95) and c-Fos expression in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Disrupting nNOS-PSD-95 coupling in the ACC decreased the expression of Histone deacetylase 2 (HDAC2), and inhibited contextual fear generalization at a remote time point. Together, our findings reveal nNOS-PSD-95 interaction in the ACC could be a promising target to prevent or reverse contextual fear generalization.


Subject(s)
Fear , Guanylate Kinases/metabolism , Gyrus Cinguli/physiology , Membrane Proteins/metabolism , Nitric Oxide Synthase Type I/metabolism , Animals , Fear/physiology , Fear/psychology , Generalization, Psychological , Male , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Protein Interaction Maps
19.
Hippocampus ; 28(8): 602-616, 2018 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29747244

ABSTRACT

The thalamic nucleus reuniens (NR) has been shown to support bidirectional medial prefrontal cortex-hippocampus communication and synchronization relevant for cognitive processing. Using non-selective or prolonged inactivation of the NR, previous studies reported its activity positively modulates aversive memory consolidation. Here we examined the NR's role in consolidating contextual fear memories with varied strength, at both recent and more remote time points, using muscimol-induced temporary inactivation in rats. Results indicate the NR negatively modulates fear memory intensity, specificity, and long-term maintenance. The more intense, generalized, and enduring fear memory induced by NR inactivation during consolidation was less prone to behavioral suppression by extinction or reconsolidation disruption induced by clonidine, an alpha-2 adrenergic receptor agonist. Lastly, we used immunohistochemistry for Arc protein, which is involved in synaptic modifications underlying memory consolidation, to investigate whether treatment condition and/or conditioning status could change its levels not only in the NR, but also in the hippocampus (dorsal and ventral CA1 subregions) and the medial prefrontal cortex (anterior cingulate, prelimbic and infralimbic subregions). Results indicate a significant imbalance in the number of Arc-expressing neurons in the brain areas investigated in muscimol fear conditioned animals when compared with controls. Collectively, present results provide convergent evidence for the NR's role as a hub regulating quantitative and qualitative aspects of a contextual fear memory during its consolidation that seem to influence the subsequent susceptibility to experimental interventions aiming at attenuating its expression. They also indicate the selectivity and duration of a given inactivation approach may influence its outcomes.


Subject(s)
Fear/physiology , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Memory/physiology , Midline Thalamic Nuclei/physiology , AIDS-Related Complex/metabolism , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Exploratory Behavior/drug effects , Extinction, Psychological/drug effects , Fear/drug effects , GABA-A Receptor Agonists/pharmacology , Male , Maze Learning/drug effects , Memory Consolidation/drug effects , Midline Thalamic Nuclei/drug effects , Muscimol/pharmacology , Rats , Rats, Wistar , Time Factors
20.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 151: 18-27, 2018 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29551602

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to examine how a daytime nap affected the consolidation of fear learning. Participants first underwent fear conditioning during which they were exposed to a large and a small circle. One of these was repeatedly paired with an electric shock (making it the CS+), whereas the other circle was never paired with the shock (the CS-). After a delay interval containing either a nap or wake, participants again viewed the CS+ and the CS- intermixed with eight novel circles that varied in size between the two stimuli seen before, as well as a blue triangle that served as a novel stimulus without prior fear relevance. We examined both fear retention (the difference between the CS+ and the CS-) and fear generalization (responses to the novel stimuli based on their similarity to the original CS+). Contrary to previous studies, results from the participants who acquired a differentiated fear response during the acquisition phase revealed that the wake group showed significantly larger skin conductance responses to the CS+ compared to the CS-, whereas no such difference was present in the sleep group. These results were not driven by differences in explicit memory or by differences in general reactivity. Analyzing responses to the novel stimuli revealed a tendency towards a more generalized response in the sleep group, with no differences between the CS+ and any other stimulus, whereas the wake group showed increased responses to the stimuli depending on their similarity to the original CS+. This effect was however only present when controlling for baseline differences in worry.


Subject(s)
Fear/physiology , Generalization, Psychological/physiology , Learning/physiology , Memory Consolidation/physiology , Sleep , Adult , Conditioning, Classical , Electroshock , Female , Galvanic Skin Response , Humans , Male , Time Factors , Wakefulness , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL