Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 26
Filtrar
1.
Cell ; 183(2): 537-548.e12, 2020 10 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33064989

RESUMEN

Sequential activation of neurons has been observed during various behavioral and cognitive processes, but the underlying circuit mechanisms remain poorly understood. Here, we investigate premotor sequences in HVC (proper name) of the adult zebra finch forebrain that are central to the performance of the temporally precise courtship song. We use high-density silicon probes to measure song-related population activity, and we compare these observations with predictions from a range of network models. Our results support a circuit architecture in which heterogeneous delays between sequentially active neurons shape the spatiotemporal patterns of HVC premotor neuron activity. We gauge the impact of several delay sources, and we find the primary contributor to be slow conduction through axonal collaterals within HVC, which typically adds between 1 and 7.5 ms for each link within the sequence. Thus, local axonal "delay lines" can play an important role in determining the dynamical repertoire of neural circuits.


Asunto(s)
Pinzones/fisiología , Prosencéfalo/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Comunicación Animal , Animales , Axones , Masculino , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología
2.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 17(3): e1008824, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33730085

RESUMEN

During development, neurons arrive at local brain areas in an extended period of time, but how they form local neural circuits is unknown. Here we computationally model the emergence of a network for precise timing in the premotor nucleus HVC in songbird. We show that new projection neurons, added to HVC post hatch at early stages of song development, are recruited to the end of a growing feedforward network. High spontaneous activity of the new neurons makes them the prime targets for recruitment in a self-organized process via synaptic plasticity. Once recruited, the new neurons fire readily at precise times, and they become mature. Neurons that are not recruited become silent and replaced by new immature neurons. Our model incorporates realistic HVC features such as interneurons, spatial distributions of neurons, and distributed axonal delays. The model predicts that the birth order of the projection neurons correlates with their burst timing during the song.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Neurológicos , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Animales , Axones/fisiología , Biología Computacional , Pinzones/fisiología , Interneuronas/fisiología , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Vocalización Animal/fisiología
3.
J Neurosci ; 37(10): 2600-2611, 2017 03 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28159910

RESUMEN

Variable motor sequences of animals are often structured and can be described by probabilistic transition rules between action elements. Examples include the songs of many songbird species such as the Bengalese finch, which consist of stereotypical syllables sequenced according to probabilistic rules (song syntax). The neural mechanisms behind such rules are poorly understood. Here, we investigate where the song syntax is encoded in the brain of the Bengalese finch by rapidly and reversibly manipulating the temperature in the song production pathway. Cooling the premotor nucleus HVC (proper name) slows down the song tempo, consistent with the idea that HVC controls moment-to-moment timings of acoustic features in the syllables. More importantly, cooling HVC alters the transition probabilities between syllables. Cooling HVC reduces the number of repetitions of long-repeated syllables and increases the randomness of syllable sequences. In contrast, cooling the downstream motor area RA (robust nucleus of the acropallium), which is critical for singing, does not affect the song syntax. Unilateral cooling of HVC shows that control of syllables is mostly lateralized to the left HVC, whereas transition probabilities between the syllables can be affected by cooling HVC in either hemisphere to varying degrees. These results show that HVC is a key site for encoding song syntax in the Bengalese finch. HVC is thus involved both in encoding timings within syllables and in sequencing probabilistic transitions between syllables. Our finding suggests that probabilistic selections and fine-grained timings of action elements can be integrated within the same neural circuits.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Many animal behaviors such as birdsong consist of variable sequences of discrete actions. Where and how the probabilistic rules of such sequences are encoded in the brain is poorly understood. We locally and reversibly cooled brain areas in songbirds during singing. Mild cooling of area HVC in the Bengalese finch brain-a premotor area homologous to the mammalian premotor cortex-alters the statistics of the syllable sequences, suggesting that HVC is critical for birdsong sequences. HVC is also known for controlling moment-to-moment timings within syllables. Our results show that timing and probabilistic sequencing of actions can share the same neural circuits in local brain areas.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Pinzones/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Animales , Vías Eferentes/fisiología , Masculino , Semántica
4.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 11(10): e1004471, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26448054

RESUMEN

Consecutive repetition of actions is common in behavioral sequences. Although integration of sensory feedback with internal motor programs is important for sequence generation, if and how feedback contributes to repetitive actions is poorly understood. Here we study how auditory feedback contributes to generating repetitive syllable sequences in songbirds. We propose that auditory signals provide positive feedback to ongoing motor commands, but this influence decays as feedback weakens from response adaptation during syllable repetitions. Computational models show that this mechanism explains repeat distributions observed in Bengalese finch song. We experimentally confirmed two predictions of this mechanism in Bengalese finches: removal of auditory feedback by deafening reduces syllable repetitions; and neural responses to auditory playback of repeated syllable sequences gradually adapt in sensory-motor nucleus HVC. Together, our results implicate a positive auditory-feedback loop with adaptation in generating repetitive vocalizations, and suggest sensory adaptation is important for feedback control of motor sequences.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Corteza Auditiva/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Pájaros Cantores/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Animales , Vías Auditivas/fisiología , Simulación por Computador , Vías Eferentes/fisiología , Retroalimentación Fisiológica/fisiología , Masculino , Movimiento/fisiología
5.
Nature ; 468(7322): 394-9, 2010 Nov 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20972420

RESUMEN

In songbirds, the remarkable temporal precision of song is generated by a sparse sequence of bursts in the premotor nucleus HVC. To distinguish between two possible classes of models of neural sequence generation, we carried out intracellular recordings of HVC neurons in singing zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). We found that the subthreshold membrane potential is characterized by a large, rapid depolarization 5-10 ms before burst onset, consistent with a synaptically connected chain of neurons in HVC. We found no evidence for the slow membrane potential modulation predicted by models in which burst timing is controlled by subthreshold dynamics. Furthermore, bursts ride on an underlying depolarization of ∼10-ms duration, probably the result of a regenerative calcium spike within HVC neurons that could facilitate the propagation of activity through a chain network with high temporal precision. Our results provide insight into the fundamental mechanisms by which neural circuits can generate complex sequential behaviours.


Asunto(s)
Pinzones/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Neuronas/metabolismo , Sinapsis/metabolismo , Animales , Canales de Calcio Tipo L/metabolismo , Señalización del Calcio/efectos de los fármacos , Masculino , Potenciales de la Membrana/efectos de los fármacos , Vías Nerviosas/efectos de los fármacos , Neuronas/efectos de los fármacos , Sueño/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología
6.
Proc Biol Sci ; 281(1792)2014 Oct 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25143037

RESUMEN

Many animals produce vocal sequences that appear complex. Most researchers assume that these sequences are well characterized as Markov chains (i.e. that the probability of a particular vocal element can be calculated from the history of only a finite number of preceding elements). However, this assumption has never been explicitly tested. Furthermore, it is unclear how language could evolve in a single step from a Markovian origin, as is frequently assumed, as no intermediate forms have been found between animal communication and human language. Here, we assess whether animal taxa produce vocal sequences that are better described by Markov chains, or by non-Markovian dynamics such as the 'renewal process' (RP), characterized by a strong tendency to repeat elements. We examined vocal sequences of seven taxa: Bengalese finches Lonchura striata domestica, Carolina chickadees Poecile carolinensis, free-tailed bats Tadarida brasiliensis, rock hyraxes Procavia capensis, pilot whales Globicephala macrorhynchus, killer whales Orcinus orca and orangutans Pongo spp. The vocal systems of most of these species are more consistent with a non-Markovian RP than with the Markovian models traditionally assumed. Our data suggest that non-Markovian vocal sequences may be more common than Markov sequences, which must be taken into account when evaluating alternative hypotheses for the evolution of signalling complexity, and perhaps human language origins.


Asunto(s)
Mamíferos/fisiología , Modelos Estadísticos , Passeriformes/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Cadenas de Markov , Espectrografía del Sonido , Especificidad de la Especie
7.
Neural Comput ; 26(3): 523-56, 2014 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24320849

RESUMEN

Speech recognition in noisy conditions is a major challenge for computer systems, but the human brain performs it routinely and accurately. Automatic speech recognition (ASR) systems that are inspired by neuroscience can potentially bridge the performance gap between humans and machines. We present a system for noise-robust isolated word recognition that works by decoding sequences of spikes from a population of simulated auditory feature-detecting neurons. Each neuron is trained to respond selectively to a brief spectrotemporal pattern, or feature, drawn from the simulated auditory nerve response to speech. The neural population conveys the time-dependent structure of a sound by its sequence of spikes. We compare two methods for decoding the spike sequences--one using a hidden Markov model-based recognizer, the other using a novel template-based recognition scheme. In the latter case, words are recognized by comparing their spike sequences to template sequences obtained from clean training data, using a similarity measure based on the length of the longest common sub-sequence. Using isolated spoken digits from the AURORA-2 database, we show that our combined system outperforms a state-of-the-art robust speech recognizer at low signal-to-noise ratios. Both the spike-based encoding scheme and the template-based decoding offer gains in noise robustness over traditional speech recognition methods. Our system highlights potential advantages of spike-based acoustic coding and provides a biologically motivated framework for robust ASR development.


Asunto(s)
Redes Neurales de la Computación , Relación Señal-Ruido , Acústica del Lenguaje , Software de Reconocimiento del Habla , Potenciales de Acción , Algoritmos , Humanos , Cadenas de Markov , Modelos Teóricos , Neuronas/fisiología , Ruido , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas , Espectrografía del Sonido , Habla , Factores de Tiempo , Vocabulario
8.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 107(47): 20512-7, 2010 Nov 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20974967

RESUMEN

Habits and rituals are expressed universally across animal species. These behaviors are advantageous in allowing sequential behaviors to be performed without cognitive overload, and appear to rely on neural circuits that are relatively benign but vulnerable to takeover by extreme contexts, neuropsychiatric sequelae, and processes leading to addiction. Reinforcement learning (RL) is thought to underlie the formation of optimal habits. However, this theoretic formulation has principally been tested experimentally in simple stimulus-response tasks with relatively few available responses. We asked whether RL could also account for the emergence of habitual action sequences in realistically complex situations in which no repetitive stimulus-response links were present and in which many response options were present. We exposed naïve macaque monkeys to such experimental conditions by introducing a unique free saccade scan task. Despite the highly uncertain conditions and no instruction, the monkeys developed a succession of stereotypical, self-chosen saccade sequence patterns. Remarkably, these continued to morph for months, long after session-averaged reward and cost (eye movement distance) reached asymptote. Prima facie, these continued behavioral changes appeared to challenge RL. However, trial-by-trial analysis showed that pattern changes on adjacent trials were predicted by lowered cost, and RL simulations that reduced the cost reproduced the monkeys' behavior. Ultimately, the patterns settled into stereotypical saccade sequences that minimized the cost of obtaining the reward on average. These findings suggest that brain mechanisms underlying the emergence of habits, and perhaps unwanted repetitive behaviors in clinical disorders, could follow RL algorithms capturing extremely local explore/exploit tradeoffs.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Hábitos , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Refuerzo en Psicología , Algoritmos , Animales , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Femenino , Modelos Biológicos , Recompensa
9.
Elife ; 122023 05 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37158590

RESUMEN

Complex motor skills in vertebrates require specialized upper motor neurons with precise action potential (AP) firing. To examine how diverse populations of upper motor neurons subserve distinct functions and the specific repertoire of ion channels involved, we conducted a thorough study of the excitability of upper motor neurons controlling somatic motor function in the zebra finch. We found that robustus arcopallialis projection neurons (RAPNs), key command neurons for song production, exhibit ultranarrow spikes and higher firing rates compared to neurons controlling non-vocal somatic motor functions (dorsal intermediate arcopallium [AId] neurons). Pharmacological and molecular data indicate that this striking difference is associated with the higher expression in RAPNs of high threshold, fast-activating voltage-gated Kv3 channels, that likely contain Kv3.1 (KCNC1) subunits. The spike waveform and Kv3.1 expression in RAPNs mirror properties of Betz cells, specialized upper motor neurons involved in fine digit control in humans and other primates but absent in rodents. Our study thus provides evidence that songbirds and primates have convergently evolved the use of Kv3.1 to ensure precise, rapid AP firing in upper motor neurons controlling fast and complex motor skills.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Motora , Canales de Potasio con Entrada de Voltaje , Pájaros Cantores , Animales , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Interneuronas , Neuronas Motoras , Canales de Potasio Shaw
10.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 7(3): e1001108, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21445230

RESUMEN

Songs of many songbird species consist of variable sequences of a finite number of syllables. A common approach for characterizing the syntax of these complex syllable sequences is to use transition probabilities between the syllables. This is equivalent to the Markov model, in which each syllable is associated with one state, and the transition probabilities between the states do not depend on the state transition history. Here we analyze the song syntax in Bengalese finch. We show that the Markov model fails to capture the statistical properties of the syllable sequences. Instead, a state transition model that accurately describes the statistics of the syllable sequences includes adaptation of the self-transition probabilities when states are revisited consecutively, and allows associations of more than one state to a given syllable. Such a model does not increase the model complexity significantly. Mathematically, the model is a partially observable Markov model with adaptation (POMMA). The success of the POMMA supports the branching chain network model of how syntax is controlled within the premotor song nucleus HVC, but also suggests that adaptation and many-to-one mapping from the syllable-encoding chain networks in HVC to syllables should be included in the network model.


Asunto(s)
Pinzones/fisiología , Modelos Biológicos , Modelos Estadísticos , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Animales , Cadenas de Markov , Música , Espectrografía del Sonido
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 106(45): 19156-61, 2009 Nov 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19850874

RESUMEN

Encoding time is universally required for learning and structuring motor and cognitive actions, but how the brain keeps track of time is still not understood. We searched for time representations in cortico-basal ganglia circuits by recording from thousands of neurons in the prefrontal cortex and striatum of macaque monkeys performing a routine visuomotor task. We found that a subset of neurons exhibited time-stamp encoding strikingly similar to that required by models of reinforcement-based learning: They responded with spike activity peaks that were distributed at different time delays after single task events. Moreover, the temporal evolution of the population activity allowed robust decoding of task time by perceptron models. We suggest that time information can emerge as a byproduct of event coding in cortico-basal ganglia circuits and can serve as a critical infrastructure for behavioral learning and performance.


Asunto(s)
Ganglios Basales/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Tiempo , Algoritmos , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Electrofisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
12.
Nature ; 437(7062): 1158-61, 2005 Oct 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16237445

RESUMEN

Learning to perform a behavioural procedure as a well-ingrained habit requires extensive repetition of the behavioural sequence, and learning not to perform such behaviours is notoriously difficult. Yet regaining a habit can occur quickly, with even one or a few exposures to cues previously triggering the behaviour. To identify neural mechanisms that might underlie such learning dynamics, we made long-term recordings from multiple neurons in the sensorimotor striatum, a basal ganglia structure implicated in habit formation, in rats successively trained on a reward-based procedural task, given extinction training and then given reacquisition training. The spike activity of striatal output neurons, nodal points in cortico-basal ganglia circuits, changed markedly across multiple dimensions during each of these phases of learning. First, new patterns of task-related ensemble firing successively formed, reversed and then re-emerged. Second, task-irrelevant firing was suppressed, then rebounded, and then was suppressed again. These changing spike activity patterns were highly correlated with changes in behavioural performance. We propose that these changes in task representation in cortico-basal ganglia circuits represent neural equivalents of the explore-exploit behaviour characteristic of habit learning.


Asunto(s)
Ganglios Basales/citología , Ganglios Basales/fisiología , Hábitos , Memoria/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Extinción Psicológica/fisiología , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/fisiología , Ratas , Recompensa
13.
Neuron ; 47(2): 267-80, 2005 Jul 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16039568

RESUMEN

Whether general principles can explain the layouts of cortical maps remains unresolved. In primary visual cortex of ferret, the relationships between the maps of visual space and response features are predicted by a "dimension-reduction" model. The representation of visual space is anisotropic, with the elevation and azimuth axes having different magnification. This anisotropy is reflected in the orientation, ocular dominance, and spatial frequency domains, which are elongated such that their directions of rapid change, or high-gradient axes, are orthogonal to the high-gradient axis of the visual map. The feature maps are also strongly interdependent-their high-gradient regions avoid one another and intersect orthogonally where essential, so that overlap is minimized. Our results demonstrate a clear influence of the visual map on each feature map. In turn, the local representation of visual space is smooth, as predicted when many features are mapped within a cortical area.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Orientación/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Anisotropía , Diagnóstico por Imagen/métodos , Dominancia Cerebral , Predominio Ocular/fisiología , Electrofisiología , Hurones , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas
14.
Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys ; 79(5 Pt 1): 051917, 2009 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19518490

RESUMEN

Spike propagation in chain networks is usually studied in the synfire regime, in which successive groups of neurons are synaptically activated sequentially through the unidirectional excitatory connections. Here we study the dynamics of chain networks with dominant global feedback inhibition that prevents the synfire activity. Neural activity is driven by suprathreshold external inputs. We analytically and numerically demonstrate that spike propagation along the chain is a unique dynamical attractor in a wide parameter regime. The strong inhibition permits a robust winner-take-all propagation in the case of multiple chains competing via the inhibition.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Relojes Biológicos/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Transmisión Sináptica/fisiología , Simulación por Computador , Retroalimentación/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología
15.
Front Neuroinform ; 13: 68, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31736735

RESUMEN

Neurons perform computations by integrating inputs from thousands of synapses-mostly in the dendritic tree-to drive action potential firing in the axon. One fruitful approach to studying this process is to record from neurons using patch-clamp electrodes, fill the recorded neurons with a substance that allows subsequent staining, reconstruct the three-dimensional architectures of the dendrites, and use the resulting functional and structural data to develop computer models of dendritic integration. Accurately producing quantitative reconstructions of dendrites is typically a tedious process taking many hours of manual inspection and measurement. Here we present ShuTu, a new software package that facilitates accurate and efficient reconstruction of dendrites imaged using bright-field microscopy. The program operates in two steps: (1) automated identification of dendritic processes, and (2) manual correction of errors in the automated reconstruction. This approach allows neurons with complex dendritic morphologies to be reconstructed rapidly and efficiently, thus facilitating the use of computer models to study dendritic structure-function relationships and the computations performed by single neurons.

16.
J Neurosci ; 27(38): 10299-310, 2007 Sep 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17881536

RESUMEN

In the adult visual cortex, multiple feature maps exist and have characteristic spatial relationships with one another. The relationships can be reproduced by "dimension-reduction" computational models, suggesting that the principles of continuity and coverage may underlie cortical map organization. However, the mechanisms responsible for establishing these relationships are unknown. We explored whether removing one feature map during development causes a coordinated reorganization of the remaining maps or whether the remaining maps are unaffected. We removed the ocular dominance map by monocular enucleation in newborn ferrets, so that single eye stimulation drove the cortex in a more spatially uniform manner in adult monocular animals compared with normal animals. Maps of orientation, spatial frequency, and retinotopy formed in monocular ferrets, but their structures and spatial relationships differed from those in normal ferrets. The wavelength of the orientation map increased, so that the average orientation gradient across the cortex decreased. The decrease in the orientation gradient in monocular animals was most prominent in the high gradient regions of the spatial frequency map, indicating a coordinated reorganization between these two maps. In monocular animals, the orthogonal relationship between the orientation and spatial frequency maps was preserved, and the orthogonal relationship between the orientation and retinotopic maps became more pronounced. These results were consistent with detailed predictions of a dimension-reduction model of cortical organization. Thus, the number of feature maps in a cortical area influences the relationships between them, and inputs to the cortex have a significant role in generating these relationships.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Enucleación del Ojo/métodos , Femenino , Hurones , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Embarazo , Conducta Espacial/fisiología
17.
Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc ; 91(1): 13-52, 2016 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25428267

RESUMEN

Animal acoustic communication often takes the form of complex sequences, made up of multiple distinct acoustic units. Apart from the well-known example of birdsong, other animals such as insects, amphibians, and mammals (including bats, rodents, primates, and cetaceans) also generate complex acoustic sequences. Occasionally, such as with birdsong, the adaptive role of these sequences seems clear (e.g. mate attraction and territorial defence). More often however, researchers have only begun to characterise - let alone understand - the significance and meaning of acoustic sequences. Hypotheses abound, but there is little agreement as to how sequences should be defined and analysed. Our review aims to outline suitable methods for testing these hypotheses, and to describe the major limitations to our current and near-future knowledge on questions of acoustic sequences. This review and prospectus is the result of a collaborative effort between 43 scientists from the fields of animal behaviour, ecology and evolution, signal processing, machine learning, quantitative linguistics, and information theory, who gathered for a 2013 workshop entitled, 'Analysing vocal sequences in animals'. Our goal is to present not just a review of the state of the art, but to propose a methodological framework that summarises what we suggest are the best practices for research in this field, across taxa and across disciplines. We also provide a tutorial-style introduction to some of the most promising algorithmic approaches for analysing sequences. We divide our review into three sections: identifying the distinct units of an acoustic sequence, describing the different ways that information can be contained within a sequence, and analysing the structure of that sequence. Each of these sections is further subdivided to address the key questions and approaches in that area. We propose a uniform, systematic, and comprehensive approach to studying sequences, with the goal of clarifying research terms used in different fields, and facilitating collaboration and comparative studies. Allowing greater interdisciplinary collaboration will facilitate the investigation of many important questions in the evolution of communication and sociality.


Asunto(s)
Vocalización Animal , Acústica , Animales , Cadenas de Markov , Modelos Biológicos , Percepción
18.
Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys ; 69(2 Pt 1): 021905, 2004 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14995489

RESUMEN

Sensory neurons in many brain areas spike with precise timing to stimuli with temporal structures, and encode temporally complex stimuli into spatiotemporal spikes. How the downstream neurons read out such neural code is an important unsolved problem. In this paper, we describe a decoding scheme using a spiking recurrent neural network. The network consists of excitatory neurons that form a synfire chain, and two globally inhibitory interneurons of different types that provide delayed feedforward and fast feedback inhibition, respectively. The network signals recognition of a specific spatiotemporal sequence when the last excitatory neuron down the synfire chain spikes, which happens if and only if that sequence was present in the input spike stream. The recognition scheme is invariant to variations in the intervals between input spikes within some range. The computation of the network can be mapped into that of a finite state machine. Our network provides a simple way to decode spatiotemporal spikes with diverse types of neurons.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Neuronas Aferentes/fisiología , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas , Transmisión Sináptica/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Animales , Simulación por Computador , Retroalimentación/fisiología , Humanos , Potenciales de la Membrana/fisiología
19.
Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys ; 65(5 Pt 1): 051922, 2002 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12059608

RESUMEN

Neural networks with recurrent connections are sometimes regarded as too slow at computation to serve as models of the brain. Here we analytically study a counterexample, a network consisting of N integrate-and-fire neurons with self excitation, all-to-all inhibition, instantaneous synaptic coupling, and constant external driving inputs. When the inhibition and/or excitation are large enough, the network performs a winner-take-all computation for all possible external inputs and initial states of the network. The computation is done very quickly: As soon as the winner spikes once, the computation is completed since no other neurons will spike. For some initial states, the winner is the first neuron to spike, and the computation is done at the first spike of the network. In general, there are M potential winners, corresponding to the top M external inputs. When the external inputs are close in magnitude, M tends to be larger. If M>1, the selection of the actual winner is strongly influenced by the initial states. If a special relation between the excitation and inhibition is satisfied, the network always selects the neuron with the maximum external input as the winner.

20.
J Vis ; 2(8): 571-6, 2002.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12678640

RESUMEN

The statistical arrangement of oriented segments in natural scenes was recently proposed to be indicative of a cocircularity rule. In particular, the probability density function of the relative position of two oriented segments was found to be maximal along fixed angles on the plane, consistent with the two segments being tangent to two points of a circle. Does this observation point to a prevalence of circles in natural scenes? Here we demonstrate that similar statistics can be obtained even when circles are not very common in visual scenes. The reason is that circles or near circular objects can heavily skew the distribution in favor of the cocircularity rule.


Asunto(s)
Teoría Gestáltica , Percepción de Cercanía/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Humanos , Cómputos Matemáticos , Orientación
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA