Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 38
Filtrar
1.
Behav Brain Sci ; 39: e235, 2016 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28355849

RESUMEN

Firestone & Scholl (F&S) assume that pure perception is unaffected by cognition. This assumption is untenable for definitional, anatomical, and empirical reasons. They discount research showing nonoptical influences on visual perception, pointing out possible methodological "pitfalls." Results generated in multiple labs are immune to these "pitfalls," suggesting that perceptions of physical layout do indeed reflect bioenergetic resources.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Percepción Visual , Metabolismo Energético , Humanos
2.
Cogn Emot ; 28(2): 361-74, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23895111

RESUMEN

Three experiments examined the hypothesis that stress-induced arousal enhances long-term memory for experiences associated with arousing events. Contrary to expectations, in each experiment exposure to a stressor (arm immersion in ice water) interfered with, rather than enhanced, long-term memory for associated material. Despite varying the stimuli (words, pictures), their emotional value (positive, negative, neutral), the time between learning and stress inductions (0 to 1 minute), and opportunities for post-learning rehearsal, each experiment produced a significant reversal of the hypothesised effect. That is, in each experiment, exposure to a stressor interfered with, rather than enhanced, long-term memory for associated material. We conclude that the relationship between stress and memory consolidation is more bounded than previously believed.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta , Memoria a Largo Plazo , Estrés Fisiológico , Adolescente , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Dimensión del Dolor , Estimulación Luminosa , Saliva/metabolismo , Caracteres Sexuales , Adulto Joven
3.
Psychol Sci ; 23(12): 1506-14, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23129060

RESUMEN

Purity is commonly regarded as being physically embodied in the color white, with even trivial deviations from whiteness indicating a loss of purity. In three studies, we explored the implications of this "white = pure" association for disgust, an emotion that motivates the detection and avoidance of impurities that threaten purity and cleanliness. We hypothesized that disgust tunes perception to prioritize the light end of the light-dark spectrum, which results in a relative hypersensitivity to changes in lightness in this range. In studies 1 and 2, greater sensitivity to disgusting stimuli was associated with greater ability to make subtle gray-scale discriminations (e.g., detecting a faint gray stimulus against a white background) at the light end of the spectrum relative to ability to make subtle gray-scale discriminations at the dark end of the spectrum. In study 3, after viewing disgusting images, disgust-sensitive individuals demonstrated a heightened ability to detect deviations from white. These findings suggest that disgust not only motivates people to avoid impurities, but actually makes them better able to see them.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Adulto , Discriminación en Psicología , Miedo/fisiología , Miedo/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
4.
Cogn Emot ; 26(4): 680-9, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22077678

RESUMEN

Research in South Korea and the United States examined how affective states facilitate or inhibit culturally dominant styles of reasoning. According to the affect-as-information hypothesis, affective cues of mood influence judgements by serving as embodied information about the value of accessible inclinations and cognitions. Extending this line of research to culture, we hypothesised that positive affect should promote (and negative affect should inhibit) culturally normative reasoning. The results of two studies of causal reasoning supported this hypothesis. Positive and negative affect functioned like "go" and "stop" signals, respectively, for culturally typical reasoning styles. Thus, in happy (compared to sad) moods, Koreans engaged in more holistic reasoning, whereas Americans engaged in more analytic reasoning.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Comparación Transcultural , Inhibición Psicológica , Pensamiento , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , República de Corea , Estados Unidos
5.
Psychol Sci ; 27(10): 1407-1409, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27582040
6.
Psychol Sci ; 20(8): 1019-25, 2009 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19619180

RESUMEN

Three studies examined automatic associations between words with moral and immoral meanings and the colors black and white. The speed of color naming in a Stroop task was faster when words in black concerned immorality (e.g., greed), rather than morality, and when words in white concerned morality (e.g., honesty), rather than immorality. In addition, priming immorality by having participants hand-copy an unethical statement speeded identification of words in the black font. Making immorality salient in this way also increased the moral Stroop effect among participants who had not previously shown it. In the final study, participants also rated consumer products. Moral meanings interfered with color naming most strongly among those participants who rated personal cleaning products as especially desirable. The moderation of the moral Stroop effect by individual differences in concerns about personal cleanliness suggests that ideas about purity and pollution are central to seeing morality in black and white.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación , Percepción de Color , Principios Morales , Religión y Psicología , Semántica , Adulto , Conflicto Psicológico , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Metáfora , Tiempo de Reacción , Deseabilidad Social , Test de Stroop
7.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 35(7): 909-22, 2009 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19487484

RESUMEN

We investigated whether the desire to have a smooth and pleasant interaction with an anticipated interaction partner caused participants' moods to become similar to their imminent partners' moods. We found evidence of anticipatory mood matching when participants were motivated to affiliate with a partner through goal priming (Experiments 1 and 2) and outcome dependency (Experiment 3). Prior research has demonstrated mood contagion arising from actual social interaction but these experiments establish contagion without contact, an outcome evident regardless of whether mood was assessed via self-report (Experiments 1 through 3) or information-processing style (Experiment 3).


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Relaciones Interpersonales , Motivación , Percepción Social , Concienciación , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio , Masculino , Conducta Social , Medio Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
8.
Cogn Syst Res ; 10(1): 21-30, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19255620

RESUMEN

Emotions and moods color cognition. In this article, we outline how emotions affect judgments and cognitive performance of human agents. We argue that affective influences are due, not to the affective reactions themselves, but to the information they carry about value, a potentially useful finding for creators of artificial agents. The kind of influence that occurs depends on the focus of the agent at the time. When making evaluative judgments, for example, agents may experience positive affect as a positive attitude toward a person or object. But when an agent focuses on a cognitive task, positive affect may act like performance feedback, with positive affect giving a green light to cognitive, relational processes. By contrast, negative affect tends to inhibit relational processing, resulting in a more perceptual, stimulus-specific processing. One result is that many textbook phenomena from cognitive psychology occur readily in happy moods, but are inhibited in sad moods.

9.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 11(9): 393-9, 2007 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17698405

RESUMEN

Being happy or sad influences the content and style of thought. One explanation is that affect serves as information about the value of whatever comes to mind. Thus, when a person makes evaluative judgments or engages in a task, positive affect can enhance evaluations and empower potential responses. Rather than affect itself, the information conveyed by affect is crucial. Tests of the hypothesis find that affective influences can be made to disappear by changing the source to which the affect is attributed. In tasks, positive affect validates and negative affect invalidates accessible cognitions, leading to relational processing and item-specific processing, respectively. Positive affect is found to promote, and negative affect to inhibit, many textbook phenomena from cognitive psychology.


Asunto(s)
Afecto/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Humanos
10.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 94(4): 560-78, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18361672

RESUMEN

In five studies, the authors examined the effects on cognitive performance of coherence and incoherence between conceptual and experiential sources of affective information. The studies crossed the priming of happy and sad concepts with affective experiences. In different experiments, these included approach or avoidance actions, happy or sad feelings, and happy or sad expressive behaviors. In all studies, coherence between affective concepts and affective experiences led to better recall of a story than did affective incoherence. The authors suggest that the experience of such experiential affective cues serves as evidence of the appropriateness of affective concepts that come to mind. The results suggest that affective coherence has epistemic benefits and that incoherence is costly in terms of cognitive performance.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Formación de Concepto , Conflicto Psicológico , Recuerdo Mental , Lectura , Atención , Comprensión , Señales (Psicología) , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Motivación , Retención en Psicología
11.
Emotion ; 8(2): 208-15, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18410195

RESUMEN

Semantic and affective priming are classic effects observed in cognitive and social psychology, respectively. The authors discovered that affect regulates such priming effects. In Experiment 1, positive and negative moods were induced before one of three priming tasks; evaluation, categorization, or lexical decision. As predicted, positive affect led to both affective priming (evaluation task) and semantic priming (category and lexical decision tasks). However, negative affect inhibited such effects. In Experiment 2, participants in their natural affective state completed the same priming tasks as in Experiment 1. As expected, affective priming (evaluation task) and category priming (categorization and lexical decision tasks) were observed in such resting affective states. Hence, the authors conclude that negative affect inhibits semantic and affective priming. These results support recent theoretical models, which suggest that positive affect promotes associations among strong and weak concepts, and that negative affect impairs such associations (Clore & Storbeck, 2006; Kuhl, 2000).


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Aprendizaje por Asociación , Inhibición Psicológica , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje por Asociación de Pares , Semántica , Atención , Toma de Decisiones , Humanos , Juicio , Tiempo de Reacción
12.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 34(8): 1096-109, 2008 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18505801

RESUMEN

How, and for whom, does disgust influence moral judgment? In four experiments participants made moral judgments while experiencing extraneous feelings of disgust. Disgust was induced in Experiment 1 by exposure to a bad smell, in Experiment 2 by working in a disgusting room, in Experiment 3 by recalling a physically disgusting experience, and in Experiment 4 through a video induction. In each case, the results showed that disgust can increase the severity of moral judgments relative to controls. Experiment 4 found that disgust had a different effect on moral judgment than did sadness. In addition, Experiments 2-4 showed that the role of disgust in severity of moral judgments depends on participants' sensitivity to their own bodily sensations. Taken together, these data indicate the importance-and specificity-of gut feelings in moral judgments.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Juicio , Principios Morales , Percepción Social , Adulto , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
13.
Curr Opin Behav Sci ; 19: 78-82, 2018 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30271831

RESUMEN

Affect and its object are separable, so that the same affective reaction can have different effects. Relevant principles from the affect-as-information approach include: (1) The impact of affect depends on implicit attributions -- what it appears to be about. (2) Affect is always taken to be about whatever is currently mentally accessible. Affective reactions can therefore serve as appraisals of objects of judgment or of initial thoughts and opinions about such objects, when they are more accessible. During problem solving, affect can serve as appraisals of thought style rather than thought content. Then, (3) positive and negative affect serve as go and stop signals for current inclinations. Affective influences on cognition are therefore not fixed, but malleable and context-dependent.

14.
Cogn Emot ; 21(6): 1212-1237, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18458789

RESUMEN

Affect and cognition have long been treated as independent entities, but in the current review we suggest that affect and cognition are in fact highly interdependent. We open the article by discussing three classic views for the independence of affect. These are (i) the affective independence hypothesis, that emotion is processed independently from cognition, (ii) the affective primacy hypothesis, that evaluative processing precedes semantic processing, and (iii) the affective automaticity hypothesis, that affectively potent stimuli commandeer attention and evaluation is automatic. We argue that affect is not independent from cognition, that affect is not primary to cognition, nor is affect automatically elicited. The second half of the paper discusses several instances of how affect influences cognition. We review experiments showing affective involvement in perception, semantic activation, and attitude activation. We conclude that one function of affect is to regulate cognitive processing.

15.
Soc Psychol Q ; 70(4): 333-339, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18461152

RESUMEN

The recent publication of David Heise's Expressive Order (2007) provides an occasion for discussing some of the key ideas in Affect Control Theory. The theory proposes that a few dimensions of affective meaning provide a common basis for interrelating personal identities and social actions. It holds that during interpersonal interactions, social behavior is continually regulated to maintain an affective tone compatible with whatever social roles or identities define the situation. We outline the intellectual history of the proposed dimensions and of the idea that each social action invites an action from the other that has a particular location along these dimensions. We also relate these ideas to the Affect-as-Information hypothesis, an approach that often guides research in psychology on the role of affect in regulating judgment and thought.

16.
Psychol Bull ; 128(6): 934-60, 2002 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12405138

RESUMEN

This review organizes a variety of phenomena related to emotional self-report. In doing so, the authors offer an accessibility model that specifies the types of factors that contribute to emotional self-reports under different reporting conditions. One important distinction is between emotion, which is episodic, experiential, and contextual, and beliefs about emotion, which are semantic, conceptual, and decontextualized. This distinction is important in understanding the discrepancies that often occur when people are asked to report on feelings they are currently experiencing versus those that they are not currently experiencing. The accessibility model provides an organizing framework for understanding self-reports of emotion and suggests some new directions for research.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Cultura , Autoevaluación (Psicología) , Humanos , Memoria , Semántica , Estereotipo
17.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 83(1): 198-215, 2002 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12088126

RESUMEN

Three studies involving 3 participant samples (Ns = 39, 55, and 53) tested the hypothesis that people retrieve episodic emotion knowledge when reporting on their emotions over short (e.g., last few hours) time frames, but that they retrieve semantic emotion knowledge when reporting on their emotions over long (e.g., last few months) time frames. Support for 2 distinct judgment strategies was based on judgment latencies (Studies 1 and 2) and priming paradigms (Studies 2 and 3). The authors suggest that self-reports of emotion over short versus long time frames assess qualitatively different sources of self-knowledge.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Cognición , Juicio , Autoimagen , Semántica , Adulto , Humanos
18.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 83(3): 663-77, 2002 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12219861

RESUMEN

One must consider both trait and state affect to predict individual differences in emotional processing. The present results document a novel trait-state interaction that is consistent with proposals concerning the epistemic functions of affect (A. R. Damasio, 1994). Four studies tested the effects of extraversion and mood on motivation-relevant processing. Study 1 measured naturally occurring mood, whereas Studies 2-4 manipulated mood. Extraverts were faster to link events to their personal motivations when in a positive mood state, whereas introverts were faster to do so in a neutral or negative mood state. Further findings indicate that this interaction affects attitude accessibility rather than event elaboration. Overall, the authors suggest that there are pragmatic benefits to trait-consistent moods, particularly for processing motivation-relevant stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Conducta de Elección , Extraversión Psicológica , Análisis de Varianza , Humanos , Illinois , Modelos Psicológicos , Motivación , Tiempo de Reacción , Análisis de Regresión
19.
Emotion ; 4(2): 139-44; discussion 151-5, 2004 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15222850

RESUMEN

Does simple displeasure cause anger without appraisals or agency attributions? The authors offer 8 observations: (a) Appraisal theory also predicts that displeasure promotes anger, (b) An emotion of frustration can be usefully distinguished from anger, (c) Aggressive reactions to norm violations among animals suggest that they too distinguish bad behavior from bad outcomes, (d) Attributions to agency are perceptual and automatic in social situations, (e) It is tenuous to argue that agency attributions are enacted in angry aggression, but absent in anger elicitation. (f) The contextualized meanings of expressive movements, rather than movements themselves, elicit emotion, (g) Expressions may be better seen as constituents than as causes of emotions, (h) Cognitive components of emotion generally come before, not after, eliciting events.


Asunto(s)
Ira , Conducta Social , Agresión/psicología , Cognición , Humanos
20.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 30(2): 237-49, 2004 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15030636

RESUMEN

In three studies, the authors show that unconscious expressive cues can lead to opposite evaluations, depending on the context in which they occur. In Study 1, brow (vs. cheek) tension reduced preferences in an easy judgment context but increased preferences in a difficult context. In Study 2, head shaking (vs. nodding) either increased or decreased prosocial affect depending on the context in which the judged character was presented. In Study 3, a subliminal smile (vs. frown) led to higher self-ratings of performance when paired with one's own actions but to lower self-ratings of performance when paired with a competitor's actions. Together, these results suggest that the meaning of unconscious expressive cues is not fixed.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Cabeza , Movimiento , Inconsciente en Psicología , Afecto , Humanos , Juicio , Postura , Grabación de Cinta de Video , Percepción Visual
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA