Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 27
Filtrar
1.
Proc Biol Sci ; 287(1937): 20202001, 2020 10 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33109010

RESUMEN

While widely acknowledged in the cultural evolution literature, ecological factors-aspects of the physical environment that affect the way in which cultural productions evolve-have not been investigated experimentally. Here, we present an experimental investigation of this type of factor by using a transmission chain (iterated learning) experiment. We predicted that differences in the distance between identical tools (drums) and in the order in which they are to be used would cause the evolution of different rhythms. The evidence confirms our predictions and thus provides a proof of concept that ecological factors-here a motor constraint-can influence cultural productions and that their effects can be experimentally isolated and measured. One noteworthy finding is that ecological factors can on their own lead to more complex rhythms.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Cultural , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Actividad Motora , Humanos
2.
Behav Brain Sci ; 42: e183, 2019 Sep 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31511107

RESUMEN

I argue, with examples, that most human cognitive skills are neither instincts nor gadgets but mechanisms shaped both by evolved dispositions and by cultural inputs. This shaping can work either through evolved skills fulfilling their function with the help of cultural skills that they contribute to shape, or through cultural skills recruiting evolved skills and adjusting to them.

3.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e182, 2018 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31064532

RESUMEN

Boyer & Petersen (B&P) assume that the intuitive systems underlying folk-economic beliefs (FEBs), and, in particular, emporiophobia, evolved in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness (EEA), before markets. This makes the historical development of markets puzzling. We suggest that what evolved in the EEA are templates that help children develop intuitive systems partly adjusted to their cultural environment. This helps resolve the puzzle.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Cognición , Niño , Humanos
4.
J Child Lang ; 44(5): 1041-1064, 2017 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27748210

RESUMEN

We suggest that preschoolers' frequent obliviousness to the risks and opportunities of deception comes from a trusting stance supporting verbal communication. Three studies (N = 125) confirm this hypothesis. Three-year-olds can hide information from others (Study 1) and they can lie (Study 2) in simple settings. Yet when one introduces the possibility of informing others in the very same settings, three-year-olds tend to be honest (Studies 1 and 2). Similarly, four-year-olds, though capable of treating assertions as false, trust deceptive informants (Study 3). We suggest that children's reduced sensitivity to the opportunities of lying, and to the risks of being lied to might help explain their difficulties on standard false belief tasks.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación , Cultura , Decepción , Optimismo/psicología , Confianza/psicología , Conducta Verbal , Carácter , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Prueba de Realidad , Percepción del Habla
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(4): 692-704, 2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25321487

RESUMEN

Logical connectives (e.g., or, if, and not) are central to everyday conversation, and the inferences they generate are made with little effort in pragmatically sound situations. In contrast, the neural substrates of logical inference-making have been studied exclusively in abstract tasks where pragmatic concerns are minimal. Here, we used fMRI in an innovative design that employed narratives to investigate the interaction between logical reasoning and pragmatic processing in natural discourse. Each narrative contained three premises followed by a statement. In Fully-deductive stories, the statement confirmed a conclusion that followed from two steps of disjunction-elimination (e.g., Xavier considers Thursday, Friday, or Saturday for inviting his girlfriend out; he removes Thursday before he rejects Saturday and declares "I will invite her out for Friday"). In Implicated-premise stories, an otherwise identical narrative included three premises that twice removed a single option from consideration (i.e., Xavier rejects Thursday for two different reasons). The conclusion therefore necessarily prompts an implication (i.e., Xavier must have removed Saturday from consideration as well). We report two main findings. First, conclusions of Implicated-premise stories are associated with more activity than conclusions of Fully-deductive stories in a bilateral frontoparietal system, suggesting that these regions play a role in inferring an implicated premise. Second, brain connectivity between these regions increases with pragmatic abilities when reading conclusions in Implicated-premise stories. These findings suggest that pragmatic processing interacts with logical inference-making when understanding arguments in narrative discourse.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiología , Comprensión/fisiología , Lógica , Pensamiento/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Análisis de Varianza , Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/irrigación sanguínea , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Oxígeno/sangre , Lectura , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
6.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e63, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786276

RESUMEN

As Kline envisages, there is an important relationship between cultural attraction and teaching. The very function of teaching is to make the content taught an attractor. Teaching, moreover, typically fulfills its function by exploiting a variety of factors of cultural attraction that help make its content learnable and teachable.


Asunto(s)
Diversidad Cultural , Curriculum , Humanos
7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(1): 59-78, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23445574

RESUMEN

What makes humans moral beings? This question can be understood either as a proximate "how" question or as an ultimate "why" question. The "how" question is about the mental and social mechanisms that produce moral judgments and interactions, and has been investigated by psychologists and social scientists. The "why" question is about the fitness consequences that explain why humans have morality, and has been discussed by evolutionary biologists in the context of the evolution of cooperation. Our goal here is to contribute to a fruitful articulation of such proximate and ultimate explanations of human morality. We develop an approach to morality as an adaptation to an environment in which individuals were in competition to be chosen and recruited in mutually advantageous cooperative interactions. In this environment, the best strategy is to treat others with impartiality and to share the costs and benefits of cooperation equally. Those who offer less than others will be left out of cooperation; conversely, those who offer more will be exploited by their partners. In line with this mutualistic approach, the study of a range of economic games involving property rights, collective actions, mutual help and punishment shows that participants' distributions aim at sharing the costs and benefits of interactions in an impartial way. In particular, the distribution of resources is influenced by effort and talent, and the perception of each participant's rights on the resources to be distributed.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Matrimonio , Principios Morales , Parejas Sexuales , Adulto , Conducta Cooperativa , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Conducta Social
8.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(1): 102-13, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23560336

RESUMEN

Our discussion of the commentaries begins, at the evolutionary level, with issues raised by our account of the evolution of morality in terms of partner-choice mutualism. We then turn to the cognitive level and the characterization and workings of fairness. In a final section, we discuss the degree to which our fairness-based approach to morality extends to norms that are commonly considered moral even though they are distinct from fairness.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Matrimonio , Principios Morales , Parejas Sexuales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
Sci Adv ; 9(43): eadi5282, 2023 10 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37889969

RESUMEN

Humans are strategic cooperators; we make decisions on the basis of costs and benefits to maintain high levels of cooperation, and this is thought to have played a key role in human evolution. In comparison, monkeys and apes might lack the cognitive capacities necessary to develop flexible forms of cooperation. We show that Guinea baboons (Papio papio) can use direct reciprocity and partner choice to develop and maintain high levels of cooperation in a prosocial choice task. Our findings demonstrate that monkeys have the cognitive capacities to adjust their level of cooperation strategically using a combination of partner choice and partner control strategies. Such capacities were likely present in our common ancestor and would have provided the foundations for the evolution of typically human forms of cooperation.


Asunto(s)
Hominidae , Papio papio , Animales , Humanos , Conducta Cooperativa
10.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 2917, 2022 02 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35190637

RESUMEN

When human infants are intentionally addressed by others, they tend to interpret the information communicated as being relevant to them and worth acquiring. For humans, this attribution of relevance leads to a preference to learn from communication, making it possible to accumulate knowledge over generations. Great apes are sensitive to communicative cues, but do these cues also activate an expectation of relevance? In an observational learning paradigm, we demonstrated to a sample of nonhuman great apes (bonobos, chimpanzees, orangutans; N = 24) how to operate on a food dispenser device. When apes had the opportunity to choose between an effective and an ineffective method in the baseline conditions, the majority of them chose the effective method. However, when the ineffective method was demonstrated in a communicative way, they failed to prioritize efficiency, even though they were equally attentive in both conditions. This suggests that the ostensive demonstration elicited an expectation of relevance that modified apes' interpretation of the situation, potentially leading to a preference to learn from communication, as human children do.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación Animal , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Hominidae/psicología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Animales , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Conocimiento , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Pongo
11.
Behav Brain Sci ; 34(2): 57-74; discussion 74-111, 2011 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21447233

RESUMEN

Reasoning is generally seen as a means to improve knowledge and make better decisions. However, much evidence shows that reasoning often leads to epistemic distortions and poor decisions. This suggests that the function of reasoning should be rethought. Our hypothesis is that the function of reasoning is argumentative. It is to devise and evaluate arguments intended to persuade. Reasoning so conceived is adaptive given the exceptional dependence of humans on communication and their vulnerability to misinformation. A wide range of evidence in the psychology of reasoning and decision making can be reinterpreted and better explained in the light of this hypothesis. Poor performance in standard reasoning tasks is explained by the lack of argumentative context. When the same problems are placed in a proper argumentative setting, people turn out to be skilled arguers. Skilled arguers, however, are not after the truth but after arguments supporting their views. This explains the notorious confirmation bias. This bias is apparent not only when people are actually arguing, but also when they are reasoning proactively from the perspective of having to defend their opinions. Reasoning so motivated can distort evaluations and attitudes and allow erroneous beliefs to persist. Proactively used reasoning also favors decisions that are easy to justify but not necessarily better. In all these instances traditionally described as failures or flaws, reasoning does exactly what can be expected of an argumentative device: Look for arguments that support a given conclusion, and, ceteris paribus, favor conclusions for which arguments can be found.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Toma de Decisiones , Solución de Problemas , Teoría Psicológica , Pensamiento , Evolución Biológica , Conducta de Elección , Formación de Concepto , Humanos , Conocimiento
12.
Proc Biol Sci ; 277(1681): 651-9, 2010 Feb 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19889707

RESUMEN

For acquired behaviour to count as cultural, two conditions must be met: it must propagate in a social group, and it must remain stable across generations in the process of propagation. It is commonly assumed that imitation is the mechanism that explains both the spread of animal culture and its stability. We review the literature on transmission chain studies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and other animals, and we use a formal model to argue that imitation, which may well play a major role in the propagation of animal culture, cannot be considered faithful enough to explain its stability. We consider the contribution that other psychological and ecological factors might make to the stability of animal culture observed in the wild.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Cultura , Conducta Imitativa/fisiología , Modelos Teóricos , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Aprendizaje/fisiología
14.
Behav Brain Sci ; 33(2-3): 84-5, 2010 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20550737

RESUMEN

While we agree that the cultural imbalance in the recruitment of participants in psychology experiments is highly detrimental, we emphasize the need to complement this criticism with a warning about the "weirdness" of some cross-cultural studies showing seemingly deep cultural differences. We take the example of economic games and suggest that the variety of results observed in these games may not be due to deep psychological differences per se, but rather due to different interpretations of the situation.


Asunto(s)
Ciencias de la Conducta/métodos , Comparación Transcultural , Percepción , Proyectos de Investigación , Cultura , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Grupos de Población , Conducta Social
15.
Cogn Sci ; 44(6): e12866, 2020 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32535972

RESUMEN

The spatial composition of human portraits obeys historically changing cultural norms. We show that it is also affected by cognitive factors that cause greater spontaneous attention to what is in front rather in the back of an agent. Scenes with more space in front of a directed object are both more often produced and judged as more aesthetically pleasant. This leads to the prediction that, in profile-oriented human portraits, compositions with more space in front of depicted agents (a "forward bias") should be over-represented. By analyzing a large dataset (total N of 1,831 paintings by 582 unique identified European painters from the 15th to the 20th century), we found evidence of this forward bias: Painters tended to put more free space in front of, rather than behind, the sitters. Additionally, we found evidence that this forward bias became stronger when cultural norms of spatial composition favoring centering became less stringent.


Asunto(s)
Orientación Espacial , Sesgo , Emociones , Humanos , Pinturas
16.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 18135, 2020 10 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33093599

RESUMEN

We investigated whether communicative cues help observers to make sense of human interaction. We recorded EEG from an observer monitoring two individuals who were occasionally communicating with each other via either mutual eye contact and/or pointing gestures, and then jointly attending to the same object or attending to different objects that were placed on a table in front of them. The analyses were focussed on the processing of the interaction outcome (i.e. presence or absence of joint attention) and showed that its interpretation is a two-stage process, as reflected in the N300 and the N400 potentials. The N300 amplitude was reduced when the two individuals shared their focus of attention, which indicates the operation of a cognitive process that involves the relatively fast identification and evaluation of actor-object relationships. On the other hand, the N400 was insensitive to the sharing or distribution of the two individuals' attentional focus. Interestingly, the N400 was reduced when the interaction outcome was preceded either by mutual eye contact or by a perceived pointing gesture. This shows that observation of communication "opens up" the mind to a wider range of action possibilities and thereby helps to interpret unusual outcomes of social interactions.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Comunicación , Señales (Psicología) , Potenciales Evocados , Gestos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Adulto , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
17.
PLoS One ; 14(10): e0224648, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31665195

RESUMEN

What role does children's trust in communication play in their acquisition of new meanings? To answer, we report two experimental studies (N = 81) testing how three- to four-year-olds interpret the meaning of a novel communicative device when it is used by a malevolent and potentially deceptive informant. Children participated in a hiding game in which they had to find a reward hidden in one of two boxes. In the initial phase of the experiments, a malevolent informant always indicated the location of the empty box using a novel communicative device, either a marker (Study 1), or an arrow (Study 2). During that phase, 3- and 4-year-olds learned to avoid the box indicated by the novel communicative device. In the second phase of the test, the malevolent informant was replaced by a benevolent one. Nevertheless, children did not change their search strategy, and they kept avoiding the box tagged by the novel communicative device as often as when it had been produced by the malevolent informant. These results suggest that during the initial phase, children (i) did not consider the possibility that the malevolent informant might intend to deceive them, and (ii) did not ignore the unfamiliar communicative signal or treat it as irrelevant. Instead, children relied on the unfamiliar communicative signal to locate the empty box's location. These results suggest that children's avoidance of the location indicated by an unfamiliar signal is not unambiguous evidence for distrust of such signal. We argue that children's trust in ostensive communication is likely to extend to unfamiliar communicative means, and that this presumption of trustworthiness plays a central role in children's acquisition of new meanings.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación , Confianza , Preescolar , Decepción , Humanos
18.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 8(1): 40-6, 2004 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14697402

RESUMEN

The existence and diversity of human cultures are made possible by our species-specific cognitive capacities. But how? Do cultures emerge and diverge as a result of the deployment, over generations and in different populations, of general abilities to learn, imitate and communicate? What role if any do domain-specific evolved cognitive abilities play in the emergence and evolution of cultures? These questions have been approached from different vantage points in different disciplines. Here we present a view that is currently developing out of the converging work of developmental psychologists, evolutionary psychologists and cognitive anthropologists.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Características Culturales , Diversidad Cultural , Evolución Cultural , Comunicación , Humanos , Conducta Imitativa , Aprendizaje , Conformidad Social , Identificación Social , Especificidad de la Especie
19.
Cognition ; 85(3): 277-90, 2002 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12169412

RESUMEN

Sperber, Cara, and Girotto (Cognition 52 (1995) 3) argued that, in Wason's selection task, relevance-guided comprehension processes tend to determine participants' performance and pre-empt the use of other inferential capacities. Because of this, the value of the selection task as a tool for studying human inference has been grossly overestimated. Fiddick, Cosmides, and Tooby (Cognition 77 (2000) 1) argued against Sperber et al. that specialized inferential mechanisms, in particular the "social contract algorithm" hypothesized by Cosmides (Cognition 31 (1989) 187), pre-empt more general comprehension abilities, making the selection task a useful tool after all. We rebut this argument. We argue and illustrate with two new experiments, that Fiddick et al. mix the true Wason selection task with a trivially simple categorization task superficially similar to the Wason task, yielding methodologically flawed evidence. We conclude that the extensive use of various kinds of selection tasks in the psychology of reasoning has been quite counter-productive and should be discontinued.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Toma de Decisiones , Teoría Psicológica , Algoritmos , Humanos , Distribución Aleatoria
20.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 369(1642): 20130368, 2014 May 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24686939

RESUMEN

Darwin-inspired population thinking suggests approaching culture as a population of items of different types, whose relative frequencies may change over time. Three nested subtypes of populational models can be distinguished: evolutionary, selectional and replicative. Substantial progress has been made in the study of cultural evolution by modelling it within the selectional frame. This progress has involved idealizing away from phenomena that may be critical to an adequate understanding of culture and cultural evolution, particularly the constructive aspect of the mechanisms of cultural transmission. Taking these aspects into account, we describe cultural evolution in terms of cultural attraction, which is populational and evolutionary, but only selectional under certain circumstances. As such, in order to model cultural evolution, we must not simply adjust existing replicative or selectional models but we should rather generalize them, so that, just as replicator-based selection is one form that Darwinian selection can take, selection itself is one of several different forms that attraction can take. We present an elementary formalization of the idea of cultural attraction.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Cultural , Modelos Teóricos , Dinámica Poblacional , Humanos
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA