Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 460
Filter
Add more filters

Publication year range
1.
Psychol Res ; 87(6): 1981-1994, 2023 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36580145

ABSTRACT

Memory systems serve an adaptive function for the fitness of organisms. A good example of this is the Survival Processing Effect (SPE) which points to increased retention of information when it is processed in a survival context compared to other contexts. Survival processing may also affect metacognitive processes, by increasing confidence judgments as well as increasing metacognitive sensitivity. No previous study, however, has directly examined whether processing information for survival also has an effect on metacognitive processes. Here we ask whether SPE extends to the metacognitive system in terms of both metacognitive sensitivity and confidence bias. In Experiment 1 participants were asked to rate a list of words in terms of relevance in a survival scenario or a moving scenario. In a surprise old/new recognition test, they were given one word at a time and asked to indicate if they have rated the presented word before and state how confident they are in that choice. Surprisingly, the results did not reveal a SPE, which may have been due to high overall performance in the recognition task. In Experiment 2 we increased the level of difficulty of the memory task, which resulted in a robust SPE, but could not find this effect in metacognitive monitoring. Together, these results suggest that survival processing may not affect metacognitive processes in a reliable fashion.


Subject(s)
Memory , Metacognition , Survival , Metacognition/physiology , Survival/physiology , Survival/psychology , Humans , Male , Female , Adolescent , Young Adult , Adult , Memory/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology
2.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 18(5): 311-319, 2017 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28360419

ABSTRACT

The nature and neural implementation of emotions is the subject of vigorous debate. Here, we use Bayesian decision theory to address key complexities in this field and conceptualize emotions in terms of their relationship to survival-relevant behavioural choices. Decision theory indicates which behaviours are optimal in a given situation; however, the calculations required are radically intractable. We therefore conjecture that the brain uses a range of pre-programmed algorithms that provide approximate solutions. These solutions seem to produce specific behavioural manifestations of emotions and can also be associated with core affective dimensions. We identify principles according to which these algorithms are implemented in the brain and illustrate our approach by considering decision making in the face of proximal threat.


Subject(s)
Algorithms , Brain/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Survival/psychology , Animals , Bayes Theorem , Decision Making , Humans , Survival/physiology
3.
Dev Psychobiol ; 63(7): e22197, 2021 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34674247

ABSTRACT

The transition to fatherhood may be challenged with anxiety and trepidation. A high prevalence has been found for paternal depression and it is reactive to maternal depression. This review aims to address potential sources of paternal depression, which may have adverse consequences on child development. We describe through three hypotheses how fathers may be at risk of depression during the transition to fatherhood: (1) psychological (interacting with ecological systems); (2) brain functional∖structural changes; and (3) (epi)genomic. We propose that paternal stressful experiences during the transition to fatherhood may be the source for paternal depression through direct stressful paternal experiences or via (potential, currently debated) nonexperienced (by the father) epigenomic transgenerational transmission. On the other hand, we suggest that resilient fathers may undergo a transient dysphoric period affected by identifying with the newborn's vulnerability as well as with the mother's postpartum vulnerability resulting in "paternity blues." In accordance with recent views on paternal "heightened sensitivity" toward the infant, we propose that the identification of both parents with the vulnerability of the newborn creates a sensitive period of Folie a Deux (shared madness) which may be a healthy transient, albeit a quasi-pathological period, recruited by the orienting response of the newborn for survival.


Subject(s)
Depression, Postpartum , Depression , Fathers , Shared Paranoid Disorder , Adaptation, Psychological , Anxiety/psychology , Depression/psychology , Depression, Postpartum/psychology , Fathers/psychology , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn/psychology , Male , Mothers/psychology , Parent-Child Relations , Postpartum Period/psychology , Shared Paranoid Disorder/psychology , Survival/psychology
4.
Australas Psychiatry ; 28(6): 636-638, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32683892

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: If the narrative of "coronavirus" has an underlying theme, it may perhaps for some be one of survival, whilst for others, the theme might be suffering. The recurring motif of survival has continued throughout history, yet for the first time the sum of all fears has amounted to a run on the bare essentials. This paper seeks to offer an alternative formulation of "panic buying," with references to literature, philosophy, and contemporary neurobiology. CONCLUSION: The bare essentials disappeared perhaps as part of some self-fulfilling prophecy: the supermarkets became bare because others inadvertently lead us to believe they would become bare. The contagion model of emotional propagation provides a psychological model of how "panic buying" by an individual might lead to the replication of panic in an observer. The Polyvagal Theory further informs us of how the threat posed by the pandemic primes our limbic system to perceive danger, and explains how witnessing others engaging in fight-flight responses might evoke a fearful affect in an observer. In the end, it is perhaps through Nietzsche's study of classical tragedy that we may find some meaning to the pandemic, allowing our collective lived experience to serve as a template for growth.


Subject(s)
Coronavirus Infections/psychology , Disaster Victims/psychology , Fear , Neurobiology , Panic , Pneumonia, Viral/psychology , Survival/psychology , Betacoronavirus , COVID-19 , Emotional Adjustment , Humans , Pandemics , Psychological Theory , Psychology, Social , SARS-CoV-2
5.
Psychol Med ; 49(16): 2764-2771, 2019 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30585557

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The 2004 tsunami, the civil conflict until 2009 and the youth insurrection in the late 1980s in Sri Lanka resulted in many persons being classified as 'missing' as they disappeared and were unaccounted for. Our aim was to compare the prevalence of major depressive disorder (MDD) and prolonged grief disorder (PGD) in families of disappeared individuals, who eventually received the mortal remains and those who did not. METHOD: An ethically approved cross sectional study was conducted in a purposively selected sample after informed consent. Information on the circumstances of the family member going missing was gathered. Culturally adapted versions of the General Health Questionnaire and the Beck Depression Scale were administered. Those who screened positive were assessed by a psychiatrist on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5 criteria to arrive at a diagnosis. RESULTS: Of 391 cases of disappearances studied, MDD (17.5% v. 6%) and PGD (22% v. 7%) were significantly higher in those who did not eventually receive the mortal remains of the disappeared person. Among those who did not receive the mortal remains, being unsure whether the disappeared person was dead or alive was highly predictive of MDD and PGD. Mothers and wives, older family members and those with a family history of mental illness were more vulnerable. CONCLUSIONS: Family members of missing individuals unsure whether their loved one was alive or dead have higher psychological morbidity in the form of MDD and PGD.


Subject(s)
Depressive Disorder, Major/epidemiology , Family/psychology , Grief , Hope , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cross-Sectional Studies , Depressive Disorder, Major/diagnosis , Disasters , Female , Humans , Logistic Models , Male , Middle Aged , Morbidity , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Sri Lanka/epidemiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/epidemiology , Survival/psychology , Young Adult
6.
Psychooncology ; 28(6): 1286-1292, 2019 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31020758

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Clinicians frequently overestimate survival time among seriously ill patients, and this can result in medical treatment at end of life that does not reflect the patient's preferences. Little is known, however, about the sources of clinicians' optimistic bias in survival estimation. Related work in social networks and experimental psychology demonstrates that psychological states-such as optimism-can transfer from one person to another. METHODS: We directly observed and audio recorded 189 initial inpatient palliative care consultations among hospitalized patients with advanced cancer. Patients self-reported their level of trait optimism and expectations for survival prognosis prior to the palliative care consultation, and the palliative care clinicians rated their expectations for the patient's survival time following the initial conversation with the patient. We followed patient mortality for 6 months. RESULTS: Patient optimism was associated with clinician overestimation of their survival in a dose-response relationship. Clinicians were approximately three times as likely to overestimate the survival of patients endorsing both high trait optimism and optimistic ratings of their survival time compared with neither (OR: 2.95; 95% CI: 1.24-7.02). This association was not attenuated by adjustment for age, gender, race, ethnicity, education, income, cancer type, functional status, quality of life, or white blood cell count (ORadj : 3.45; 95% CI: 1.24-9.66). CONCLUSION: Patients' optimism may have some influence over their clinicians' prognostic judgments.


Subject(s)
Neoplasms/diagnosis , Neoplasms/psychology , Optimism , Palliative Care/psychology , Survival/psychology , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prognosis , Quality of Life/psychology
7.
Community Ment Health J ; 55(3): 493-496, 2019 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29691769

ABSTRACT

We investigated the association between exposure to chemical warfare and chronic mental/physical conditions. This was a secondary analysis of data from a case-control study on Iranian male veterans. Participants with neuropsychiatric disorders other than depressive/anxiety disorders, anatomical defects, or malignancies were excluded. Compared to non-exposed veterans, exposed veterans demonstrated significantly higher odds of PTSD [OR (95% CI) = 5.23 (1.98-13.85)], hypertension [OR (95% CI) = 5.57 (1.68-18.48)], coronary heart disease [OR (95% CI) = 6.8 (1.62-28.49)], and diabetes [OR (95% CI) = 3.88 (1.35-11.16)], and marginally higher odds of moderate to severe depressive symptoms [OR (95% CI) = 2.21 (0.93-5.28)]. This study provides preliminary evidence on association of exposure to chemical warfare with long-term mental disorders as well as chronic medical conditions.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/epidemiology , Chemical Warfare , Chronic Disease/epidemiology , Depression/epidemiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/epidemiology , Survival/psychology , Veterans/psychology , Chemical Warfare/psychology , Chronic Disease/psychology , Coronary Disease/epidemiology , Diabetes Mellitus/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Hypertension/epidemiology , Iran/epidemiology , Male , Mental Health , Middle Aged , Population Surveillance , Risk Factors , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Time
8.
J Community Psychol ; 47(6): 1548-1562, 2019 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31212376

ABSTRACT

AIMS: Using an entrepreneurship lens, this study examined the narratives of urban adults experiencing homelessness and living with mental illness, to explore strategies used for day-to-day survival. METHODS: Semi-structured qualitative interviews were conducted with 14 females, 30 males, and one individual identifying as "other," living in a mid-sized Canadian city. The average age was 39 years. Data were transcribed verbatim and analyzed using thematic analysis informed by grounded theory. FINDINGS: Participants described creative and intentional strategies for managing life on the street without permanent shelter, including recognition of opportunities, mobilization of their own or acquired resources, and use of social connections and communication skills, and strategies that demonstrated entrepreneurial processes. CONCLUSIONS: Findings suggest that participants used survival entrepreneurship strategies and processes to navigate daily life while experiencing homelessness. Recognition and validation of the propensity for enterprise and self-sufficiency are central for both individual recovery and ending homelessness within similar populations.


Subject(s)
Entrepreneurship/trends , Ill-Housed Persons/psychology , Mental Disorders/psychology , Survival/psychology , Urban Population/statistics & numerical data , Adult , Canada/epidemiology , Female , Housing , Humans , Interview, Psychological/methods , Male , Mental Disorders/epidemiology , Middle Aged , Narration , Qualitative Research , Risk Factors , Social Environment , Thematic Apperception Test , Urban Population/trends
9.
BMC Psychiatry ; 18(1): 234, 2018 07 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30029631

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The internal suicide debate hypothesis assumes that in a suicidal crisis, individuals are involved in an internal struggle over whether to live or die. Reasons for living (RFL) and Reasons for dying (RFD) are important individual reasons for staying alive (e.g. family) or wanting to die (e.g. hopelessness) and reflect this internal motivational conflict of the suicidal mind. The aim of this study was to explore the association between RFL and RFD of suicide attempters and current and future suicide ideation and behavior. METHOD: The sample consisted of 60 patients who were admitted at a psychiatric emergency unit in Switzerland following an attempted suicide. They received treatment as usual, participated in an assessment interview and completed self-report questionnaires. Additionally, they were instructed to write down up to five individual RFL and RFD. The number of RFL and RFD responses, depressive symptoms, and suicide ideation were assessed at baseline and 6, 12, and 24 months follow-up. Outcome measures were suicide ideation and repeated suicide attempts. Multiple imputations were used in order to address missing data. RESULTS: The number of RFD responses was the strongest predictor for increased suicide ideation at baseline. The number of RFL responses was not associated with suicide ideation and reattempts. RFD, depressive symptoms, and baseline suicide ideation predicted subsequent suicide reattempt up to 12 months later in simple regression analyses. Mediation analyses suggested that RFD mediated the effect of depressive symptoms at baseline on suicide ideation at 12-months follow-up. CONCLUSION: RFL were unrelated to the mental health of study participants and did not function as protective factor against suicide risk. RFD may be an important motivational driver in the suicidal process. Clinical interventions should focus more on the reduction of RFD than on RFL in suicidal individuals.


Subject(s)
Attitude to Death , Suicidal Ideation , Suicide, Attempted/psychology , Survival/psychology , Adult , Depression/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation , Prospective Studies , Regression Analysis , Surveys and Questionnaires , Switzerland
10.
J Pers ; 86(2): 233-246, 2018 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28192852

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: People believe that they can respond effectively to threats, but actually they experience difficulties in disengaging from ongoing tasks and shifting their attention to life-threatening events. We contend that this tendency is especially true for secure people with respect to their worldview and perception of others and not for insecure individuals. METHOD: In Study 1 (N = 290), we examined individuals' reactions to various threat scenarios. In Study 2 (N = 65), we examined these reactions using a behavioral design high in ecological validity. In Study 3 (N = 78), we examined group-level benefits for the actions of insecure individuals by manipulating asocial behavior in response to an emergency. RESULTS: Study 1 indicated that anxiously attached individuals stayed away from threats and sought help; avoidant people tended to take action by either assessing the risk of the event and/or enacting an asocial action such as fight or flight. Study 2 added ecological validity to these findings, and Study 3 showed that priming asocial behavior responses promoted actions that increased group members' chances of survival. CONCLUSION: Results validate the central tenets of social defense theory and indicate that actions that are deemed asocial may paradoxically promote the survival of individuals and groups.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/psychology , Dangerous Behavior , Defense Mechanisms , Fear , Object Attachment , Social Behavior , Adolescent , Adult , Attention , Avoidance Learning , Female , Humans , Israel , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation , Psychological Theory , Self Report , Survival/psychology , Young Adult
11.
Qual Health Res ; 28(6): 939-949, 2018 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29577848

ABSTRACT

Research on clinicians' interpretations of self-harming practices has shown that they can often be negative. To date there has been limited consideration of other professionals' narratives, notably those working in social care. This article presents focus group and interview data generated with foster carers ( n = 15) and residential carers ( n = 15) to explore the symbolic meanings ascribed to self-harm among the children and young people they care for. Three repertoires of interpretation are presented: survival, which conceives self-harm as a mechanism for redefining the identity of "looked-after"; signaling, which understands self-harm as a communicative tool for the expression of emotion; and security, which sees self-harming practices as testing the authenticity and safety of the caring relationship. Through their focus on sociocultural narratives, carers position themselves as experts on self-harm due to their intimacy with young people's social worlds. This construction potentially creates distance from health professionals, which is problematic given the current privileging of interprofessional working.


Subject(s)
Caregivers/psychology , Foster Home Care/psychology , Orphanages , Self-Injurious Behavior/psychology , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Interviews as Topic , Male , Qualitative Research , Survival/psychology , Wales
12.
Aust Health Rev ; 42(1): 53-58, 2018 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27978419

ABSTRACT

Objective The aim of the present study was to understand the reasons for the delivery of non-beneficial cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR) attempts in a tertiary private hospital over 12 months. We determined doctors' expectations of survival after CPR for their patient, whether they had considered a not-for-resuscitation (NFR) order and the barriers to completion of NFR orders. Methods Anonymous questionnaires were sent to the doctors primarily responsible for a given patient's care in the hospital within 2 weeks of the unsuccessful CPR attempt. The data were analysed quantitatively where appropriate and qualitatively for themes for open-text responses Results Most doctors surveyed in the present study understood the poor outcome after CPR in the older person. Most doctors had an expectation that their own patient had a poor prognosis and a poor likely predicted outcome after CPR. This implied that the patient's death was neither unexpected nor likely to be reversible. Some doctors considered NFR orders, but multiple barriers to completion were cited, including the family's wishes, being time poor and diffusion or deferral of responsibility. Conclusions It is likely that futile CPR is provided contrary to policy and legal documents relating to end-of-life care, with the potential for harms relating to both patient and family, and members of resuscitation teams. The failure appears to relate to process rather than recognition of poor patient outcome. What is known about the topic? Mandatory CPR has been established in Australian hospitals on the premise that it will save lives. The outcome from in-hospital cardiac arrest has not improved despite significant training and resources. The outcome for those acutely hospitalised patients aged over 80 years has been repeatedly demonstrated to be poor with significant morbidity in the survivors. There is emerging literature on the extent of the delivery of non-beneficial treatments at the end of life, including futile CPR, the recognition of harms incurred by patients, families and members of the resuscitation teams and on the opportunity cost of the inappropriate use of resources. What does this paper add? This is the first study, to our knowledge, that has demonstrated that doctors understood the outcomes for CPR, particularly in those aged 80 years and older, and that failure to recognise poor outcome and prognosis in their own patients is not a barrier to writing NFR orders. What are the implications for practitioners? Recognition of the poor outcomes from CPR for the elderly patient for whom the doctor has a duty of care should result in a discussion with the patients, allowing an exploration of values and expectations of treatment. This would promote shared decision making, which includes the use of CPR. Facilitation of these discussions should be the focus of health service review.


Subject(s)
Attitude of Health Personnel , Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation/psychology , Physicians/psychology , Resuscitation Orders/psychology , Survival/psychology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Databases, Factual , Female , Hospital Mortality , Hospitals, Teaching , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Physician-Patient Relations , Prognosis , Risk Assessment , Surveys and Questionnaires , Tertiary Care Centers , Victoria/epidemiology
13.
J Theor Biol ; 430: 86-91, 2017 10 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28684294

ABSTRACT

Consider and infinitely large asexual population without mutations and direct interactions. The activities of an individual determine the fecundity and the survival probability of individuals, moreover each activity takes time. We view this population model as a simple combination of life history and optimal foraging models. The phenotypes are given by probability distributions on these activities. We concentrate on the following phenotypes defined by optimization of different objective functions: selfish individual (maximizes the average offspring number during life span), survival phenotype (maximizes the probability of non-extinction of descendants) and Darwinian phenotype (maximizes the phenotypic growth rate). We find that the objective functions above can achieve their maximum at different activity distributions, in general. We find that the objective functions above can achieve their maximum at different activity distributions, in general. The novelty of our work is that we let natural selection act on the different objective functions. Using the classical Darwinian reasoning, we show that in our selection model the Darwinian phenotype outperforms all other phenotypes.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Genetic Fitness , Phenotype , Survival/psychology , Animals , Competitive Behavior , Humans , Selection, Genetic
14.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci ; 266(4): 329-36, 2016 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26324882

ABSTRACT

The traumatic life events, including earthquakes, war, and interpersonal conflicts, cause a cascade of psychological and biological changes known as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Malondialdehyde (MDA) is a reliable marker of lipid peroxidation, and paraoxonase is a known antioxidant enzyme. The aims of this study were to investigate the relationship between earthquake trauma, PTSD effects on oxidative stress and the levels of serum paraoxonase 1 (PON1) enzyme activity, and levels of serum MDA. The study was carried out on three groups called: the PTSD group, the traumatized with earthquake exercise group, and healthy control group, which contained 32, 31, and 38 individuals, respectively. Serum MDA levels and PON1 enzyme activities from all participants were measured, and the results were compared across all groups. There were no significant differences between the PTSD patients and non-PTSD earthquake survivors in terms of the study variables. The mean PON1 enzyme activity from PTSD patients was significantly lower, while the mean MDA level was significantly higher than that of the healthy control group (p < 0.01 for both measurements). Similarly, earthquake survivors who did not develop PTSD showed higher MDA levels and lower PON1 activity when compared to healthy controls. However, the differences between these groups did not reach a statistically significant level. Increased MDA level and decreased PON1 activity measured in PTSD patients after earthquake and may suggest increased oxidative stress in these patients. The nonsignificant trends that are observed in lipid peroxidation markers of earthquake survivors may indicate higher impact of PTSD development on these markers than trauma itself. For example, PTSD diagnosis seems to add to the effect of trauma on serum MDA levels and PON1 enzyme activity. Thus, serum MDA levels and PON1 enzyme activity may serve as biochemical markers of PTSD diagnosis.


Subject(s)
Earthquakes , Lipid Peroxidation/physiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/blood , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/physiopathology , Survival/psychology , Adult , Aryldialkylphosphatase/blood , Earthquakes/mortality , Female , Humans , Male , Malondialdehyde/blood , Middle Aged , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Retrospective Studies , Statistics, Nonparametric , Young Adult
15.
Memory ; 22(1): 65-75, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23163792

ABSTRACT

In a recent paper Hart and Burns (2012) presented evidence that conditions that prime thoughts of one's mortality benefit recall. Drawing on the conceptual relation between thoughts of death and thoughts of survival, Hart and Burns interpret their findings as suggestive of the possibility that death-related thoughts function in manner similar to survival-related thoughts in enhancing recall. In the present study I draw on evolutionary arguments to question whether a conceptual relation between thoughts of death and thoughts of survival translates into a functional relation. I then present data showing that while death-related thoughts can promote high levels of recall, (a) the level achieved does not match that produced by survival processing and (b) survival and death cognition likely rely on different mechanisms to achieve their effects.


Subject(s)
Death , Memory/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Survival/psychology , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
16.
Memory ; 22(1): 9-18, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23324076

ABSTRACT

Previous research has demonstrated that participants remember significantly more survival-related information and more information that is processed for its survival relevance. Recent research has also shown that survival materials and processing result in more false memories, ones that are adaptive inasmuch as they prime solutions to insight-based problems. Importantly, false memories for survival-related information facilitate problem solving more than false memories for other types of information. The present study explores this survival advantage using an incidental rather than intentional memory task. Here participants rated information either in the context of its importance to a survival-processing scenario or to moving to a new house. Following this, participants solved a number of compound remote associate tasks (CRATs), half of which had the solution primed by false memories that were generated during the processing task. Results showed that (a) CRATs were primed by false memories in this incidental task, with participants solving significantly more CRATs when primed than when unprimed, (b) this effect was greatest when participants rated items for survival than moving, and (c) processing items for a survival scenario improved overall problem-solving performance even when specific problems themselves were not primed. Results are discussed with regard to adaptive theories of memory.


Subject(s)
Mental Processes/physiology , Problem Solving/physiology , Repression, Psychology , Survival/psychology , Cues , Female , Humans , Intention , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult
17.
Memory ; 22(1): 36-50, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23607295

ABSTRACT

Processing items for their relevance to survival improves recall for those items relative to numerous other deep processing encoding techniques. Perhaps related, placing individuals in a mortality salient state has also been shown to enhance retention of items encoded after the morality salience manipulation (e.g., in a pleasantness rating task), a phenomenon we dubbed the "dying-to-remember" (DTR) effect. The experiments reported here further explored the effect and tested the possibility that the DTR effect is related to survival processing. Experiment 1 replicated the effect using different encoding tasks, demonstrating that the effect is not dependent on the pleasantness task. In Experiment 2 the DTR effect was associated with increases in item-specific processing, not relational processing, according to several indices. Experiment 3 replicated the main results of Experiment 2, and tested the effects of mortality salience and survival processing within the same experiment. The DTR effect and its associated difference in item-specific processing were completely eliminated when the encoding task required survival processing. These results are consistent with the interpretation that the mechanisms responsible for survival processing and DTR effects are overlapping.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Death , Memory/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Survival/psychology , Adult , Algorithms , Analysis of Variance , Cluster Analysis , Environment , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult
18.
Memory ; 22(1): 19-25, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23638617

ABSTRACT

Prior studies have convincingly demonstrated that survival-related processing of information enhances its subsequent retention. This phenomenon, known as the survival recall advantage, generalises to other stimuli, memory domains, and research populations, thereby underscoring its reliability. As previous studies used only short retention intervals between survival processing and the memory test, an important yet hitherto unanswered issue is whether this effect persists over time. The present experiment therefore examined whether survival processing also produces mnemonic benefits when retention is tested after longer delay periods. Participants (N =81) rated the relevance of words according to a survival and a moving scenario, and were then randomly assigned to the typical immediate (3-minute delay) retention test condition or conditions that included a 24- or 48-hour interval between survival processing and memory testing. In each of these conditions survival processing led to higher surprise free recall and recognition rates than processing words according to the moving scenario. Thus this study provides evidence that illustrates the longevity of survival processing advantages on memory performance.


Subject(s)
Death , Memory/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Survival/psychology , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
19.
Memory ; 22(1): 51-64, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23678953

ABSTRACT

Merely contemplating one's death improves retention for entirely unrelated material learned subsequently. This "dying to remember" effect seems conceptually related to the survival processing effect, whereby processing items for their relevance to being stranded in the grasslands leads to recall superior to that of other deep processing control conditions. The present experiments directly compared survival processing scenarios with "death processing" scenarios. Results showed that when the survival and dying scenarios are closely matched on key dimensions, and possible congruency effects are controlled, the dying and survival scenarios produced equivalently high recall levels. We conclude that the available evidence (cf. Bell, Roer, & Buchner, 2013; Klein, 2012), while not definitive, is consistent with the possibility of overlapping mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Death , Memory/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Survival/psychology , Affect/physiology , Female , Humans , Imagination , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Television , Young Adult
20.
Memory ; 22(1): 26-35, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23786410

ABSTRACT

Information that is processed in relation to survival tends to promote superior recall relative to other elaborate encoding manipulations (e.g., Nairne, Thompson, & Pandeirada, 2007). The current research examines whether perceived threat plays a role in the survival processing memory advantage. In the current experiment survival processing was manipulated such that participants were presented with an ancestral (grasslands) or modern (city) context, and either a low, medium, or high threat level. The results revealed a strong survival processing advantage, with the magnitude of the advantage related to level of perceived threat. The findings as a whole suggest that perceived threat contributes to the recall advantage.


Subject(s)
Death , Memory/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Survival/psychology , Affect , Environment , Fear/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Perception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL