Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 37
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Psychol Sci ; 28(4): 470-481, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28207350

RESUMEN

Task-switching experiments have documented a puzzling phenomenon: Advance warning of the switch reduces but does not eliminate the switch cost. Theoretical accounts have posited that the residual switch cost arises when one selects the relevant stimulus-response mapping, leaving earlier perceptual processes unaffected. We put this assumption to the test by seeking electrophysiological markers of encoding a perceptual dimension. Participants categorized a colored letter as a vowel or consonant or its color as "warm" or "cold." Orthogonally to the color manipulation, some colors were eight times more frequent than others, and the letters were in upper- or lowercase. Color frequency modulated the electroencephalogram amplitude at around 150 ms when participants repeated the color-classification task. When participants switched from the letter task to the color task, this effect was significantly delayed. Thus, even when prepared for, a task switch delays or prolongs encoding of the relevant perceptual dimension.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
2.
Cogn Psychol ; 86: 27-61, 2016 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26859519

RESUMEN

Flexible behavior requires a control system that can inhibit actions in response to changes in the environment. Recent studies suggest that people proactively adjust response parameters in anticipation of a stop signal. In three experiments, we tested the hypothesis that proactive inhibitory control involves adjusting both attentional and response settings, and we explored the relationship with other forms of proactive and anticipatory control. Subjects responded to the color of a stimulus. On some trials, an extra signal occurred. The response to this signal depended on the task context subjects were in: in the 'ignore' context, they ignored it; in the 'stop' context, they had to withhold their response; and in the 'double-response' context, they had to execute a secondary response. An analysis of event-related brain potentials for no-signal trials in the stop context revealed that proactive inhibitory control works by biasing the settings of lower-level systems that are involved in stimulus detection, action selection, and action execution. Furthermore, subjects made similar adjustments in the double-response and stop-signal contexts, indicating an overlap between various forms of proactive action control. The results of Experiment 1 also suggest an overlap between proactive inhibitory control and preparatory control in task-switching studies: both require reconfiguration of task-set parameters to bias or alter subordinate processes. We conclude that much of the top-down control in response inhibition tasks takes place before the inhibition signal is presented.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Encéfalo/fisiología , Inhibición Psicológica , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Adolescente , Adulto , Sesgo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
3.
Neuroimage ; 87: 61-71, 2014 Feb 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24188811

RESUMEN

We report the first electrophysiological investigation of the inverse base-rate effect (IBRE), a robust non-rational bias in predictive learning. In the IBRE, participants learn that one pair of symptoms (AB) predicts a frequently occurring disease, whilst an overlapping pair of symptoms (AC) predicts a rarely occurring disease. Participants subsequently infer that BC predicts the rare disease, a non-rational decision made in opposition to the underlying base rates of the two diseases. Error-driven attention theories of learning state that the IBRE occurs because C attracts more attention than B. On the basis of this account we predicted and observed the occurrence of brain potentials associated with visual attention: a posterior Selection Negativity, and a concurrent anterior Selection Positivity, for C vs. B in a post-training test phase. Error-driven attention theories further predict no Selection Negativity, Selection Positivity or IBRE, for control symptoms matched on frequency to B and C, but for which there was no shared symptom (A) during training. These predictions were also confirmed, and this confirmation discounts alternative explanations of the IBRE based on the relative novelty of B and C. Further, we observed higher response accuracy for B alone than for C alone; this dissociation of response accuracy (B>C) from attentional allocation (C>B) discounts the possibility that the observed attentional difference was caused by the difference in response accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
4.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218241256361, 2024 Jun 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38724482

RESUMEN

Task-switching experiments have shown that the "switch cost" (poorer performance for task switches than for repetitions) is smaller when the probability of a switch is high (e.g., 0.75) than when it is low (e.g., 0.25). Some theoretical accounts explain this effect in terms of top-down control deployed in advance of the task cue ("pre-cue reconfiguration"). We tested such accounts by manipulating the time available before the onset of the cue (the response-cue interval, RCI), reasoning that top-down pre-cue reconfiguration requires time and therefore its effect should increase with RCI. Participants heard a man and a woman simultaneously speaking number words and categorised the number (< 5 vs. > 5) spoken by the voice specified by a pictorial gender-related cue presented at an RCI of 100 ms or 2,200 ms. The target voice switched with a probability of 0.25 or 0.75 (in separate sessions). In Experiment 1, RTs revealed a large effect of switch probability on the switch cost in the short RCI, which did not increase in the long RCI. Errors hinted at such an increase, but it did not receive clear statistical support and was disconfirmed by a direct and better powered replication in Experiment 2, which fully confirmed the RT pattern from Experiment 1. Thus, the effect of switch probability on the switch cost required little/no time following the response to emerge-it was already at full magnitude at a short RCI-challenging accounts that assume "phasic" deployment of top-down task-set control in advance of the cue.

5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(3): 750-767, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38212478

RESUMEN

Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice 'switch cost' reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear-the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target's gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, 'congruent' trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, 'incongruent' trials (where the voices afforded different responses). 'Conflict prevalence' had a strong effect on selectivity-the incongruent-congruent difference ('congruence effect') was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition-but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Conflicto Psicológico , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción Auditiva , Percepción del Habla , Tiempo de Reacción , Voz , Adolescente , Probabilidad
6.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 62(3): 1547-1565, 2023 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37039361

RESUMEN

Previous studies suggested that social identity switches are rapid and highly effective, raising the question of whether people can intentionally control such switches. In two studies, we tested if participants could exert top-down control to prevent a social identity switch triggered by the experimental context. In Study 1, participants (N = 198) were given a writing task aimed at prompting a switch from their parent identity to their feminist identity. Before the prompt, half of the participants (the experimental group) were instructed to remain in their parent identity, avoiding an identity switch; the control group was not given such instructions. We found no significant difference between the groups in either self-reported salience or the implicit computational measure of salience based on participants' linguistic style, both measures suggesting a switch in both groups. Study 2 (N = 380) followed the same design but included a monetary incentive to prevent the switch in the experimental group. The groups differed significantly in their self-reported salience but not in the implicit measure, which suggests limited ability to avoid the switch even when participants report being able to do so. These results point to limited intentional control over exogenously triggered identity switches, with important practical implications.


Asunto(s)
Motivación , Identificación Social , Humanos , Feminismo , Autoinforme
7.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 33(5): 1137-54, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21630376

RESUMEN

Behavioural and neurophysiological studies of task-switching have tended to employ 'bivalent' stimuli (which afford responses in two tasks). Using brain potential recordings, we investigated task-switching with 'univalent' stimuli affording responses in only one of the tasks, and compared the outcomes to those recently obtained with bivalent stimuli (Lavric et al. [2008]: Eur J Neurosci 1-14), in order to examine two phenomena. First, when only univalent stimuli are presented, the processing of task cues becomes optional. Our results showed that in these circumstances linguistic (but not pictorial) cues were still effective in eliciting at least some degree of preparation for a task-switch, as evidenced by the reduction in the error cost of switching at the longer preparation interval and by a posterior switch-induced ERP positivity at about 450-800 ms in the cue-stimulus interval. Second, single affordance stimuli not only reduced behavioural switch costs relative to bivalent stimuli; they also produced a smaller post-stimulus switch-induced negativity, consistent with the latter being a marker of conflict between task-sets. However, using stimuli not associated with responses in the alternative task did not completely eliminate the negativity. We speculate that the residue reflects other sources of conflict: attention to the irrelevant perceptual dimension and/or persistence of task goals.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Conflicto Psicológico , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
8.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 150(10): 1956-1973, 2021 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33523686

RESUMEN

A substantial literature relates task-set control and language selection in bilinguals-with "switching" paradigms serving as a methodological "bridge." We asked a basic question: Is preparation for a switch equally effective in the two domains? Bilinguals switched between naming pictures in one language and another, or between the tasks of naming and categorizing pictures. The critical trials used for comparing the two kinds of switching were identical in all respects-task (naming), stimuli, responses-except one: whether the shape cue presented before the picture specified the language or the task. The effect of preparation on the "switch cost" was examined by varying the cue-stimulus interval (CSI; 50/800/1,175 ms). Preparation for a task switch was more effective: Increasing the CSI from 50 to 800 ms reduced the reaction time task switch cost by ∼63% to its minimum, but the language switch cost only by ∼24%, the latter continuing to reduce with further opportunity for preparation (CSI = 1,175 ms). The switch costs in the two domains correlated moderately (r = .36). We propose that preparation for a language switch is less effective, because (a) it must preemptively counteract greater interference during a language switch than during a task switch, and/or (b) lexical access is less amenable to "top-down" control than (components of) task-set. We also investigated the associations between stimuli and the language (or task) where they were last encountered. Associative history influenced performance-but similarly for switches and repetitions-indicating that stimulus-induced associative retrieval of language (or task-set) did not contribute to switch costs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
9.
J Cogn ; 4(1): 36, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34430795

RESUMEN

In Part 1 we review task-switching and other studies showing that, even with time for preparation, participants' ability to shift attention to a relevant attribute or object before the stimulus onset is limited: there is a 'residual cost'. In particular, several brain potential markers of perceptual encoding are delayed on task-switch trials, compared to task-repeat trials that require attention to the same attribute as before. Such effects have been documented even for a process often considered 'automatic' - visual word recognition: ERP markers of word frequency and word/nonword status are (1) delayed when the word recognition task follows a judgement of a perceptual property compared to repeating the lexical task, and (2) strongly attenuated during the perceptual judgements. Thus, even lexical access seems influenced by the task/attentional set. In Part 2, we report in detail a demonstration of what seems to be a special case, where task-set and a task switch have no such effect on perceptual encoding. Participants saw an outline letter superimposed on a face expressing neutral or negative emotion, and were auditorily cued to categorise the letter as vowel/consonant, or the face as emotional/neutral. ERPs exhibited a robust emotional-neutral difference (Emotional Expression Effect) no smaller or later when switching to the face task than when repeating it; in the first half of its time-course it did not vary with the task at all. The initial encoding of the valence of a fixated facial emotional expression appears to be involuntary and invariant, whatever the endogenous task/attentional set.

10.
Neuropsychologia ; 160: 107984, 2021 09 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34339718

RESUMEN

Among the issues examined by studies of cognitive control in multitasking is whether processes underlying performance in the different tasks occur serially or in parallel. Here we ask a similar question about processes that pro-actively control task-set. In task-switching experiments, several indices of task-set preparation have been extensively documented, including anticipatory orientation of gaze to the task-relevant location (an unambiguous marker of reorientation of attention), and a positive polarity brain potential over the posterior cortex (whose functional significance is less well understood). We examine whether these markers of preparation occur in parallel or serially, and in what order. On each trial a cue required participants to make a semantic classification of one of three digits presented simultaneously, with the location of each digit consistently associated with one of three classification tasks (e.g., if the task was odd/even, the digit at the top of the display was relevant). The EEG positivity emerged following, and appeared time-locked to, the anticipatory fixation on the task-relevant location, which might suggest serial organisation. However, the fixation-locked positivity was not better defined than the cue-locked positivity; in fact, for the trials with the earliest fixations the positivity was better time-locked to the cue onset. This is more consistent with (re)orientation of spatial attention occurring in parallel with, but slightly before, the reconfiguration of other task-set components indexed by the EEG positivity.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción
11.
J Cogn ; 3(1): 16, 2020 Jul 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32743338

RESUMEN

Commentary on Schmidt, Liefooghe & De Houwer (2020, JoC): "An episodic model of task switching effects: Erasing the homunculus from memory".

12.
J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn ; 46(1): 83-98, 2020 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31657942

RESUMEN

This article reports results from three experiments that investigate how a particular neuro-stimulation procedure is able, in certain circumstances, to selectively increase the face inversion effect by enhancing recognition for upright faces, and argues that these effects can be understood in terms of the McLaren-Kaye-Mackintosh (MKM) theory of stimulus representation. We demonstrate how a specific transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) methodology can improve performance in circumstances where error-based salience modulation is making face recognition harder. The 3 experiments used an old/new recognition task involving sets of normal versus Thatcherized faces. The main characteristic of Thatcherized faces is that the eyes and the mouth are upside down, thus emphasizing features that tend to be common to other Thatcherized faces and so leading to stronger generalization making recognition worse. Experiment 1 combined a behavioral and event-related potential study looking at the N170 peak component, which helped us to calibrate the set of face stimuli needed for subsequent experiments. In Experiment 2, we used our tDCS procedure (between-subjects and double-blind) in an attempt to reduce the negative effects induced by error-based modulation of salience on recognition of upright Thatcherized faces. Results largely confirmed our predictions. In addition, they showed a significant improvement on recognition performance for upright normal faces. Experiment 3 provides the first direct evidence in a single study that the same tDCS procedure is able to both enhance performance when normal faces are presented with Thatcherized faces, and to reduce performance when normal faces are presented with other normal faces (i.e., male vs. female faces). We interpret our results by analyzing how salience modulation influences generalization between similar categories of stimuli. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Generalización del Estimulo/fisiología , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 143: 107470, 2020 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32315696

RESUMEN

The following study investigates the effects of tDCS on face recognition skills indexed by the face inversion effect (better recognition performance for upright vs. inverted faces). We combined tDCS and EEG simultaneously to examine the effects of tDCS on the face inversion effect behaviourally and on the N170 ERPs component. The results from two experiments (overall N = 112) show that anodal tDCS delivered at Fp3 site for 10 min at 1.5 mA (double-blind and between-subjects) can reduce behaviourally the face inversion effect compared to sham (control) stimulation. The ERP results provide some evidence for tDCS being able to influence the face inversion effect on the N170. Specifically, we find a dissociation of the tDCS-induced effects where for the N170 latencies the tDCS reduces the usual face inversion effect (delayed N170 in response to inverted vs. upright faces) compared to sham. Contrarily, the same tDCS procedure on the same participants increased the inversion effect seen in the N170 amplitudes by making the negative deflection for the inverted faces that much greater than that for upright faces. We interpret our results in the context of the literature on the face inversion effect and the N170 peak component. In doing so, we extend our results to previous studies investigating the effects of tDCS on perceptual learning and face recognition.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos , Aprendizaje , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Reconocimiento en Psicología
14.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 45(7): 1224-1233, 2019 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30024253

RESUMEN

A key index of top-down control in task switching-preparation for a switch-is underexplored in language switching. The well-documented EEG "signature" of preparation for a task switch-a protracted positive-polarity modulation over the posterior scalp-has thus far not been reported in language switching, and the interpretation of previously reported effects of preparation on language switching performance is complicated by confounding factors. In an experiment using event-related potentials (ERPs) and an optimized picture-naming paradigm that addressed these confounds the language was specified by an auditory cue on every trial and changed unpredictably. There were two key manipulations. First, the cue-stimulus interval allowed either generous (1,500 ms) or little (100 ms) opportunity for preparation. Second, to explore the interplay between bottom-up and top-down language selection, we compared a highly transparent and familiar "supercue"-the name of the language spoken in that language to a relatively opaque cue (short speeded-up fragment of national anthem). Preparation for a switch elicited a brain potential strongly reminiscent of the posterior switch positivity documented in task switching. As previously shown in task switching, its amplitude inversely predicted the performance "switch cost," demonstrated by our ERP analyses contingent on reaction time (RT). This overlap in the electrophysiological correlates of preparing to switch tasks and languages suggests domain-general processes for top-down selection of task-set and language for production. But, the surprisingly small language switch cost following the supercue in the short CSI suggests that rapid and (possibly automatic) bottom-up selection-not typically observed in task switching-may also occur. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Multilingüismo , Psicolingüística , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 45(7): 966-982, 2019 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31021156

RESUMEN

We can selectively attend to one of two simultaneous voices sharing a source location. Can we endogenously select the voice before speech is heard? Participants heard two digit names, spoken simultaneously by a male voice and a female voice, following a visual cue indicating which voice's digit to classify as odd or even. There was a substantial cost in reaction time and errors when the target voice switched from one trial to the next. In Experiment 1, with a highly familiar pair of voices, the switch cost reduced by nearly half as the cue-stimulus interval increased from 50 to 800 ms, indicating (contrary to previous reports) effective endogenous preparation for a change of voice. No further reduction in switch cost occurred with a longer preparation interval-this "residual" switch cost may be attributable to attentional "inertia." In Experiment 2, with previously unfamiliar voices, the pattern of switch costs was very similar, though repeated attention to the same target voice over a run of trials improved performance more. Delaying the onset of one voice by 366 ms improved performance, but the pattern of preparatory tuning effects was similar. Thus, endogenous preparation for a voice is possible, but it is limited in efficacy, as for some other attentional domains. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción Auditiva , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Comportamiento Multifuncional , Tiempo de Reacción , Voz , Adulto Joven
16.
Eur J Neurosci ; 28(5): 1016-29, 2008 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18717737

RESUMEN

Changing between cognitive tasks requires a reorganization of cognitive processes. Behavioural evidence suggests this can occur in advance of the stimulus. However, the existence or detectability of an anticipatory task-set reconfiguration process remains controversial, in part because several neuroimaging studies have not detected extra brain activity during preparation for a task switch relative to a task repeat. In contrast, electrophysiological studies have identified potential correlates of preparation for a task switch, but their interpretation is hindered by the scarcity of evidence on their relationship to performance. We aimed to: (i) identify the brain potential(s) reflecting effective preparation for a task-switch in a task-cuing paradigm that shows clear behavioural evidence for advance preparation, and (ii) characterize this activity by means of temporal segmentation and source analysis. Our results show that when advance preparation was effective (as indicated by fast responses), a protracted switch-related component, manifesting itself as widespread posterior positivity and concurrent right anterior negativity, preceded stimulus onset for approximately 300 ms, with sources primarily in the left lateral frontal, right inferior frontal and temporal cortices. When advance preparation was ineffective (as implied by slow responses), or made impossible by a short cue-stimulus interval (CSI), a similar component, with lateral prefrontal generators, peaked approximately 300 ms poststimulus. The protracted prestimulus component (which we show to be distinct from P3 or contingent negative variation, CNV) also correlated over subjects with a behavioural measure of preparation. Furthermore, its differential lateralization for word and picture cues was consistent with a role for verbal self-instruction in preparatory task-set reconfiguration.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Prefrontal/anatomía & histología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Procesamiento de Señales Asistido por Computador , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto Joven
17.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 71(11): 2464-2476, 2018 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30362407

RESUMEN

The face inversion effect refers to a decrement in performance when we try to recognise familiar faces turned upside down (inverted), compared with familiar faces presented in their usual (upright) orientation. Recently, we have demonstrated that the inversion effect can also be found with checkerboards drawn from prototype-defined categories when the participants have been trained with these categories, suggesting that factors such as expertise and the relationships between stimulus features may be important determinants of this effect. We also demonstrated that the typical inversion effect on the N170 seen with faces is found with checkerboards, suggesting that modulation of the N170 is a marker for disruption in the use of configural information. In the present experiment, we first demonstrate that our scrambling technique greatly reduces the inversion effect in faces. Following this, we used Event-Related Potentials ( ERPs) recorded while participants performed an Old/New recognition study on normal and scrambled faces presented in both upright and inverted orientations to investigate the impact of scrambling on the N170. We obtained the standard robust inversion effect for normal faces: The N170 was both larger and delayed for normal inverted faces as compared with normal upright faces, whereas a significantly reduced inversion effect was recorded for scrambled faces. These results show that the inversion effect on the N170 is greater for normal compared with scrambled faces, and we interpret the smaller effect for scrambled faces as being due to the reduction in expertise for those faces consequent on scrambling.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Cara , Orientación/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
18.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 43(6): 862-873, 2017 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27936844

RESUMEN

The performance overhead associated with changing tasks (the "switch cost") usually diminishes when the task is specified in advance but is rarely eliminated by preparation. A popular account of the "residual" (asymptotic) switch cost is that it reflects "task-set inertia": carry-over of task-set parameters from the preceding trial(s). New evidence for a component of "task-set inertia" comes from eye-tracking, where the location associated with the previously (but no longer) relevant task is fixated preferentially over other irrelevant locations, even when preparation intervals are generous. Might such limits in overcoming task-set inertia in general, and "attentional inertia" in particular, result from suboptimal scheduling of preparation when the time available is outside one's control? In the present study, the stimulus comprised 3 digits located at the points of an invisible triangle, preceded by a central verbal cue specifying which of 3 classification tasks to perform, each consistently applied to just 1 digit location. The digits were presented only when fixation moved away from the cue, thus giving the participant control over preparation time. In contrast to our previous research with experimenter-determined preparation intervals, we found no sign of attentional inertia for the long preparation intervals. Self-paced preparation reduced but did not eliminate the performance switch cost-leaving a clear residual component in both reaction time and error rates. That the scheduling of preparation accounts for some, but not all, components of the residual switch cost, challenges existing accounts of the switch cost, even those which distinguish between preparatory and poststimulus reconfiguration processes. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica , Atención , Función Ejecutiva , Movimientos Oculares , Desempeño Psicomotor , Análisis de Varianza , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Psicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
19.
Emotion ; 6(1): 40-61, 2006 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16637749

RESUMEN

On the basis of a review of the extant literature describing emotion-cognition interactions, the authors propose 4 methodological desiderata for studying how task-irrelevant affect modulates cognition and present data from an experiment satisfying them. Consistent with accounts of the hemispheric asymmetries characterizing withdrawal-related negative affect and visuospatial working memory (WM) in prefrontal and parietal cortices, threat-induced anxiety selectively disrupted accuracy of spatial but not verbal WM performance. Furthermore, individual differences in physiological measures of anxiety statistically mediated the degree of disruption. A second experiment revealed that individuals characterized by high levels of behavioral inhibition exhibited more intense anxiety and relatively worse spatial WM performance in the absence of threat, solidifying the authors' inference that anxiety causally mediates disruption. These observations suggest a revision of extant models of how anxiety sculpts cognition and underscore the utility of the desiderata.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/fisiopatología , Ansiedad/psicología , Memoria , Corteza Prefrontal , Percepción Espacial , Cognición , Electromiografía , Emociones , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino , Psicometría , Tiempo de Reacción , Estados Unidos
20.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 62(1): 54-9, 2006 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16503359

RESUMEN

The acquisition of ERPs concurrently with fMRI in cognitive paradigms is appealing, but technically challenging. Little is known about the effects of the fMRI environment on the time-course and topography of previously documented ERP effects. We examined the replicability of ERP differences in the scanner at the level of individual subjects, using two cognitive paradigms and two statistical procedures. ERP P3 differences found outside the scanner in both paradigms were also robustly detected in the ERPs acquired during fMRI scanning. These P3 effects had equivalent time-courses and scalp topographies inside and outside the scanner. This replication at the level of individual data-sets has implications for the clinical applicability of ERP-fMRI and, more generally, for the quality of scanner recorded ERPs.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Oxígeno/sangre , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA