Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 69
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Dev Psychopathol ; 35(3): 1529-1539, 2023 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35796242

RESUMEN

This study investigated the transactional relations between vocabulary and disruptive behaviors (DB; physical aggression and opposition/rule breaking/theft and vandalism), during the transition to formal schooling, using a community sample of 572 children. Cross-lagged panel model analyses were used to examine bidirectional relationships, comparing physical aggression to non-aggressive DB. Transactional associations between vocabulary and DB were observed, coinciding with school entry. Lower vocabulary in preschool (60mo.) was predictive of higher physical aggression scores in kindergarten. In turn, higher physical aggression in kindergarten was predictive of lower vocabulary in 1st grade. For non-aggressive DB, recurrent associations were found. Lower verbal skills in preschool (42mo.) and kindergarten predicted higher non-aggressive DB scores later in preschool and in 1st grade respectively. In turn, higher non-aggressive DB in kindergarten predicted lower vocabulary scores in 1st grade. In contrast to transactional paths from vocabulary to DB, transactional paths from DB to vocabulary observed after the transition to elementary school remained significant after controlling for comorbid hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention behaviors, suggesting these links were specific to aggressive and non-aggressive DB. Practical implications for prevention are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Problema de Conducta , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Vocabulario , Instituciones Académicas , Escolaridad , Agresión
2.
J Adolesc ; 95(1): 181-189, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36281743

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Adolescence is a time of increased emotional reactivity and improving cognitive control. Mindfulness meditation training may foster adolescents' cognitive control and emotional regulation skills; however little is known about the impact of mindfulness training in adolescents compared to adults. We examined the effect of mindfulness meditation versus a closely matched active control condition (relaxation training) on behavioral and neural measures of cognitive control and emotional reactivity in a small group of adolescents and adults. METHODS: Structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging data were collected before and after 8 weeks of training in 26 adolescent (12-14 years) and 17 adult (23-33 years) female participants in the United Kingdom while they completed an n-back task with emotional face distractors and an attentional control task. Participants of each group chose a class date/time and the classes were then randomly allocated to mindfulness or relaxation conditions. RESULTS: Compared to relaxation training, mindfulness training led to an increase in the speed of reorienting attention across age groups. In addition, there was preliminary evidence for reduced amygdala response to emotional face distractors in adolescents after mindfulness training. CONCLUSIONS: An 8-week mindfulness program showed similar facilitative effects in adolescent and adult females on the reorienting of attention, a skill that is repeatedly practiced during mindfulness meditation. Mindfulness also reduced left amygdala reactivity to emotional face distractors in adolescents only. Mindfulness meditation practice can therefore have a facilitative effect on female adolescents' attentional control, and possibly attenuate their emotional reactivity.


Asunto(s)
Meditación , Atención Plena , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Amígdala del Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Atención , Meditación/métodos , Meditación/psicología , Atención Plena/métodos , Neuroimagen
3.
Dev Sci ; 25(5): e13254, 2022 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35195319

RESUMEN

Whereas accuracy is used as an indicator of cognitive flexibility in preschool-age children, reaction time (RT), or a combination of accuracy and RT, provide better indices of performance as children transition to school. Theoretical models and cross-sectional studies suggest that a speed-accuracy tradeoff may be operating across this transition, but the lack of longitudinal studies makes this transition difficult to understand. The current study explored the longitudinal and bidirectional associations between accuracy and RT on the DCCS (mixed block) at 5, 6, and 7 years of age using cross-lagged panel analyses. The study also examined the roles of working memory and language, as potential longitudinal mediators between RT at Time X and accuracy at Time X + 1, and explored the role of inhibitory control. The sample consisted of 425 children from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development. Results show lagged associations from slower RT to greater improvements in accuracy between 5 and 6 years and between 6 and 7 years. Further, higher accuracy at 6 years predicted faster RT at 7 years. Only working memory acted as a partial mediator between RT at 5 years and accuracy at 6 years. These results provide needed longitudinal evidence to support theoretical claims that slower RT precedes improved accuracy in the development of cognitive flexibility, that working memory may be involved in the early stage of this process, and that accuracy and reaction time become more efficient in later stages of this process.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Niño , Preescolar , Estudios Transversales , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Tiempo de Reacción
4.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol ; 16: 431-454, 2020 05 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32075434

RESUMEN

Executive function (EF) skills are neurocognitive skills that support the reflective, top-down coordination and control of other brain functions, and there is neural and behavioral evidence for a continuum from more "cool" EF skills activated in emotionally neutral contexts to more "hot" EF skills needed for the reversal of motivationally significant tendencies. Difficulties in EF are transdiagnostic indicators of atypical development. A neurodevelopmental model traces the pathway from adverse childhood experiences and stress to disruption of the development of neural systems supporting reflection and EF skills to an increased risk for general features of psychopathology. Research indicates that EF skills can be cultivated through scaffolded training and are a promising target for therapeutic and preventive intervention. Intervention efficacy can be enhanced by mitigating disruptive bottom-up influences such as stress, training both hot and cool EF skills, and adding a reflective, metacognitive component to promote far transfer of trained skills.


Asunto(s)
Experiencias Adversas de la Infancia , Función Ejecutiva , Metacognición , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo , Plasticidad Neuronal , Estrés Psicológico , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Humanos , Metacognición/fisiología , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo/etiología , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo/fisiopatología , Trastornos del Neurodesarrollo/terapia , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Estrés Psicológico/complicaciones , Estrés Psicológico/fisiopatología , Estrés Psicológico/terapia
5.
Behav Res Methods ; 49(3): 960-966, 2017 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27325165

RESUMEN

The Developmental Emotional Faces Stimulus Set (DEFSS) is designed to provide a standardized set of emotional stimuli that includes both child and adult faces and that has been validated by participants across a wide range of ages. This article describes the creation and validation of the DEFSS, which includes 404 validated facial photographs of people between 8 and 30 years old displaying five different emotional expressions: happy, angry, fearful, sad, and neutral. The emotions in all photographs were identified correctly by 86 % of the raters (minimum 55 %), and validity did not vary as a function of the age group of the model or of the raters, indicating that the pictures are equally appropriate for use across the entire age range. Strengths and limitations of the DEFSS are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Expresión Facial , Estándares de Referencia , Adolescente , Adulto , Niño , Bases de Datos Factuales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
6.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 57(1): 30-38, 2016 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25981677

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The goal of this study is to investigate differences in executive function (EF) in children with different levels of disruptive behavior problems (DBP). METHODS: Ninety-three children between 7 and 12 years old with DBP were compared to 63 normally developing peers on a battery of EF tasks that varied in the amount of required emotion regulation ('hot' EF). RESULTS: Differences in EF were found between DBP and comparison groups as indexed by hot EF tasks. Self-reported emotion scales, in conjunction with physiological recordings of heart rate, confirmed that emotions were elicited during hot EF. CONCLUSIONS: Results suggest that difficulties in hot EF underlie externalizing problem behaviors in middle childhood.

7.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 20(6): 620-9, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24960301

RESUMEN

This study describes psychometric properties of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) executive function measures in an adult sample. The NIHTB-CB was designed for use in epidemiologic studies and clinical trials for ages 3 to 85. A total of 268 self-described healthy adults were recruited at four university-based sites, using stratified sampling guidelines to target demographic variability for age (20-85 years), gender, education and ethnicity. The NIHTB-CB contains two computer-based instruments assessing executive function: the Dimensional Change Card Sort (a measure of cognitive flexibility) and a flanker task (a measure of inhibitory control and selective attention). Participants completed the NIHTB-CB, corresponding gold standard convergent and discriminant measures, and sociodemographic questionnaires. A subset of participants (N=89) was retested 7 to 21 days later. Results reveal excellent sensitivity to age-related changes during adulthood, excellent test-retest reliability, and adequate to good convergent and discriminant validity. The NIH Toolbox EF measures can be used effectively in epidemiologic and clinical studies.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.)/normas , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Atención/fisiología , Niño , Preescolar , Diagnóstico por Computador , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estados Unidos , Estudios de Validación como Asunto , Adulto Joven
8.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 20(6): 579-87, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24960474

RESUMEN

The objective of this study is to evaluate the construct validity of the NIH Neurobehavioral Toolbox Cognitive Health Battery (NIHTB-CHB) in adults. Confirmatory factor analysis was used to evaluate the dimensional structure underlying the NIHTB-CHB and Gold Standard tests chosen to serve as concurrent validity criteria for the NIHTB-CHB. These results were used to evaluate the convergent and discriminant validity of the NIHTB-CHB in adults ranging from 20 to 85 years of age. Five dimensions were found to explain the correlations among NIHTB-CHB and Gold Standard tests: Vocabulary, Reading, Episodic Memory, Working Memory and Executive Function/Processing Speed. NIHTB-CHB measures and their Gold Standard analogues defined factors in a pattern that broadly supported the convergent and discriminant validity of the NIHTB-CHB tests. This 5-factor structure was found to be invariant across 20-60 year old (N=159) and 65-85 year old (N=109) age groups that were included in the current validity study. Second order Crystallized Abilities (Vocabulary and Reading) and Fluid Abilities (Episodic Memory, Working Memory, Executive/Speed) factors parsimoniously explained correlations among the five first order factors. These results suggest that the NIHTB-CHB will provide both fine-grained and broad characterization of cognition across the adult age span.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.)/normas , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis Discriminante , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estados Unidos , Estudios de Validación como Asunto , Adulto Joven
9.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 20(6): 567-78, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24959840

RESUMEN

This study introduces a special series on validity studies of the Cognition Battery (CB) from the U.S. National Institutes of Health Toolbox for the Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function (NIHTB) (Gershon, Wagster et al., 2013) in an adult sample. This first study in the series describes the sample, each of the seven instruments in the NIHTB-CB briefly, and the general approach to data analysis. Data are provided on test-retest reliability and practice effects, and raw scores (mean, standard deviation, range) are presented for each instrument and the gold standard instruments used to measure construct validity. Accompanying papers provide details on each instrument, including information about instrument development, psychometric properties, age and education effects on performance, and convergent and discriminant construct validity. One study in the series is devoted to a factor analysis of the NIHTB-CB in adults and another describes the psychometric properties of three composite scores derived from the individual measures representing fluid and crystallized abilities and their combination. The NIHTB-CB is designed to provide a brief, comprehensive, common set of measures to allow comparisons among disparate studies and to improve scientific communication.


Asunto(s)
Conducta/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.)/normas , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
10.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(34): 14324-9, 2011 Aug 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21844357

RESUMEN

Maternal separation and poor maternal care in animals have been shown to have important effects on the developing hippocampus and amygdala. In humans, children exposed to abuse/maltreatment or orphanage rearing do not present changes in hippocampal volumes. However, children reared in orphanages present enlarged amygdala volumes, suggesting that the amygdala may be particularly sensitive to severely disturbed (i.e., discontinous, neglectful) care in infancy. Maternal depressive symptomatology has been associated with reductions in overall sensitivity to the infant, and with an increased rate of withdrawn, disengaged behaviors. To determine if poor maternal care associated with maternal depressive symptomatology has a similar pattern of association to the volumes of the hippocampus and amygdala in children, as is the case for severely disturbed infant care (orphanage rearing), we measured hippocampal and amygdala volumes as well as stress hormone (glucocorticoid) levels in children exposed (n = 17) or not (n = 21) to maternal depressive symptomatology since birth. Results revealed no group difference in hippocampal volumes, but larger left and right amygdala volumes and increased levels of glucocorticoids in the children of mothers presenting depressive symptomatology since birth. Moreover, a significant positive correlation was observed between mothers' mean depressive scores and amygdala volumes in their children. The results of this study suggest that amygdala volume in human children may represent an early marker of biological sensitivity to quality of maternal care.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/patología , Trastorno Depresivo/patología , Hipocampo/patología , Conducta Materna , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Tamaño de los Órganos , Saliva/metabolismo
11.
Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol ; 52(6): 919-931, 2024 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38329683

RESUMEN

EF skills play a central role in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety, but it is unclear whether they act as moderators or mediators in the relation between early behavioral inhibition (BI) and later anxiety. The current study tested two models by examining whether two executive functions (EF) skills (cognitive flexibility and working memory) assessed at age 6 acted as moderators or mediators in the relation between BI at 5 years and anxiety symptoms at 7 years. The sample consisted of 422 children from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development. We tested the moderation model, main and interaction effects using hierarchical multiple regression analyses and the mediation model with the product of coefficients test. Results showed that higher BI at 5 years predicted high anxiety at 7 years only at low levels of cognitive flexibility or working memory at 6 years. This suggests that high levels of cognitive flexibility or working memory at 6 years may act as protective factors. In contrast, neither cognitive flexibility nor working memory at age 6 acted as mediators in the association between BI at 5 years and anxiety at 7 years. Results support the hypothesis that goal-driven cognitive control processes act as moderators and promote adaptive functioning by dampening the effect of early BI on later anxiety.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad , Función Ejecutiva , Inhibición Psicológica , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Humanos , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Niño , Masculino , Femenino , Ansiedad/psicología , Estudios Longitudinales , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Preescolar , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Quebec , Conducta Infantil/psicología , Conducta Infantil/fisiología
12.
Child Dev ; 84(3): 955-69, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23106222

RESUMEN

The neurocognitive development of moral and conventional judgments was examined. Event-related potentials were recorded while 24 adolescents (13 years) and 30 young adults (20 years) read scenarios with 1 of 3 endings: moral violations, conventional violations, or neutral acts. Participants judged whether the act was acceptable or unacceptable when a rule was assumed or removed. Across age, reaction times were faster for moral than conventional violations when a rule was assumed. Adolescents had larger N2 amplitudes than adults for moral and neutral, but not conventional, acts. N2 amplitudes were larger when a rule was removed than assumed for moral, but not conventional, violations. These findings suggest that the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying moral and conventional judgments continue to develop beyond early adolescence.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Principios Morales , Conducta Social , Valores Sociales , Adolescente , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
13.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev ; 78(4): 133-46, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23952207

RESUMEN

This monograph describes the creation of the National Institutes of Health Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIH Toolbox CB) and reports validation data for children ages 3-15 years. Individual chapters described measures of executive function, episodic memory, language, working memory, speed of processing, and attention. Separate chapters were devoted to the factor structure of the test battery and composite measures of cognitive health (Total Composite, Fluid Composite, Crystallized Composite). In all cases, the NIH Toolbox CB measures showed sensitivity to age-related changes across the 3- to 15-year range as well as test/retest reliability. The measures also demonstrated adequate to excellent convergent validity, and there was evidence of greater discriminant validity among older than younger children. Confirmatory factor analyses revealed five- and three-factor models for the older (8-15 year olds) and younger (3-6 year olds) children, respectively. The correlation between the Fluid and Crystallized Composite scores was higher among the younger than the older children. The overall pattern is suggestive of greater differentiation of cognitive abilities with age. The strong psychometric properties of the CB and its apparent sensitivity to patterns of developmental change suggest that it is an important advance in the study of cognitive development and has the potential to substantially accelerate discoveries through use of common methods across disparate laboratories and even disciplines.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Cognición/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estados Unidos
14.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev ; 78(4): 16-33, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23952200

RESUMEN

In this chapter, we discuss two measures designed to assess executive function (EF) as part of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB) and report pediatric data from the validation study. EF refers to the goal-directed cognitive control of thought, action, and emotion. Two measures were adapted for standardized computer administration: the Dimensional Change Card Sort (a measure of cognitive flexibility) and a flanker task (a measure of inhibitory control in the context of selective visual attention). Results reveal excellent developmental sensitivity across childhood, excellent reliability, and (in most cases) excellent convergent validity. Correlations between the new NIH Toolbox measures and age were higher for younger children (3-6 years) than for older children (8-15 years), and evidence of increasing differentiation of EF from other aspects of cognition (indexed by receptive vocabulary) was obtained.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adolescente , Niño , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estados Unidos , Estudios de Validación como Asunto
15.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev ; 78(4): 103-18, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23952205

RESUMEN

Confirmatory factor analysis was used the evaluate the dimensional structure underlying the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB) and the measures chosen to serve as concurrent validity criteria for the NIH Toolbox CB. These results were used to evaluate the convergent and discriminant validity of the CB in children ranging from 3 to 15 years of age. Results were evaluated separately for a 3- to 6-year-old group and a 8- to 15-year-old group because different validation measures were used in these age groups. Three distinct dimensions were found for the 3- to 6-year-old group: Vocabulary, Reading, and Fluid Abilities. Five dimensions were found for 8-15 year olds: Vocabulary, Reading, Episodic Memory, Working Memory, and Executive Function/Processing Speed. CB measures and their validation analogues consistently defined common factors in a pattern that broadly supported the convergent and discriminant validity of the CB, but results showed higher intercorrelation and less differentiation of cognitive dimensions in younger than in older children and in older children compared with adults. Age was strongly related to the cognitive dimensions underlying test performance in both groups of children and results are consistent with broader literature showing increasing differentiation of cognitive abilities associated with the rapid brain development that occurs from early childhood into adulthood.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Preescolar , Análisis Factorial , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estados Unidos
16.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev ; 78(4): 119-32, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23952206

RESUMEN

The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB) includes 7 tests covering 6 cognitive abilities. This chapter describes the psychometric characteristics in children ages 3-15 years of a total summary score and composite scores reflecting two major types of cognition: "crystallized" (more dependent upon past learning experiences) and "fluid" (capacity for new learning and information processing in novel situations). Both types of cognition are considered important in everyday functioning, but are thought to be differently affected by brain health status throughout life, from early childhood through older adulthood. All three Toolbox composite scores showed excellent test-retest reliability, robust developmental effects across the childhood age range considered here, and strong correlations with established measures of similar abilities. Additional preliminary evidence of validity includes significant associations between all three Toolbox composite scores and maternal reports of children's health status and school performance.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estados Unidos
17.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev ; 78(4): 1-15, 2013 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23952199

RESUMEN

This monograph presents the pediatric portion of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB) of the NIH Toolbox for the Assessment of Neurological and Behavioral Function. The NIH Toolbox is an initiative of the Neuroscience Blueprint, a collaborative framework through which 16 NIH Institutes, Centers, and Offices jointly support neuroscience-related research, to accelerate discoveries and reduce the burden of nervous system disorders. The CB is one of four modules that measure cognitive, emotional, sensory, and motor health across the lifespan. The CB is unique in its continuity across childhood, adolescence, early adulthood, and old age, and in order to help create a common currency among disparate studies, it is also available at low cost to researchers for use in large-scale longitudinal and epidemiologic studies. This chapter describes the evolution of the CB; methods for selecting cognitive subdomains and instruments; the rationale for test design; and a validation study in children and adolescents, ages 3-15 years. Subsequent chapters feature detailed discussions of each test measure and its psychometric properties (Chapters 2-6), the factor structure of the test battery (Chapter 7), the effects of age and education on composite test scores (Chapter 8), and a final summary and discussion (Chapter 9). As the chapters in this monograph demonstrate, the CB has excellent psychometric properties, and the validation study provided evidence for the increasing differentiation of cognitive abilities with age.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Adolescente , Niño , Preescolar , Emociones , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria Episódica , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Estados Unidos
18.
Cogn Dev ; 28(3): 222-232, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24882942

RESUMEN

Executive function (EF) improves between the ages of 3 and 5 and has been assessed reliably using the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS), a task in which children first sort bivalent cards by one dimension (e.g., shape) and then are instructed to sort by a different dimension (e.g., color). Three-year-olds typically perseverate on the pre-switch dimension, whereas 5-year-olds switch flexibly. Labeling task stimuli can facilitate EF performance (Jacques & Zelazo, 2005; Kirkham, Cruess, & Diamond, 2003), but the nature of this effect is unclear. In 3 experiments we examined 2 hypotheses deriving from different theoretical perspectives: first, that labels facilitate performance in a more bottom-up fashion, by biasing attention to relevant task rules (Kirkham et al., 2003); and second, that labels aid performance in a more top-down fashion by prompting reflection and an understanding of the hierarchical nature of the task (Zelazo, 2004). Children performed better on the DCCS when labels referred to the relevant sorting dimension (Experiment 1). This was a function of the content of the labels rather than the change in auditory signal across phases (Experiment 2). Furthermore, labeling the opposite dimension only did not have a symmetrically negative effect on performance (Experiment 3). Together, these results suggest external, verbal labels bias children to attend to task-relevant information, likely through interaction with emerging top-down, endogenous control.

19.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 48(8): 373-386, 2023 Nov 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38044631

RESUMEN

The National Institutes of Health Toolbox includes two executive function measures: the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) and the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test. Developmental extension (Dext) versions were created with easier levels for younger and more disadvantaged children. Although research on early (E-Prime) and later (iPad) versions of the Dext measures demonstrated their short-term validity, this study investigated their longer-term predictive validity. Participants included 402 children (Mage = 55.02 months) who completed the DCCS-Dext and Flanker-Dext (E-Prime) during early childhood screening and achievement tests in the third grade. Both measures significantly predicted math and reading scores among diverse groups of children.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Función Ejecutiva , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Logro , Matemática , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
20.
Dev Sci ; 15(3): 408-16, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22490180

RESUMEN

To explore the influence of circadian rhythms on executive function during early adolescence, we administered a battery of executive function measures (including a Go-Nogo task, the Iowa Gambling Task, a Self-ordered Pointing task, and an Intra/Extradimensional Shift task) to Morning-preference and Evening-preference participants (N = 80) between the ages of 11 and 14 years who were tested in the morning or afternoon. Significant Chronotype × Time of Day interactions (controlling for amount of sleep the previous night) revealed that adolescents tested at their optimal times of day performed better than those tested at their nonoptimal times. Implications for our understanding of physiological arousal, sleep, and executive function during adolescence are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Sueño/fisiología , Adolescente , Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA