Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 59
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Psychol Med ; 43(6): 1323-33, 2013 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22975296

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Increased reaction time variability (RTV) on cognitive tasks requiring a speeded response is characteristic of several psychiatric disorders. In attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), the association with RTV is strong phenotypically and genetically, yet high RTV is not a stable impairment but shows ADHD-sensitive improvement under certain conditions, such as those with rewards. The state regulation theory proposed that the RTV difference score, which captures change from baseline to a rewarded or fast condition, specifically measures 'state regulation'. By contrast, the interpretation of RTV baseline (slow, unrewarded) scores is debated. We aimed to investigate directly the degree of phenotypic and etiological overlap between RTV baseline and RTV difference scores. Method We conducted genetic model fitting analyses on go/no-go and fast task RTV data, across task conditions manipulating rewards and event rate, from a population-based twin sample (n=1314) and an ADHD and control sibling-pair sample (n=1265). RESULTS: Phenotypic and genetic/familial correlations were consistently high (0.72-0.98) between RTV baseline and difference scores, across tasks, manipulations and samples. By contrast, correlations were low between RTV in the manipulated condition and difference scores. A comparison across two different go/no-go task RTV difference scores (slow-fast/slow-incentive) showed high phenotypic and genetic/familial overlap (r = 0.75-0.83). CONCLUSIONS: Our finding that RTV difference scores measure largely the same etiological process as RTV under baseline condition supports theories emphasizing the malleability of the observed high RTV. Given the statistical shortcomings of difference scores, we recommend the use of RTV baseline scores for most analyses, including genetic analyses.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Tiempo de Reacción/genética , Gemelos/genética , Adolescente , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Modelos Genéticos , Fenotipo , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Gemelos/psicología , Gemelos Dicigóticos/genética , Gemelos Dicigóticos/psicología , Gemelos Monocigóticos/genética , Gemelos Monocigóticos/psicología
2.
Psychol Med ; 41(4): 861-71, 2011 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20522277

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Twin and sibling studies have identified specific cognitive phenotypes that may mediate the association between genes and the clinical symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). ADHD is also associated with lower IQ scores. We aimed to investigate whether the familial association between measures of cognitive performance and the clinical diagnosis of ADHD is mediated through shared familial influences with IQ. METHOD: Multivariate familial models were run on data from 1265 individuals aged 6-18 years, comprising 920 participants from ADHD sibling pairs and 345 control participants. Cognitive assessments included a four-choice reaction time (RT) task, a go/no-go task, a choice-delay task and an IQ assessment. The analyses focused on the cognitive variables of mean RT (MRT), RT variability (RTV), commission errors (CE), omission errors (OE) and choice impulsivity (CI). RESULTS: Significant familial association (rF) was confirmed between cognitive performance and both ADHD (rF=0.41-0.71) and IQ (rF=-0.25 to -0.49). The association between ADHD and cognitive performance was largely independent (80-87%) of any contribution from etiological factors shared with IQ. The exception was for CI, where 49% of the overlap could be accounted for by the familial variance underlying IQ. CONCLUSIONS: The aetiological factors underlying lower IQ in ADHD seem to be distinct from those between ADHD and RT/error measures. This suggests that lower IQ does not account for the key cognitive impairments observed in ADHD. The results have implications for molecular genetic studies designed to identify genes involved in ADHD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/genética , Trastornos del Conocimiento/psicología , Inteligencia/genética , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Fenotipo , Adolescente , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Niño , Conducta de Elección , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Europa (Continente) , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Control Interno-Externo , Masculino , Análisis Multivariante , Determinación de la Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Psicometría , Tiempo de Reacción/genética , Recompensa
3.
Psychol Med ; 40(12): 2089-100, 2010 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20380783

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) is frequently co-occurring with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children and adolescents. Because ODD is a precursor of later conduct disorder (CD) and affective disorders, early diagnostic identification is warranted. Furthermore, the predictability of three recently confirmed ODD dimensions (ODD-irritable, ODD-headstrong and ODD-hurtful) may assist clinical decision making. METHOD: Receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) analysis was used in order to test the diagnostic accuracy of the Conners' Parent Rating Scale revised (CPRS-R) and the parent version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (PSDQ) in the prediction of ODD in a transnational sample of 1093 subjects aged 5-17 years from the International Multicentre ADHD Genetics study. In a second step, the prediction of three ODD dimensions by the same parent rating scales was assessed by backward linear regression analyses. RESULTS: ROC analyses showed adequate diagnostic accuracy of the CPRS-R and the PSDQ in predicting ODD in this ADHD sample. Furthermore, the three-dimensional structure of ODD was confirmed by confirmatory factor analysis and the CPRS-R emotional lability scale significantly predicted the ODD irritable dimension. CONCLUSIONS: The PSDQ and the CPRS-R are both suitable screening instruments in the identification of ODD. The emotional lability scale of the CPRS-R is an adequate predictor of irritability in youth referred for ADHD.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/diagnóstico , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/psicología , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Adolescente , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/complicaciones , Déficit de la Atención y Trastornos de Conducta Disruptiva/complicaciones , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Responsabilidad Parental , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Pronóstico , Curva ROC , Análisis de Regresión
4.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet ; 153B(1): 97-102, 2010 Jan 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19388000

RESUMEN

We [Hawi et al. (2005); Am J Hum Genet 77:958-965] reported paternal over-transmission of risk alleles in some ADHD-associated genes. This was particularly clear in the case of the DAT1 3'-UTR VNTR. In the current investigation, we analyzed three new sample comprising of 1,248 ADHD nuclear families to examine the allelic over-transmission of DAT1 in ADHD. The IMAGE sample, the largest of the three-replication samples, provides strong support for a parent of origin effect for allele 6 and the 10 repeat allele (intron 8 and 3'-UTR VNTR, respectively) of DAT1. In addition, a similar pattern of over-transmission of paternal risk haplotypes (constructed from the above alleles) was also observed. Some support is also derived from the two smaller samples although neither is independently significant. Although the mechanism driving the paternal over-transmission of the DAT risk alleles is not known, these finding provide further support for this phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Alelos , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Proteínas de Transporte de Dopamina a través de la Membrana Plasmática/genética , Impresión Genómica , Haplotipos , Regiones no Traducidas 3' , Humanos , Repeticiones de Minisatélite
5.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol ; 30: 102-113, 2020 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30292416

RESUMEN

Known comorbidities for Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) include conduct problems, substance use disorder and gaming. Comorbidity with conduct problems may increase the risk for substance use disorder and gaming in individuals with ADHD. The aim of the study was to build a causal model of the relationships between ADHD and comorbid conduct problems, and alcohol, nicotine, and other substance use, and gaming habits, while accounting for age and sex. We used a state-of-the-art causal discovery algorithm to analyze a case-only sample of 362 ADHD-diagnosed individuals in the ages 12-24 years. We found that conduct problem severity mediates between ADHD severity and nicotine use, but not with more severe alcohol or substance use. More severe ADHD-inattentive symptoms lead to more severe gaming habits. Furthermore, our model suggests that ADHD severity has no influence on severity of alcohol or other drug use. Our findings suggest that ADHD severity is a risk factor for nicotine use, and that this effect is fully mediated by conduct problem severity. Finally, ADHD-inattentive severity was a risk factor for gaming, suggesting that gaming dependence has a different causal pathway than substance dependence and should be treated differently. By identifying these intervention points, our model can aid both researchers and clinicians.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/psicología , Trastorno de la Conducta/psicología , Trastorno de Adicción a Internet/psicología , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/psicología , Juegos de Video/psicología , Adolescente , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Teorema de Bayes , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Preescolar , Trastorno de la Conducta/diagnóstico , Trastorno de la Conducta/epidemiología , Femenino , Humanos , Trastorno de Adicción a Internet/diagnóstico , Trastorno de Adicción a Internet/epidemiología , Masculino , Factores de Riesgo , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/diagnóstico , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias/epidemiología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
6.
Mol Psychiatry ; 13(5): 514-21, 2008 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18180756

RESUMEN

As part of the International Multi-centre ADHD Genetics project we completed an affected sibling pair study of 142 narrowly defined Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) proband-sibling pairs. No linkage was observed on the most established ADHD-linked genomic regions of 5p and 17p. We found suggestive linkage signals on chromosomes 9 and 16, respectively, with the highest multipoint nonparametric linkage signal on chromosome 16q23 at 99 cM (log of the odds, LOD=3.1) overlapping data published from the previous UCLA (University of California, Los Angeles) (LOD>1, approximately 95 cM) and Dutch (LOD>1, approximately 100 cM) studies. The second highest peak in this study was on chromosome 9q22 at 90 cM (LOD=2.13); both the previous UCLA and German studies also found some evidence of linkage at almost the same location (UCLA LOD=1.45 at 93 cM; German LOD=0.68 at 100 cM). The overlap of these two main peaks with previous findings suggests that loci linked to ADHD may lie within these regions. Meta-analysis or reanalysis of the raw data of all the available ADHD linkage scan data may help to clarify whether these represent true linked loci.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Cromosomas Humanos Par 16/genética , Cromosomas Humanos Par 9/genética , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Niño , Comorbilidad , Europa (Continente)/epidemiología , Europa (Continente)/etnología , Femenino , Genotipo , Humanos , Israel/epidemiología , Escala de Lod , Masculino , Variaciones Dependientes del Observador , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Hermanos , Estados Unidos/epidemiología , Población Blanca/genética
7.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet ; 147B(8): 1564-7, 2008 Dec 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18937296

RESUMEN

Replication is a key to resolving whether a reported genetic association represents a false positive finding or an actual genetic risk factor. In a previous study screening 51 candidate genes for association with ADHD in a multi-centre European sample (the IMAGE project), two single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within the norepinephrine transporter (SLC6A2) gene were found to be associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The same SNP alleles were also reported to be associated with ADHD in a separate study from the Massachusetts General Hospital in the US. Using two independent samples of ADHD DSM-IV combined subtype trios we attempted to replicate the reported associations with SNPs rs11568324 and rs3785143 in SLC6A2. Significant association of the two markers was not observed in the two independent replication samples. However, across all four datasets the overall evidence of association with ADHD was significant (for SNP rs11568324 P = 0.0001; average odds ratio = 0.33; for SNP rs3785143 P = 0.008; average odds ratio = 1.3). The data were consistent for rs11568324, suggesting the existence of a rare allele conferring protection for ADHD within the SLC6A2 gene. Further investigations should focus on identifying the mechanisms underlying the protective effect.


Asunto(s)
Alelos , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Proteínas de Transporte de Noradrenalina a través de la Membrana Plasmática/genética , Adolescente , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Niño , Preescolar , Frecuencia de los Genes , Marcadores Genéticos , Genotipo , Heterocigoto , Humanos , Intrones , Madres/estadística & datos numéricos , Estudios Multicéntricos como Asunto , Oportunidad Relativa , Padres , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Hermanos
8.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet ; 147B(8): 1519-23, 2008 Dec 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18668530

RESUMEN

Multiple studies have reported an association between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and the 10-repeat allele of a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphism in the 3'-untranslated region (3'UTR) of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1). Yet, recent meta-analyses of available data find little or no evidence for this association; although there is strong evidence for heterogeneity between datasets. This pattern of findings could arise for several reasons including the presence of relatively rare risk alleles on common haplotype backgrounds or the functional interaction of two or more loci within the gene. We previously described the importance of a specific haplotype at the 3' end of DAT1, as well as the identification of associated single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within or close to 5' regulatory sequences. In this study we replicate the association of SNPs at the 5' end of the gene and identify a specific risk haplotype spanning the 5' and 3' markers. These findings indicate the presence of at least two loci associated with ADHD within the DAT1 gene and suggest that either additive or interaction effects of these two loci on the risk for ADHD. Overall these data provide further evidence that genetic variants of the dopamine transporter gene confer an increased risk for ADHD.


Asunto(s)
Regiones no Traducidas 5'/genética , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Proteínas de Transporte de Dopamina a través de la Membrana Plasmática/genética , Heterogeneidad Genética , Variación Genética , Alelos , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Europa (Continente) , Frecuencia de los Genes , Marcadores Genéticos , Haplotipos , Humanos , Desequilibrio de Ligamiento , Repeticiones de Microsatélite , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Regiones Promotoras Genéticas , Factores de Riesgo , Población Blanca
9.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet ; 147B(1): 94-9, 2008 Jan 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17525975

RESUMEN

Season of birth (SOB) has been associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in two existing studies. One further study reported an interaction between SOB and genotypes of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene. It is important that these findings are further investigated to confirm or refute the findings. In this study, we investigated the SOB association with ADHD in four independent samples collected for molecular genetic studies of ADHD and found a small but significant increase in summer births compared to a large population control dataset. We also observed a significant association with the 7-repeat allele of the DRD4 gene variable number tandem repeat polymorphism in exon three with probands born in the winter season, with no significant differential transmission of this allele between summer and winter seasons. Preferential transmission of the 2-repeat allele to ADHD probands occurred in those who were born during the summer season, but did not surpass significance for association, even though the difference in transmission between the two seasons was nominally significant. However, following adjustment for multiple testing of alleles none of the SOB effects remained significant. We conclude that the DRD4 7-repeat allele is associated with ADHD but there is no association or interaction with SOB for increased risk for ADHD. Our findings suggest that we can refute a possible effect of SOB for ADHD.


Asunto(s)
Alelos , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Parto , Receptores de Dopamina D4/genética , Estaciones del Año , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Desequilibrio de Ligamiento , Masculino
10.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet ; 147B(7): 1306-9, 2008 Oct 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18452186

RESUMEN

Several independent studies have reported association between serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) polymorphisms and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Five studies found evidence for association between the long-allele of a 44-bp insertion/deletion polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and ADHD. Another two studies corroborated this finding while a further six studies did not find such an association. For a second polymorphism within the gene, a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) within intron 2, one study demonstrated that the 12/12 genotype was significantly less frequent in ADHD cases compared to controls, while a second study found that the 12-allele was preferentially transmitted to offspring affected with ADHD. To provide further clarification of the reported associations, we investigated the association of these two markers with ADHD in a sample of 1,020 families with 1,166 combined type ADHD cases for the International Multi-Centre ADHD Genetics project, using the Transmission Disequilibrium Test. Given the large body of work supporting the association of the promoter polymorphism and mood disorders, we further analyzed the group of subjects with ADHD plus mood disorder separately. No association was found between either of the two markers and ADHD in our large multisite study or with depression within the sample of ADHD cases.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/genética , Polimorfismo Genético/genética , Proteínas de Transporte de Serotonina en la Membrana Plasmática/genética , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Depresión/genética , Salud de la Familia , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad/genética , Humanos , Trastornos del Humor/genética
11.
Prog Neurobiol ; 29(4): 365-91, 1987.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3039581

RESUMEN

An attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity in children (ADDH) is now recognized in most countries although diagnostic practices differ. Evidence is presented to show that the two cardinal symptoms of poor attentional performance and high motor activity may be functionally and causally separate. Both are temporarily relieved in a proportion of subjects that respond to psychostimulants. Beneficial treatment decreases nonadrenergic metabolism and normalizes variable levels of dopaminergic metabolism. Parallels are drawn with other clinical syndromes arising from changed catecholaminergic activity and with behavioral interpretations of the result of damage to the dorsal noradrenergic bundle and dopaminergic A10 nucleus. Prognosis of ADDH subjects after treatment remains poor. There may be a further defect of neurotransmitter metabolism in the ADDH syndrome. Research strategies are suggested based on the neurobiological correlates of the cognitive style of ADDH subjects and septal function in the animal model of the hypertensive rat.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Catecolaminas/fisiología , Animales , Perros , Humanos , Ratas , Ratas Endogámicas SHR , Ratas Endogámicas WKY , Transmisión Sináptica
12.
Schizophr Res ; 76(1): 25-41, 2005 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15927796

RESUMEN

Mismatch negativity (MMN) is an event-related potential measure of auditory change detection. It is widely reported to be smaller in patients with schizophrenia and may not improve along with otherwise successful clinical treatment. The main aim of this report is to explore ways of measuring and presenting four features of frequency-deviant MMN dipole sources (dipole moment, peak latency, brain location and orientation) and to relate these to the processes of psychopathology and illness progression. Data from early onset patients (EOS) at the start of the illness in adolescence, and others who had their first break in adolescence 15 years ago (S-15Y) were compared with two groups of age-matched healthy controls (C-EOS, C-15Y). A four-source model fitted the MMN waveform recorded from all four groups, whether MMN amplitude was more (EOS) or less (S-15Y) reduced. The locations were in the left superior temporal and anterior cingulate gyri, right superior temporal and inferior/mid frontal cortices. Dipole latencies confirmed a bottom-up sequence of processing and dipole moments were larger in the temporal lobes and on the left. Patients showed small dipole location changes that were more marked in the S-15Y than the EOS group (more rostral for the left anterior cingulate, more caudal for the right mid-frontal dipole) consistent with illness progression. The modelling of MMN dipole sources on brain atlas and anatomical images suggests that there is a degree of dissociation during illness between small progressive anatomical changes and some functional recovery indexed by scalp recordings from patients with an onset in adolescence 15 years before compared to adolescents in their first episode.


Asunto(s)
Variación Contingente Negativa/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiopatología , Esquizofrenia/diagnóstico , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Enfermedad Crónica , Progresión de la Enfermedad , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Giro del Cíngulo/fisiopatología , Humanos , Masculino , Valores de Referencia , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Sensibilidad y Especificidad
13.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 9(2): 261-82, 1985.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3925395

RESUMEN

Neuronal catecholaminergic activity modulates central nervous function. Specifically noradrenaline can exert a tuning or biassing function whereby the signal to noise ratio is altered. Dopamine activity may promote switching between inputs and outputs of information to specific brain regions. It has been ten years since evidence for a tuning function was advanced for noradrenaline and in the last 5 years the switching hypothesis for dopamine has been tentatively put forward. Recent studies are reviewed to show that while catecholamine activity contributes to neural interactions in separate brain regions that give rise to the organization of different functions, their working principles may be common between species and independent of the nucleus of origin. Behavioral examples are discussed and an attempt is made to integrate this with evidence from intracellular recording studies. It is suggested that the tuning principle in noradrenergic systems is particularly important for the formation of associations and neural plasticity (interference control) and that the switching principle of dopaminergic systems modulates the timing, time-sharing and initiation of responses (program-control).


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Dopamina/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Motivación/fisiología , Norepinefrina/fisiología , Fibras Adrenérgicas/fisiología , Anfetamina/farmacología , Animales , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Cuerpo Estriado/fisiología , Estimulación Eléctrica , Humanos , Hidroxidopaminas/farmacología , Sistema Límbico/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Oxidopamina , Psicofisiología/métodos , Ratas , Sustancia Negra/fisiología
14.
Biol Psychiatry ; 17(2): 243-58, 1982 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7074182

RESUMEN

An ability to distinguish relevance from irrelevance has been attributed to an attention-related mechanism and may be disturbed in thought-disordered schizophrenics. Stimulus choice strategies depend on such mechanisms (ia) and are anomalous in some schizophrenics. An impaired function of the ventral tegmentum (VTA) has been postulated for schizophrenia. The effects of VTA damage on the relevance/irrelevance distinction and strategy formation in rats has been studied. Over a 5 day-period food-deprived rats were given nine sessions of ten trials each on a 16-hole board. They searched for food pellets placed consistently in four holes. During testing the control group (C) reduced the number of empty holes visited more than the group with VTA damage. The proportion of repeated visits to relevant holes (had food) to irrelevant holes (never had food) increased for the C but not for the VTA group. The frequency with which a preferred sequence of food-hole visits was repeated during a session increased over sessions for the C but not the VTA group. The VTA group changed their preference between sessions more often. Animals with VTA damage were capable of simple learning, but were impaired when complexity increased. This may be due in part to a deficit in attention-related mechanisms. This encourages further study of the contribution of the VTA to putative attentional dysfunction and the use of the hole-board search task as a model for the study of cognitive function and dysfunction.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Aprendizaje Discriminativo/fisiología , Sistema Límbico/fisiología , Modelos Biológicos , Esquizofrenia , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Animales , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Dopamina/fisiología , Conducta Exploratoria/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Privación de Alimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Ratas , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología
15.
Biol Psychiatry ; 41(12): 1196-210, 1997 Jun 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9171910

RESUMEN

Mismatch negativity (MMN), in the deviant-minus-standard event-related potential (ERP) difference-waveform, may represent a working memory trace of the tone difference. Most but not all studies find MMN reduced in schizophrenic patients. This report investigates if differences may be attributable to experimental condition (diffuse vs focused attention), component identification (N1-like vs N2-like), topographic distribution, and clinical condition (with/without paranoid-hallucinatory symptoms, PH/NP). Comparisons were made for 12 PH, 12 NP schizophrenic patients with 13 obsessive compulsive and 25 normal control subjects. Frontal MMN reduction in schizophrenics largely resulted from an absence of an increase in focused attention conditions as in comparison groups. But there was a marked temporal activity locus in NP patients. These features were not reflected in other components except for a visible but nonsignificant N1-like temporal locus in NP patients. Further, schizophrenic patients did not show an increase in late positivity with focused attention like the comparison groups. The results show that so-called automatic processing deficits (amount and locus of MMN) are best seen in situations requiring the activation of controlled attentional processes. It is suggested that impaired processing of irrelevant stimuli and reduced frontal MMN in NP patients may reflect reduced dopaminergic responsivity.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/fisiopatología , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Esquizofrenia Paranoide/fisiopatología , Esquizofrenia Paranoide/psicología , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica
16.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 19(4): 373-85, 1994.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8047641

RESUMEN

Psychosis is reported to show a later age of onset in women than in men and its nature and course in women may also differ. The purpose of this study was to determine if levels of four steroid hormones at the start of early onset psychosis differ from the levels of other groups of young people and if predicted low levels of estrogen (E2) are a feature of female psychosis. Two blood samples from 22 young psychotic patients were analysed by radioimmunoassay for E2, progesterone (PROG), testosterone (TE), and dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate (DHEAS). Female psychotic patients showed E2 levels lower than matched healthy cycling controls but higher than those on a contraceptive pill; they also showed higher TE levels than controls. Male psychotic patients had higher DHEAS levels than healthy or obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) subjects. We suggest that illness-related changes of steroids can be measured superimposed on medication-induced changes and that lower E2 levels in psychotic women may increase their vulnerability to psychosis. Changes of TE in female and DHEAS in male psychotics may be more a consequence of the illness.


Asunto(s)
Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/sangre , Esquizofrenia/sangre , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Deshidroepiandrosterona/análogos & derivados , Deshidroepiandrosterona/sangre , Sulfato de Deshidroepiandrosterona , Dopamina/fisiología , Estradiol/sangre , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ciclo Menstrual/fisiología , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/sangre , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Progesterona/sangre , Maduración Sexual/fisiología , Testosterona/sangre
17.
Schizophr Res ; 49(1-2): 157-70, 2001 Apr 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11343874

RESUMEN

Measures of selective attention processing like latent inhibition (LI) and conditioned blocking (CB) are disturbed in some patients with schizophrenia. [LI is the delay in learning about the associations of a stimulus that has been associated with no event (versus de novo learning); CB is the delay in learning the associations of a stimulus-component when the other component has already started to acquire these associations.] We proposed: (1) to replicate the reported decreases of CB in patients without paranoid-hallucinatory symptoms; (2) to see if CB depends on the age of illness-onset and its duration, as reported for LI. We studied 101 young and old, acute and chronically ill patients with schizophrenia, of whom 62 learned a modified 'mouse-in-house' CB task, and compared them with 62 healthy controls matched for age, education and socio-economic background. CB was more evident in patients with a diagnosis of paranoid schizophrenia than other subtypes. An unusual persistence of high CB scores through testing was associated with productive symptoms (including positive thought disorder). Reduced CB related to the expression of (a) Schneider's first rank symptoms of ideas-of-reference and (b) to negative symptoms like poor rapport and poor attention. CB was less evident in the older patients and those with an earlier illness-onset. In contrast to the similar LI test of selective attention, CB is found in patients with paranoid schizophrenia and its expression is not related closely to illness duration. This implies that the two tests reflect the activity of different underlying processes. We suggest that reduced CB on initial test-trials in nonparanoid schizophrenia reflects the unusual persistence of controlled information processing strategies that would normally become automatic during conditioning. In contrast, continued CB during testing reflects an unusual persistence of automatic processing strategies.


Asunto(s)
Condicionamiento Psicológico/fisiología , Trastornos Psicóticos/diagnóstico , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatología , Enfermedad Aguda , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Enfermedad Crónica , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Trastornos del Conocimiento/fisiopatología , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Masculino , Trastornos Psicóticos/fisiopatología , Tiempo de Reacción , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Pensamiento/fisiología
18.
Neuroreport ; 6(8): 1187-90, 1995 May 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7662904

RESUMEN

MMN is a negative component resulting from the difference in event-related potential (ERP) waveforms elicited by a standard and a deviant stimulus. It is usually studied in the absence of attentional requirements. We compared this measure of perceptual comparison in a non-task situation (three tones presented) with that obtained in a task requiring focused attention and response to the third tone. MMN (comparison of standard and deviant irrelevant tones) increased with focused attention to the third (target) tone and frontal maxima shifted slightly posteriorly. The succeeding P3 in the difference waveform increased more posteriorly than frontally confirming continued differential processing of irrelevant stimuli under active conditions. This demonstrates that not only attending to stimuli, but the active processing of irrelevant stimuli (vs passive perception) involves small changes in the amount and distribution of neural activity.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
19.
J Psychiatr Res ; 30(5): 369-90, 1996.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8923341

RESUMEN

Conditioned blocking (CB) refers to a delay in learning that a new stimulus, added during learning, has the same consequences as the conditioned stimulus already present. In animals such "learned inattention" depends on monoaminergic and limbic function and, thus, CB performance should be informative on selective information processing impairments found in subgroups of psychotic patients. Attenuated CB in acute schizophrenia has been reported to normalize rapidly. This study examines in young patients the specificity of CB performance to illness, and its associations with symptoms, personality traits and monoaminergic metabolic status. CB was attenuated in psychotic patients with non-paranoid symptoms (NP: n = 12, mean age 17 years) with respect to obsessive-compulsive (OCD: n = 13, mean age 16 years) and healthy subjects (CON, n = 29, mean age 18 years), but only a transient attenuation was observed in paranoid hallucinatory patients (PH: n = 14, mean age 19 years). Outgoing personality traits in CON and OCD subjects correlated with CB. In NP patients attenuated CB was associated with increasing neurotic lability. In PH patients CB correlated positively with "manic" but negatively with psychotic or neurotic scores. The severity of negative symptoms in psychosis and specific negative/positive symptoms in the NP/PH groups was associated with reduced CB. Increased dopamine activity (24-h urine samples) correlated positively with CB, but relative increases of noradrenaline metabolism in NP and serotonin metabolism in OCD patients interfered. In summary, marked psychotic or neurotic traits and some symptom-states were associated with reduced CB. The particular selective processing problems of NP patients may reflect inappropriate NA activity.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/metabolismo , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Trastornos Paranoides/metabolismo , Trastornos Paranoides/psicología , Personalidad , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
20.
Behav Brain Res ; 88(1): 115-31, 1997 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9401715

RESUMEN

Reversal, and intra-dimensional (ID) and extra-dimensional (ED) nonreversal discrimination shifts were studied to see if learned inattention to the irrelevant dimension differentially influenced the efficacy of learning and stimulus choice strategy. Performance was compared with conditioned blocking (CB) and monoamine metabolic status between healthy controls, patients with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) or schizophrenia with (PH) or without (NP) active paranoid hallucinatory symptoms. PH and NP patients improved learning with practice, but showed an impaired shift on each task. OCD patients were impaired only on the ED-shift. The NP patient's impairment was nonspecific and, unlike PH and controls, it related to reversal performance. All subjects acquired an attentional set for colour reflected in the length of stimulus-response sequences. Analysis of paired-stimulus choice-strategies showed that while all patients showed fewer correct win-stay choices, only PH patients perseverated with lose-stay choices. Learning about the added stimulus in the CB task related to ID-shift efficiency in NP patients. Increases of dopamine activity related to delayed learning but more switches of stimulus choice in the shift-tasks. Increases of serotonin activity correlated with faster learning in controls, OCD and PH patients. In NP patients the opposite held for dopamine and serotonin activity. Thus the two learned inattention tasks have different if related requirements and correlates: the data are consistent with the use of automatic exogenous attention strategies by NP patients, of inefficient controlled attention by PH patients and the automatization of endogenous processes in controls.


Asunto(s)
Monoaminas Biogénicas/metabolismo , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Esquizofrenia Paranoide/psicología , Psicología del Esquizofrénico , Adolescente , Adulto , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Dopamina/metabolismo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/metabolismo , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Esquizofrenia Paranoide/metabolismo , Serotonina/metabolismo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA