Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 22
Filtrar
1.
Psychophysiology ; 61(6): e14536, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38323360

RESUMEN

The present research tested the effect of manipulated perceived control (over obtaining the outcomes) and effort on reward valuation using the event-related potential known as the Reward Positivity (RewP). This test was conducted in an attempt to integrate two research literatures with opposite findings: Effort justification occurs when high effort leads to high reward valuation, whereas effort discounting occurs when high effort leads to low reward valuation. Based on an examination of past methods used in these literatures, we predicted that perceived control and effort would interactively influence RewP. Consistent with the effort justification literature (cognitive dissonance theory), when individuals have high perceived control, high effort should lead to more reward valuation than low effort should. Consistent with the effort discounting literature, when individuals have low perceived control, low effort should lead to more reward valuation than high effort should. Results supported these interactive and integrative predictions.


Asunto(s)
Disonancia Cognitiva , Recompensa , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Teoría Psicológica , Adolescente
2.
Behav Brain Sci ; 43: e38, 2020 04 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32292158

RESUMEN

The action-based model of cognitive dissonance proposes an adaptive function for rationalization that differs from the one offered by Cushman. The one proposed by Cushman is concerned more with the cold construction of cognitions, whereas the one proposed by the action-based model is a motivated protection of a strongly held cognition.


Asunto(s)
Disonancia Cognitiva , Racionalización , Cognición
3.
Cogn Emot ; 31(6): 1153-1168, 2017 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27380127

RESUMEN

Skin-transmitted pathogens have threatened humans since ancient times. We investigated whether skin-transmitted pathogens were a subclass of disgust stimuli that evoked an emotional response that was related to, but distinct from, disgust and fear. We labelled this response "the heebie jeebies". In Study 1, coding of 76 participants' experiences of disgust, fear, and the heebie jeebies showed that the heebie jeebies was elicited by unique stimuli which produced skin-crawling sensations and an urge to protect the skin. In Experiment 2,350 participants' responses to skin-transmitted pathogen, fear-inducing, and disgust-inducing vignettes showed that the vignettes elicited sensations and urges which loaded onto heebie jeebies, fear, and disgust factors, respectively. Experiment 3 largely replicated findings from Experiment 2 using video stimuli (178 participants). Results are consistent with the notion that skin-transmitted pathogens are a subclass of disgust stimuli which motivate behaviours that are functionally consistent with disgust yet qualitatively distinct.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades Transmisibles/psicología , Emociones , Piel , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
4.
PLoS One ; 17(10): e0275990, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36228024

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: The present research was designed to test predictions derived from the action-based model of cognitive dissonance theory. These predictions were that dissonance arousal would be negatively related to effective behavior, and that dissonance reduction would be positively related to effective behavior. METHOD: Dissonance arousal and reduction were measured using an individual differences questionnaire. Effective behavior was measured as amount of physical exercise obtained from an exercise app that measures exercise using GPS (cycling kilometers over one year; Study 1) and from self-reports (number of days during the previous week; Study 2-3). RESULTS: Results suggested that individual differences in dissonance arousal relate to less exercise and that individual differences in dissonance reduction relate to more exercise. Statistically controlling for trait approach and avoidance motivation as well as satisfaction with life revealed that dissonance processes predicted exercise behavior over these traits. This pattern of results was generally consistent across the three studies. Moreover, results from Studies 2-3 suggested possible statistical mediators from the exercise commitment literature of the relationship between trait dissonance arousal/reduction and exercise behavior. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION: These results highlight the importance of considering dissonance processes as adaptive ones, and they suggest possible ways of increasing exercise behavior.


Asunto(s)
Disonancia Cognitiva , Individualidad , Nivel de Alerta , Ejercicio Físico , Motivación
5.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 14: 157, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32477082

RESUMEN

The present research was designed to test whether the subjective experience of more effort related to more reward valuation as measured by a neural response. This prediction was derived from the theory of cognitive dissonance and its effort justification paradigm. Young adult participants (n = 82) engaged in multiple trails of a low or high effort task that resulted in a loss or reward on each trial. Neural responses to the reward (loss) cue were measured using EEG so that the event-related potential known as the Reward Positivity (RewP) could be assessed. Results revealed no significant differences between low and high effort conditions on the RewP. However, within the high effort condition, a more subjective experience of effort was associated with a larger RewP. This research extends past research on the effort justification paradigm of cognitive dissonance theory by suggesting that effort justification is associated with an implicit measure of reward valuation. It, therefore, challenges recent perspectives on dissonance processes that posit that these evaluative changes should only occur on explicit but not implicit measures.

6.
Biol Psychol ; 154: 107910, 2020 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32473260

RESUMEN

Past psychological theory and research suggest two opposing relationships between effort and reward valuation. Cognitive dissonance theory and research suggest that increased effort is associated with increased reward valuation, whereas theory and research on effort discounting suggest that increased effort is associated with decreased reward valuation. The present study was designed to test these two opposing perspectives, by examining a potential moderator of the relationship between effort and reward valuation - the belief that the effort was necessary to obtain the reward. Results (n = 44) suggested that increased perceptions of effort following a task were associated with larger neural responses to rewards (as assessed by the event-related potential referred to as the reward positivity) when individuals believed that their effort led to the reward but not when they believed that their effort did not lead to the reward. Discussion considers the implications of these results.


Asunto(s)
Teoría Psicológica , Recompensa , Toma de Decisiones , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
7.
Emotion ; 9(2): 183-96, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19348531

RESUMEN

As a prototypic negative emotion, anger would seem to have little in common with positive activation, as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; D. Watson, L. A. Clark, & A. Tellegen, 1988). However, growing evidence suggests that both anger and positive affect are associated with approach motivation. This suggests the counterintuitive hypotheses that positive affect should be increased by anger-evoking situations, and that positive affect and anger should be directly correlated in such situations. Four studies tested and supported these hypotheses. Discussion focuses on the implications of these results.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Ira , Motivación , Adolescente , Adulto , Emociones , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Análisis Multivariante , Pruebas Psicológicas , Análisis de Regresión , Estados Unidos
8.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 117(6): 1189-1202, 2019 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30843724

RESUMEN

Four studies examined whether pain offset reduces rumination in response to anger or sadness. Past research has demonstrated that, following the offset of pain, individuals show a distinct state of relief involving both reduction in negative affect and an increase in positive affect. This response may help to explain why people sometimes seek out pain and discomfort (e.g., vigorous exercise, self-harm) to regulate negative emotion and suggests that following pain people should recover better from negative emotional states. To test this, we examined ruminative responses to anger and sadness. These negative, approach-related emotions often produce rumination; a response that is generally considered maladaptive. In Study 1, pain was manipulated through a cold pressor task, and participants were induced to experience anger through autobiographical recall. In Study 2, pain was also manipulated pain via a cold pressor task, and anger and sadness were induced through social exclusion using the Cyberball paradigm. In Study 3, pain was manipulated by squeezing exercise handgrips, and sadness was induced with imagery from a sad video. Study 4 replicated the methods of Study 3 and added measures of relief and distraction to examine whether these moderated the effect. A minimeta-analysis showed that, across all studies, individuals engaged in less rumination in the pain conditions as measured by a thought-listing task and a self-reported rumination questionnaire. These results suggest that the regulation of anger and sadness are improved following pain offset. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Ira/fisiología , Dolor/psicología , Rumiación Cognitiva/fisiología , Tristeza/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
9.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 94(1): 1-15, 2008 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18179314

RESUMEN

The action-based model of dissonance predicts that following decisional commitment, approach-oriented motivational processes occur to assist in translating the decision into effective and unconflicted behavior. Therefore, the modulation of these approach-oriented processes should affect the degree to which individuals change their attitudes to be more consistent with the decisional commitment (spreading of alternatives). Experiment 1 demonstrated that a neurofeedback-induced decrease in relative left frontal cortical activation, which has been implicated in approach motivational processes, caused a reduction in spreading of alternatives. Experiment 2 manipulated an action-oriented mindset following a decision and demonstrated that the action-oriented mindset caused increased activation in the left frontal cortical region as well as increased spreading of alternatives. Discussion focuses on how this integration of neuroscience and dissonance theory benefits both parent literatures.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Disonancia Cognitiva , Toma de Decisiones , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Actitud , Biorretroalimentación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos
10.
Front Psychol ; 9: 2665, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30671003

RESUMEN

Past theory and research have suggested that motivationally intense affective states narrow cognitive scope. Research has also suggested manipulations that broaden cognitive scope reduce responses to appetitive positive affective stimuli and disgusting stimuli, thus suggesting that cognitive broadening reduces motivational intensity. This led to the hypothesis that cognitive broadening would reduce the approach-motivated negative emotion of anger. Seven studies assessed the effect of cognitive broadening on reported trait anger, state anger, attitudes toward anger, attributions of anger to ambiguous pictures, and accessibility of aggressive words. Results from individual studies found mixed support for these predictions. A meta-analysis, however, suggested a small but significant effect on trait anger/aggression and attitudes toward anger across studies. These results may indicate that cognitive scope, as manipulated in these studies, has a small effect on anger-related responses. Discussion speculates on potential explanations of these findings, and their importance for informing future research.

11.
Behav Sci (Basel) ; 7(4)2017 Sep 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28961185

RESUMEN

We review research on the structure and functions of emotions that has benefitted from a serious consideration of both discrete and dimensional perspectives on emotion. To illustrate this point, we review research that demonstrates: (1) how affective valence within discrete emotions differs as a function of individuals and situations, and how these differences relate to various functions; (2) that anger (and other emotional states) should be considered as a discrete emotion but there are dimensions around and within anger; (3) that similarities exist between approach-related positive and negative discrete emotions and they have unique motivational functions; (4) that discrete emotions and broad dimensions of emotions both have unique functions; and (5) evidence that a "new" discrete emotion with discrete functions exists within a broader emotion family. We hope that this consideration of both discrete and dimensional perspectives on emotion will assist in understanding the functions of emotions.

12.
Psychophysiology ; 54(2): 310-322, 2017 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28118688

RESUMEN

The reward positivity (RewP) and the stimulus preceding negativity (SPN), two ERPs associated with reward delivery and reward anticipation, are modulated by motivational intensity. Motivational intensity is the effort organisms would make to exert behaviors, and it varies with the difficulty of exerting that behavior. If a task is perceived as impossible, which means that one does not have control over own outcomes, motivational intensity is low. In the current study, we tested the prediction that perceiving control over one's outcomes increases both the RewP to feedback and the SPN prior to feedback compared to perceiving no control. We also examined whether P300 and LPP amplitudes to reward and nonreward images were similarly modulated. Twenty-five female participants completed a gambling task in which correct choices were followed by pictures of attractive men and incorrect choices were followed by pictures of rocks. To manipulate perceived control, participants were told that, in one block of trials, they could learn a mouse-click rule in order to see only pictures of men (high perceived control condition), while in the other block, the pictures would appear randomly (low perceived control condition). However, in both conditions, feedback appeared randomly. Although the RewP was elicited in both blocks, the RewP and SPN were higher in the high perceived control condition (i.e., when participants thought that they could influence their outcomes). Perceived control did not modulate the P300 and LPP to pictures. The results suggest that approach motivation and its intensity modulate the processing of performance feedback.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados , Motivación/fisiología , Recompensa , Autoeficacia , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Relacionados con Evento P300 , Femenino , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología
13.
Emotion ; 16(7): 1086-96, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27685155

RESUMEN

Psychological research often yields null results on self-reported emotion as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), even when using manipulations that might intuitively be expected to be emotionally impactful. Three studies reported here support the hypothesis that changes in self-reported negative emotion may be detected more sensitively when discrete emotions are measured rather than by either PANAS NA or a measure created by combining discrete emotions, and when participants were instructed to report how they felt during an emotion-eliciting event versus how they felt afterward. In Study 1, emotion was manipulated with disgusting photographs, in Study 2, with recall of social exclusion/inclusion, and in Study 3, with reminders of personal mortality. Discussion focuses on implications for detecting emotional changes in psychological research, and the inadvisability of interpreting null results on an insensitive measure as indicating that emotional changes did not occur. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Autoinforme , Adulto Joven
14.
PLoS One ; 11(8): e0159915, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27500829

RESUMEN

Several discrete emotions have broad theoretical and empirical importance, as shown by converging evidence from diverse areas of psychology, including facial displays, developmental behaviors, and neuroscience. However, the measurement of these states has not progressed along with theory, such that when researchers measure subjectively experienced emotions, they commonly rely on scales assessing broad dimensions of affect (positivity and negativity), rather than discrete emotions. The current manuscript presents four studies that validate a new instrument, the Discrete Emotions Questionnaire (DEQ), that is sensitive to eight distinct state emotions: anger, disgust, fear, anxiety, sadness, happiness, relaxation, and desire. Emotion theory supporting the importance of distinguishing these specific emotions is reviewed.


Asunto(s)
Ira/fisiología , Ansiedad/psicología , Emociones/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Autoinforme , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Felicidad , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Teóricos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
15.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 82(4): 610-8, 2002 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11999927

RESUMEN

The behavioral approach system (BAS) reflects the propensity to respond to signals of reward, including stimuli associated with safety and goal-oriented attack (e.g., anger). Hypomania/mania has been posited to involve increased BAS activity. In contrast, depression has been posited to involve decreased BAS activity. Building on past research, which suggests that increased left frontal cortical activity is a neurophysiological index of BAS activity, the present research tested the hypotheses that proneness toward hypomania/mania symptoms would be related to increased relative left frontal activity and that proneness toward depression symptoms would be related to decreased relative left frontal activity in response to an anger-evoking event. Results from 67 individuals who had completed a measure of proneness toward these affective symptoms and were exposed to an anger-evoking event supported the hypotheses.


Asunto(s)
Ira/fisiología , Trastorno Bipolar/diagnóstico , Trastorno Depresivo/diagnóstico , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Motivación , Trastorno Bipolar/fisiopatología , Trastorno Depresivo/fisiopatología , Susceptibilidad a Enfermedades , Electroencefalografía/estadística & datos numéricos , Electrooculografía/estadística & datos numéricos , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Recompensa , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
16.
Emotion ; 4(1): 95-101, 2004 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15053729

RESUMEN

The present research extended past research demonstrating that approach-motivated anger is associated with greater left than right frontal cortical activity. Because past research had examined difference scores between left and right frontal activity, it was unable to test whether approach-motivated anger increased left activity, decreased right activity, or both. In addition, the present research examined a potential moderator of the effect of insult on left frontal activity. That is, it tested whether sympathy for an insulting person would reduce the left frontal activity that occurs following being insulted. Results indicated that left frontal activity was increased and right frontal activity was decreased by the insult. Moreover, these effects were inhibited when high levels of sympathy were first aroused.


Asunto(s)
Ira , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Empatía , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino
17.
Cogn Emot ; 17(1): 1-24, 2003 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29715737

RESUMEN

The present research tested whether relative left midfrontal cortical activity would be greater when individuals believed they could engage in behaviour to ameliorate an anger-inducing situation as compared to when they believed they could do nothing. The research also examined whether relative left midfrontal cortical activity would relate to behaviors aimed at ameliorating the anger-inducing situation, when individuals believed they could do something. To test these primary hypotheses, university students who were opposed to a tuition increase at their university were exposed to a pilot radio editorial that argued for increased tuition, and their electroencephalographic (EEG) activity, self-reported emotions, and behavioural responses were then collected. Prior to hearing the editorial, participants were led to believe that the tuition increase would definitely occur or that the tuition increase may occur and that petitions were being circulated to attempt to prevent it. Results supported the primary hypotheses and also revealed that relative left midfrontal activity was associated with anger in response to the editorial and that the effects were specific to the midfrontal cortical regions.

18.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 100(1): 172-81, 2011 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20853981

RESUMEN

In this set of studies, we examine the perceptual similarities between emotions that share either a valence or a motivational direction. Determination is a positive approach-related emotion, whereas anger is a negative approach-related emotion. Thus, determination and anger share a motivational direction but are opposite in valence. An implemental mind-set has previously been shown to produce high-approach-motivated positive affect. Thus, in Study 1, participants were asked to freely report the strongest emotion they experienced during an implemental mind-set. The most common emotion reported was determination. On the basis of this result, we compared the facial expression of determination with that of anger. In Study 2, naive judges were asked to identify photographs of facial expressions intended to express determination, along with photographs intended to express basic emotions (joy, anger, sadness, fear, disgust, neutral). Correct identifications of intended determination expressions were correlated with misidentifications of the expressions as anger but not with misidentifications as any other emotion. This suggests that determination, a high-approach-motivated positive affect, is perceived as similar to anger. In Study 3, naive judges quantified the intensity of joy, anger, and determination expressed in photographs. The intensity of perceived determination was directly correlated with the intensity of perceived anger (a high-approach-motivated negative affect) and was inversely correlated with the intensity of perceived joy (a low-approach-motivated positive affect). These results demonstrate perceptual similarity between emotions that share a motivational direction but differ in valence.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Ira , Motivación , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
19.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 37(3): 395-408, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21307178

RESUMEN

The action-based model of dissonance and recent advances in neuroscience suggest that commitment to action should cause greater relative left frontal cortical activity. Two experiments were conducted in which electroencephalographic activity was recorded following commitment to action, operationalized with a perceived choice manipulation. Perceived high as compared to low choice to engage in the action, regardless of whether it was counterattitudinal or proattitudinal, caused greater relative left frontal cortical activity. Moreover, perceived high as compared to low choice caused attitudes to be more consistent with the action. These results broaden the theoretical reach of the action-based model by suggesting that similar neural and motivational processes are involved in attitudinal responses to counterattitudinal and proattitudinal commitments.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Disonancia Cognitiva , Electroencefalografía/psicología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Motivación , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
20.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 101(6): 1332-50, 2011 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21843012

RESUMEN

The present work outlines a theory of attitudes toward emotions, provides a measure of attitudes toward emotions, and then tests several predictions concerning relationships between attitudes toward specific emotions and emotional situation selection, emotional traits, emotional reactivity, and emotion regulation. The present conceptualization of individual differences in attitudes toward emotions focuses on specific emotions and presents data indicating that 5 emotions (anger, sadness, joy, fear, and disgust) load on 5 separate attitude factors (Study 1). Attitudes toward emotions predicted emotional situation selection (Study 2). Moreover, attitudes toward approach emotions (e.g., anger, joy) correlated directly with the associated trait emotions, whereas attitudes toward withdrawal emotions (fear, disgust) correlated inversely with associated trait emotions (Study 3). Similar results occurred when attitudes toward emotions were used to predict state emotional reactivity (Study 4). Finally, attitudes toward emotions predicted specific forms of emotion regulation (Study 5).


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Emociones , Análisis de Varianza , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivación , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Distribución por Sexo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA