Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 17 de 17
Filtrar
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(29): 7521-7526, 2018 07 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29959208

RESUMEN

Biologists and social scientists have long tried to understand why some societies have more fluid and open interpersonal relationships and how those differences influence culture. This study measures relational mobility, a socioecological variable quantifying voluntary (high relational mobility) vs. fixed (low relational mobility) interpersonal relationships. We measure relational mobility in 39 societies and test whether it predicts social behavior. People in societies with higher relational mobility report more proactive interpersonal behaviors (e.g., self-disclosure and social support) and psychological tendencies that help them build and retain relationships (e.g., general trust, intimacy, self-esteem). Finally, we explore ecological factors that could explain relational mobility differences across societies. Relational mobility was lower in societies that practiced settled, interdependent subsistence styles, such as rice farming, and in societies that had stronger ecological and historical threats.


Asunto(s)
Agricultura , Conducta Social , Movilidad Social , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
2.
Curr Res Ecol Soc Psychol ; 4: 100089, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36685995

RESUMEN

Given that mask-wearing proved to be an important tool to slow the spread of infection during the COVID-19 pandemic, investigating the psychological and cultural factors that influence norms for mask wearing across cultures is exceptionally important. One factor that may influence mask wearing behavior is the degree to which people believe masks potentially impair emotion recognition. Based on previous research suggesting that there may be cultural differences in facial regions that people in Japan and the United States attend to when inferring a target's emotional state, we predicted that Americans would perceive masks (which cover the mouth) as more likely to impair emotion recognition, whereas Japanese would perceive facial coverings that conceal the eye region (sunglasses) to be more likely to impair emotion recognition. The results showed that Japanese participants reported wearing masks more than Americans. Americans also reported higher expected difficulty in interpreting emotions of individuals wearing masks (vs. sunglasses), while Japanese reported the reverse effect. Importantly, expectations about the negative impact of facial masks on emotion recognition explained cultural differences in mask-wearing behavior, even accounting for existing social norms.

3.
Front Psychol ; 12: 648042, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34646188

RESUMEN

In this paper, we examine whether relational mobility (RM) (the ability for individuals to voluntarily form and terminate relationships within a given social environment) on a country level related to individuals' tendencies to restrict their movement following the onset of the global COVID-19 pandemic and following the issuance of stay-at-home orders in their country. We use data on geographic mobility, composed of records of geolocation information provided via mobile phones, to examine changes in geographic mobility at the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. We show that individuals in countries with higher RM tended to decrease their geographic mobility more than those in countries with lower RM following the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. Similar results were found for wealth gross domestic product (GDP), but were independent of RM. These results suggest that individuals in countries with higher RM were more responsive to calls to reduce geographic mobility.

4.
Psychol Sci ; 21(10): 1471-8, 2010 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20817913

RESUMEN

In the current research, we tested a novel explanation for previously demonstrated findings that East Asians disclose less personal information to other people than do Westerners. We propose that both between- and within-culture differences in self-disclosure to close friends may be explained by the construct of relational mobility, the general degree to which individuals in a society have opportunities to form new relationships and terminate old ones. In Study 1, we found that cross-cultural differences (Japan vs. United States) in self-disclosure to a close friend were mediated by individuals' perceptions of relational mobility. In Study 2, two separate measures of relational mobility predicted self-disclosure within a single culture (Japan), and this relationship was mediated by the motivation to engage in self-disclosure to strengthen personal relationships. We conclude that societies and social contexts higher in relational mobility (in which relationships can be formed and dissolved relatively easily) produce stronger incentives for self-disclosure as a social-commitment device.


Asunto(s)
Comparación Transcultural , Amigos/psicología , Autorrevelación , Percepción Social , Valores Sociales , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Juicio , Masculino , Motivación , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
5.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 105(1-2): 130-7, 2010.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19857876

RESUMEN

The purpose of the current study was to examine the role of theory of mind in fairness-related behavior in preschoolers and to introduce a tool for examining fairness-related behavior in children. A total of 68 preschoolers played the Ultimatum Game in a face-to-face setting. Acquisition of theory of mind was defined as the understanding of false beliefs using the Sally-Anne task. The results showed that preschoolers who had acquired theory of mind proposed higher mean offers than children who had not acquired theory of mind. These findings imply that the ability to infer the mental states of others plays an important role in fairness-related behavior.


Asunto(s)
Justicia Social , Teoría de la Mente , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Preescolar , Ética , Femenino , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Masculino
6.
Curr Opin Psychol ; 32: 129-132, 2020 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31491705

RESUMEN

Relational mobility is a socio-ecological variable that represents how much freedom and opportunity a society affords individuals to select and replace interpersonal relationships based on their personal preferences. As a socio-ecological dimension of variation in human societies, relational mobility can vary between countries, regions, and different points in history. In this article, we review evidence on how societal differences in relational mobility may lead to differences in behavioral and psychological tendencies of people who reside there. We particularly focus on two sets of consequences of relational mobility found by new studies: interpersonal strategies, such as passionate love and commitment behavior, as well as cultural thinking styles, such as attribution and attention.


Asunto(s)
Cultura , Relaciones Interpersonales , Conducta Social , Medio Social , Pensamiento , Humanos
7.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 119(4): 861-880, 2020 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31815500

RESUMEN

Researchers commonly conceptualize forgiveness as a rich complex of psychological changes involving attitudes, emotions, and behaviors. Psychometric work with the measures developed to capture this conceptual richness, however, often points to a simpler picture of the psychological dimensions in which forgiveness takes place. In an effort to better unite forgiveness theory and measurement, we evaluate several psychometric models for common measures of forgiveness. In doing so, we study people from the United States and Japan to understand forgiveness in both nonclose and close relationships. In addition, we assess the predictive utility of these models for several behavioral outcomes that traditionally have been linked to forgiveness motives. Finally, we use the methods of item response theory, which place person abilities and item responses on the same metric and, thus, help us draw psychological inferences from the ordering of item difficulties. Our results highlight models based on correlated factors models and bifactor (S-1) models. The bifactor (S-1) model evinced particular utility: Its general factor consistently predicts variation in relevant criterion measures, including 4 different experimental economic games (when played with a transgressor), and also suffuses a second self-report measure of forgiveness. Moreover, the general factor of the bifactor (S-1) model identifies a single psychological dimension that runs from hostility to friendliness while also pointing to other sources of variance that may be conceived of as method factors. Taken together, these results suggest that forgiveness can be usefully conceptualized as prosocial change along a single attitudinal continuum that ranges from hostility to friendliness. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Perdón , Hostilidad , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adulto , Actitud , Emociones , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Motivación , Psicometría , Estados Unidos
8.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 116(4): 495-518, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30614727

RESUMEN

We hypothesized that individuals in cultures typified by lower levels of relational mobility would tend to show more attention to the surrounding social and physical context (i.e., holistic vs. analytic thinking) compared with individuals in higher mobility cultural contexts. Six studies provided support for this idea. Studies 1a and 1b showed that differences in relational mobility in cultures as diverse as the U.S., Spain, Israel, Nigeria, and Morocco predicted patterns of dispositional bias as well as holistic (vs. analytic) attention. Study 2 demonstrated that, for Americans and Japanese, relational mobility offered better predictive validity of these cognitive tendencies than related cultural constructs; moreover, Studies 1b and 2 showed that relational mobility mediated cross-cultural differences in perception and attribution. Studies 3a and 3b showed that lower relational mobility induces a weaker sense of internal locus of control and a stronger sense of external locus of control, which led to more holistic (vs. analytic) cognition. Last, Study 4 replicated these results in an experimental setting and demonstrated the causal effect of relational mobility on analytic/holistic cognition. Overall, we suggest that relational mobility may be an important socioecological factor that can help explain robust cognitive differences observed across cultures. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Atención , Cognición , Comparación Transcultural , Control Interno-Externo , Relaciones Interpersonales , Pensamiento , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Israel/etnología , Japón/etnología , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Marruecos/etnología , Nigeria/etnología , Percepción Social , España/etnología , Estados Unidos/etnología
9.
PLoS One ; 14(9): e0222492, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31560694

RESUMEN

We conducted a survey about the 2014 FIFA World Cup that measured attitudes about FIFA, players, and officials in 18 languages with 4600 respondents from 29 countries. Sixty percent of respondents perceived FIFA officials as being dishonest, and people from countries with less institutional corruption and stronger rule of law perceived FIFA officials as being more corrupt and less competent running the tournament than people from countries with more corruption and weaker rule of law. In contrast, respondents evaluated players as skilled and honest and match officials as competent and honest. We discuss the implications of our findings for perceptions of corruption in general.


Asunto(s)
Fraude , Fútbol/ética , Atletas , Actitud , Humanos , Internacionalidad , Fútbol/legislación & jurisprudencia , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
10.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 114(5): 804-824, 2018 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28240940

RESUMEN

Previous studies have shown that the maximizing orientation, reflecting a motivation to select the best option among a given set of choices, is associated with various negative psychological outcomes. In the present studies, we examined whether these relationships extend to friendship selection and how the number of options for friends moderated these effects. Across 5 studies, maximizing in selecting friends was negatively related to life satisfaction, positive affect, and self-esteem, and was positively related to negative affect and regret. In Study 1, a maximizing in selecting friends scale was created, and regret mediated the relationships between maximizing and well-being. In a naturalistic setting in Studies 2a and 2b, the tendency to maximize among those who participated in the fraternity and sorority recruitment process was negatively related to satisfaction with their selection, and positively related to regret and negative affect. In Study 3, daily levels of maximizing were negatively related to daily well-being, and these relationships were mediated by daily regret. In Study 4, we extended the findings to samples from the U.S. and Japan. When participants who tended to maximize were faced with many choices, operationalized as the daily number of friends met (Study 3) and relational mobility (Study 4), the opportunities to regret a decision increased and further diminished well-being. These findings imply that, paradoxically, attempts to maximize when selecting potential friends is detrimental to one's well-being. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Emociones , Amigos/psicología , Motivación , Satisfacción Personal , Autoimagen , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Estudiantes/psicología , Estudiantes/estadística & datos numéricos , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
11.
Front Psychol ; 7: 888, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27445881

RESUMEN

This study examined the relation between the acquisition of false-beliefs theory of mind (ToM) and reciprocity in preschoolers. Preschool-aged children completed a task assessing the understanding of false beliefs, and played an Ultimatum Game (UG) with another child in a face-to-face setting. Negative reciprocity was assessed by examining the rejection of unfair offers made by another child in the UG, while positive reciprocity was assessed by examining allocations made by participants in a Dictator Game (DG) following the UG. The results indicated that children who had passed a task assessing first-order false beliefs were more likely to make generous offers in a DG following a fair offer made by their partner in a proceeding UG, but that false beliefs ToM was unrelated to the rejection of unfair offers in the UG.

12.
PLoS One ; 11(4): e0153128, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27055206

RESUMEN

A number of studies have shown that individuals often spontaneously mimic the facial expressions of others, a tendency known as facial mimicry. This tendency has generally been considered a reflex-like "automatic" response, but several recent studies have shown that the degree of mimicry may be moderated by contextual information. However, the cognitive and motivational factors underlying the contextual moderation of facial mimicry require further empirical investigation. In this study, we present evidence that the degree to which participants spontaneously mimic a target's facial expressions depends on whether participants are motivated to infer the target's emotional state. In the first study we show that facial mimicry, assessed by facial electromyography, occurs more frequently when participants are specifically instructed to infer a target's emotional state than when given no instruction. In the second study, we replicate this effect using the Facial Action Coding System to show that participants are more likely to mimic facial expressions of emotion when they are asked to infer the target's emotional state, rather than make inferences about a physical trait unrelated to emotion. These results provide convergent evidence that the explicit goal of understanding a target's emotional state affects the degree of facial mimicry shown by the perceiver, suggesting moderation of reflex-like motor activities by higher cognitive processes.


Asunto(s)
Electromiografía/métodos , Emociones/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Adolescente , Pueblo Asiatico/psicología , Mimetismo Biológico , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Adulto Joven
13.
Sci Rep ; 6: 38662, 2016 12 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27929138

RESUMEN

This study examined the association between salivary oxytocin (sOT) levels and generosity in preschoolers. Fifty preschoolers played two dictator games (DG) by deciding how to allocate 10 chocolates between themselves and another child, who was either from the same class as the participant (ingroup member), or an unknown child from another class (outgroup member). sOT levels were assessed in saliva collected from the children immediately prior to the DG tasks. While sOT levels were negatively associated with allocations made to both ingroup and outgroup members by boys, among girl sOT levels were positively related to allocations made to ingroup members, and unrelated to allocations made to outgroup members. These results suggest sex differences in the association between salivary oxytocin and generosity.


Asunto(s)
Oxitocina/metabolismo , Saliva/metabolismo , Conducta Social , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Hormonas , Humanos , Masculino , Factores Sexuales
14.
Front Psychol ; 6: 895, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26175707

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of peer monitoring on generosity in boys and girls aged 6-12 years. A total of 120 elementary school students played a one-shot dictator game (DG) with and without peer monitoring by classmates. Children decided how to divide 10 chocolates between themselves and a classmate either in a condition in which their allocations were visible to their peers, or in private. While the effect of peer monitoring on the allocation amount in the DG was clearly present in boys, it was not observed in girls. Furthermore, the effect of peer monitoring in boys appeared at the age of 9 years. These results suggest that the motivation to draw peers' attention plays a stronger role for older boys than for girls or younger boys. The potential roles of higher-order theory of mind, social roles, and emergence of secondary sex characteristics on the influence of peer monitoring on generosity shown by boys are discussed.

15.
PLoS One ; 9(9): e108462, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25255309

RESUMEN

We conducted a simple resource allocation game known as the ultimatum game (UG) with preschoolers to examine the role of cognitive and emotional perspective-taking ability on allocation and rejection behavior. A total of 146 preschoolers played the UG and completed a false belief task and an emotional perspective-taking test. Results showed that cognitive perspective taking ability had a significant positive effect on the proposer's offer and a negative effect on the responder's rejection behavior, whereas emotional perspective taking ability did not impact either the proposer's or responder's behavior. These results imply that the ability to anticipate the responder's beliefs, but not their emotional state, plays an important role in the proposer's choice of a fair allocation in an UG, and that children who have not acquired theory of mind still reject unfair offers.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Toma de Decisiones , Emociones , Juegos Experimentales , Preescolar , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino
16.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 38(11): 1423-36, 2012 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22745332

RESUMEN

Rankings of countries on mean levels of self-reported Conscientiousness continue to puzzle researchers. Based on the hypothesis that cross-cultural differences in the tendency to prefer extreme response categories of ordinal rating scales over moderate categories can influence the comparability of self-reports, this study investigated possible effects of response style on the mean levels of self-reported Conscientiousness in 22 samples from 20 countries. Extreme and neutral responding were estimated based on respondents' ratings of 30 hypothetical people described in short vignettes. In the vignette ratings, clear cross-sample differences in extreme and neutral responding emerged. These responding style differences were correlated with mean self-reported Conscientiousness scores. Correcting self-reports for extreme and neutral responding changed sample rankings of Conscientiousness, as well as the predictive validities of these rankings for external criteria. The findings suggest that the puzzling country rankings of self-reported Conscientiousness may to some extent result from differences in response styles.


Asunto(s)
Actitud , Cultura , Personalidad , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Comparación Transcultural , Empatía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Determinación de la Personalidad , Inventario de Personalidad/estadística & datos numéricos , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Autoinforme , Percepción Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
17.
Psychol Sci ; 19(6): 579-84, 2008 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18578848

RESUMEN

In this article, we present a new framework for interpreting cultural differences in behavior -- what we call the institutional approach. In this framework, individuals' behaviors are conceptualized as strategies adapted to various incentive structures. Cultural differences in behavior are thus viewed as differences in the default adaptive strategies that individuals come to rely on in unclear situations. Through two studies, we demonstrate that the East Asian "preference" for conformity is actually a default strategy to avoid accrual of negative reputation. When the possibility for negative evaluations in a given situation was clearly defined, cultural differences in the tendency for uniqueness disappeared. This approach carries important implications to psychologists who interpret cultural differences in behavior in terms of preferences, and can serve as a common framework branching out toward other disciplines in the social sciences.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Comparación Transcultural , Conducta Social , Características Culturales , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino , Michigan , Motivación , Distribución por Sexo , Conformidad Social , Estudiantes/psicología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA