Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 34
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 61(10): 1138-1149, 2020 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32924153

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Findings from primarily cross-sectional studies have linked more extensive social media use to poorer sleep and affective wellbeing among adolescents and young adults. This study examined bedtime social media use, sleep, and affective wellbeing, using an experience sampling methodology with the aim of establishing a day-to-day temporal link between the variables. The study hypothesized a positive association between increased bedtime social media use and lower affective wellbeing the following day, mediated by poorer sleep. METHODS: Using a smartphone application, 101 undergraduate students (Mage  = 19.70 years, SD = 1.09 years), completed daily questionnaires assessing the previous night's bedtime social media use and sleep duration and satisfaction (one measurement per day, questionnaire sent at 08:00), and momentary affective wellbeing (five measurements per day, at randomly varying times between 08:00 and 22:00 on weekdays and 10:00 and 22:00 on weekends), for 14 consecutive days. Objective assessments of total sleep time and sleep efficiency were obtained via wrist-worn actigraphs. By means of separate multilevel models, it was tested whether increased bedtime social media use predicted poorer sleep the same night, whether poorer sleep was predictive of positive and negative affect the following day, and whether sleep mediated the relationship between social media use and affective wellbeing. RESULTS: Increased bedtime social media use was not associated with poorer sleep the same night. Apart from subjective sleep satisfaction, no other sleep variable (i.e., subjective sleep duration, objective total sleep time and objective sleep efficiency) predicted positive or negative affect the following day. CONCLUSIONS: This study found that bedtime social media use is not detrimental to the sleep and affective wellbeing of healthy young adults. However, it is possible that bedtime social media use may be harmful to the sleep of vulnerable individuals.


Asunto(s)
Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Estado de Salud , Trastornos del Inicio y del Mantenimiento del Sueño , Sueño , Medios de Comunicación Sociales/estadística & datos numéricos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
2.
Cogn Emot ; 31(6): 1140-1152, 2017 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27322353

RESUMEN

This study investigates how sadness and minor/moderate depression influences the three functions of attention: alerting, orienting, and executive control using the Attention Network Test. The aim of the study is to investigate whether minor-to-moderate depression is more similar to sadness or clinical depression with regard to attentional processing. It was predicted that both induced sadness and minor-to-moderate depression will influence executive control by narrowing spatial attention and in turn this will lead to less interference from the flanker items (i.e. less effects of congruency) due to a focused attentional state. No differences were predicted for alerting or orienting functions. The results from the two experiments, the first inducing sadness (Experiment 1) and the second measuring subclinical depression (Experiment 2), show that, as expected, participants who are sad or minor to moderately depressed showed less flanker interference compared to participants who were neither sad nor depressed. This study provides strong evidence, that irrespective of its aetiology, sadness and minor/moderate depression have similar effects on spatial attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Depresión , Emociones , Adolescente , Adulto , Nivel de Alerta , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Orientación , Pruebas Psicológicas , Adulto Joven
3.
J Behav Addict ; 13(1): 163-176, 2024 Mar 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38353729

RESUMEN

Background and Aims: Problematic Social Networking Site Use (PSNSU) is not a formally recognised addiction, but it is increasingly discussed as such in academic research and online. Taking a quantitative, exploratory approach, this study aims to (1) determine whether PSNSU is presented like clinically defined addictions by the affected community and (2) address how well measurements of PSNSU fit with the thematic content found within the associated discourse. Methods: Four corpora were created for this study: a corpus concerning PSNSU and three control corpora concerning established addictions, including Alcohol Use Disorder, Tobacco Use Disorder and Gaming Disorder. Keywords were identified, collocates and concordances were explored, and shared themes were compared. Results: Findings show broad thematic similarities between PSNSU and the three control addictions as well as prominent interdiscursive references, which indicate possible confirmation bias among speakers. Conclusions: Scales based upon the components model of addiction are suggested as the most appropriate measure of this emerging disorder.


Asunto(s)
Alcoholismo , Conducta Adictiva , Tabaquismo , Juegos de Video , Humanos , Red Social
4.
Emotion ; 24(2): 451-464, 2024 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37535565

RESUMEN

Previous research has associated sleep with subjective well-being (SWB), but less is known about the underlying within-person processes. In the current study, we investigated how self-reported and actigraphy-measured sleep parameters (sleep onset latency, sleep duration, sleep satisfaction, social jetlag, and sleep efficiency) influence SWB (positive affect [PA], negative affect [NA], and life satisfaction [LS]) at the within- and between-person levels. Multilevel analyses of data from 109 university students who completed a 2-week experience sampling study revealed that higher within-person sleep satisfaction was a significant predictor of all three components of next day's SWB (ps < .005). Higher between-person sleep satisfaction was also related to higher levels of PA and LS (ps < .005), whereas shorter self-reported between-person sleep onset latency was associated with higher PA and LS, and lower NA (ps < .05). However, longer actigraphy-measured within-person sleep onset latency was associated with higher next day's LS (p = .028). When including within- and between-person sleep parameters into the same models predicting SWB, only within- and between-person sleep satisfaction remained a significant predictor of all components of SWB. Additionally, we found an effect of higher self-reported within-person sleep onset latency on PA and of shorter self-reported within-person sleep duration on LS (ps < .05). Our results indicate that the evaluative component of sleep-sleep satisfaction-is most consistently linked with SWB. Thus, sleep interventions that are successful in not only altering sleep patterns but also enhancing sleep satisfaction may stand a better chance at improving students' SWB. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Humanos , Sueño , Autoinforme , Estudiantes
5.
J Vis ; 13(3): 15, 2013 Jul 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23838562

RESUMEN

In visual search, detection of a target is faster when it is presented within a spatial layout of repeatedly encountered nontarget items, indicating that contextual invariances can guide selective attention (contextual cueing; Chun & Jiang, 1998). However, perceptual regularities may interfere with contextual learning; for instance, no contextual facilitation occurs when four nontargets form a square-shaped grouping, even though the square location predicts the target location (Conci & von Mühlenen, 2009). Here, we further investigated potential causes for this interference-effect: We show that contextual cueing can reliably occur for targets located within the region of a segmented object, but not for targets presented outside of the object's boundaries. Four experiments demonstrate an object-based facilitation in contextual cueing, with a modulation of context-based learning by relatively subtle grouping cues including closure, symmetry, and spatial regularity. Moreover, the lack of contextual cueing for targets located outside the segmented region was due to an absence of (latent) learning of contextual layouts, rather than due to an attentional bias towards the grouped region. Taken together, these results indicate that perceptual segmentation provides a basic structure within which contextual scene regularities are acquired. This in turn argues that contextual learning is constrained by object-based selection.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Atención , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Adulto Joven
6.
J Vis ; 13(3)2013 Jul 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23881952

RESUMEN

Observers' capability to extract statistical regularities from the visual world can facilitate attentional orienting. For instance, visual search benefits from the repetition of target locations by means of probability learning. Furthermore, repeated (old) contexts of nontargets contribute to faster visual search in comparison to random (new) arrangements of nontargets. Chun and Jiang (1998) called this effect "contextual cueing" because old contexts provide spatial cues to repeated target locations. In the present study, we investigated how probability learning modulates the adaptation of contextual cueing to a change in target location. After an initial learning phase, targets were relocated within their respective contexts to new positions that were, however, familiar from previous presentations in other spatial contexts. Contextual cueing was observed for relocated targets that originated from old contexts, but it turned into costs when relocated targets had previously been presented in new contexts. Thus, probability learning was not sufficient to observe adaptive contextual cueing for relocated targets. Instead, the contextual past of target locations--whether they had been cued or not--modulated the integration of relocated targets into a learned context. These findings imply that observers extract multiple levels of available statistical information and use them to infer hypotheses about future occurrences of familiar stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Estadística como Asunto , Adulto Joven
7.
J Biol Rhythms ; 36(2): 169-184, 2021 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33353473

RESUMEN

People differ in their sleep timings that are often referred to as a chronotype and can be operationalized as mid-sleep (midpoint between sleep onset and wake-up). The aims of the present studies were to examine intraindividual variability and longer-term temporal stability of mid-sleep on free and workdays, while also considering the effect of age. We used data from a 2-week experience sampling study of British university students (Study 1) and from a panel study of Estonian adults who filled in the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire twice up to 5 years apart (Study 2). Results of Study 1 showed that roughly 50% of the variance in daily mid-sleep scores across the 14-day period was attributed to intraindividual variability as indicated by the intraclass correlation coefficient. However, when the effect of free versus workdays was considered, the intraindividual variability in daily mid-sleep across 2 weeks was 0.71 the size of the interindividual variability. In Study 2, mid-sleep on free and workdays showed good levels of temporal stability-the retest correlations of mid-sleep on free and workdays were 0.66 and 0.58 when measured twice over a period of 0-1 to 5 years. The retest stability of mid-sleep scores on both free and workdays sharply increased from young adulthood and reached their peak when participants were in late 40 to early 50 years of age, indicating that age influences the stability of mid-sleep. Future long-term longitudinal studies are necessary to explore how age-related life circumstances and other possible factors may influence the intraindividual variability and temporal stability of mid-sleep.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Sueño/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
8.
J Dev Behav Pediatr ; 41(6): 461-469, 2020 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32345797

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: To determine whether the attention problems in adults born very preterm/very low birth weight (VP/VLBW; <32 weeks' gestation/<1500 g) or extremely preterm (EP; <26 weeks' gestation) are associated with specific executive or general cognitive deficits. METHOD: Cohorts of VP/VLBW (the Bavarian Longitudinal Study [BLS]) and EP (the EPICure Study) participants were followed from birth to early adulthood, each also following a respective control group. Adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms were assessed via self-report in both cohorts and additionally by parent report in the BLS. Participants in both cohorts also had their attention span rated by trained observers. Performed separately in each cohort, hierarchical regression analyses were used to assess whether the association between preterm birth status and attention problems remained after accounting for executive functioning (inhibitory control and working memory) in adulthood, childhood intelligence score (IQ), or sex. RESULTS: In the discovery cohort of the BLS, significant differences were found between VP/VLBW adults and controls for parent-rated inattention (p < 0.001). However, for self-reported measures of ADHD, no significant differences were found in the BLS or in the EPICure replication cohort. In both cohorts, observer-rated attention spans were lower for VP/VLBW and EP participants in comparison to their respective control groups (p < 0.001). In final models for the BLS, inhibitory control and childhood IQ were significantly associated with parent-rated inattention symptoms (p < 0.006), whereas working memory and childhood IQ were significantly associated with observer-rated attention span (p < 0.001). The effect of childhood IQ on observer-rated attention span was replicated in EPICure. CONCLUSION: VP/VLBW and EP adults are at increased risk of observer-rated attention problems. These problems were predominantly associated with poorer general cognitive ability in early childhood and somewhat with adult executive functioning.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Recién Nacido de Bajo Peso/fisiología , Recien Nacido Prematuro/fisiología , Inteligencia/fisiología , Adulto , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/epidemiología , Niño , Disfunción Cognitiva/epidemiología , Femenino , Alemania/epidemiología , Humanos , Recien Nacido Extremadamente Prematuro/fisiología , Recién Nacido , Inhibición Psicológica , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología
9.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 15(3): 365-71, 2009 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19402922

RESUMEN

There is equivocal evidence whether or not patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) share an attentional bias for concern-related material and if so, whether this reflects hypervigilance towards or problems to disengage from disorder-related material. In a recent study, we failed to detect an attentional bias in OCD patients using an emotional variant of the inhibition of return (IOR) paradigm containing OCD-relevant and neutral words. We reinvestigated the research question with a more stringent design that addressed potential moderators. A new IOR paradigm was set up using visual stimuli. Forty-two OCD patients and 31 healthy controls were presented with neutral (e.g., cup), anxiety-relevant (e.g., shark), checking-relevant (e.g., broken door), and washing-relevant (e.g., dirty toilet) cue pictures at one of two possible locations. Following a short or long interval sensitive to automatic versus controlled processes, a simple target stimulus appeared at either the cued or the uncued location. OCD patients responded significantly slower to targets that were preceded by an OCD-relevant cue. Results lend support to the claim that OCD patients share a processing abnormality for concern-related visual material.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/etiología , Sesgo , Inhibición Psicológica , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/complicaciones , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Emociones/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología
10.
Psychol Res ; 73(2): 244-53, 2009 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19082621

RESUMEN

Previous studies have shown that a change in an existing object is not as effective in capturing attention as the appearance of a new object. This view was recently challenged by Lu and Zhou (Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 12:567-572, 2005), who found strong capture effects for an object changing its color. We suspected that this finding critically depends on a procedural particularity in Lu and Zhou's study, namely that the color of the unique item and the color of the no-unique items randomly switched between trials. In the current study we replicate Lu and Zhou's capture effect (Experiment 1) and show that no capture occurs when the color-to-stimuli assignment is fixed (Experiment 2). Two further experiments suggest that the capture effect in Experiment 1 is not because the unique item switched color (Experiment 3), but because all the no-unique items switched color (Experiment 4). The results are discussed considering top-down modulation and inter-trial priming effects.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Color , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Color , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Percepción Espacial
11.
Prog Brain Res ; 247: 47-69, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31196443

RESUMEN

This chapter provides an overview of the literature on emotion and global/local processing and presents an empirical study exploring how the combination of motion and emotion influences the focus of attention. In two experiments, fear-related pictures either loomed toward the observer or were stationary, and in one of these experiments the emotional content was masked (i.e., scrambled pictures). In the context of fearful pictures, it was expected that the additional element of looming motion would further focus attention based on looming motion's behaviorally urgent properties. However, the combination of a fearful image and looming motion was shown to broaden as opposed to narrow attention. This effect did not occur with simply neutral/looming or fearful/static images. Further, the separation of the emotional content from looming motion (scrambled pictures) revealed no effect on attentional breadth. This suggests that it is the unique combination of the fear-related content and the looming motion, which is broadening attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Miedo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
12.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 80(5): 1072-1082, 2018 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29729000

RESUMEN

Gable and Harmon-Jones (Psychological Science, 21(2), 211-215, 2010) reported that sadness broadens attention in a global-local letter task. This finding provided the key test for their motivational intensity account, which states that the level of spatial processing is not determined by emotional valence, but by motivational intensity. However, their finding is at odds with several other studies, showing no effect, or even a narrowing effect of sadness on attention. This paper reports two attempts to replicate the broadening effect of sadness on attention. Both experiments used a global-local letter task, but differed in terms of emotion induction: Experiment 1 used the same pictures as Gable and Harmon-Jones, taken from the IAPS dataset; Experiment 2 used a sad video underlaid with sad music. Results showed a sadness-specific global advantage in the error rates, but not in the reaction times. The same null results were also found in a South-Asian sample in both experiments, showing that effects on global/local processing were not influenced by a culturally related processing bias.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Motivación , Tristeza , Estimulación Acústica , Humanos , Música/psicología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción
13.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 33(6): 1297-310, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18085944

RESUMEN

These 6 experiments explored the ability of moving random dot patterns to attract attention, as measured by a simple probe-detection task. Each trial began with random motion (i.e., dots linearly moved in random directions). After 1 s motion in 1 hemifield became gradually coherent (i.e., all dots moved up-, down-, left-, or rightwards, or either towards or away from a vanishing point). The results show that only looming motion attracted attention, even when the task became a more demanding discrimination task. This effect is not due to an apparent magnification of stimuli presented in the focus of expansion. When the coherent motion started abruptly, all types of motion attracted attention at a short stimulus onset asynchrony. The looming motion effect only disappeared when attention was drawn to the target location by an arrow. These results suggest that looming motion plays a unique role in guiding spatial attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción de Profundidad , Percepción de Movimiento , Orientación , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ilusiones Ópticas , Psicofísica , Percepción del Tamaño
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 33(2): 271-84, 2007 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17469968

RESUMEN

Eye movements are often misdirected toward a distractor when it appears abruptly, an effect known as oculomotor capture. Fundamental differences between eye movements and attention have led to questions about the relationship of oculomotor capture to the more general effect of sudden onsets on performance, known as attentional capture. This study explores that issue by examining the time course of eye movements and manual localization responses to targets in the presence of sudden-onset distractors. The results demonstrate that for both response types, the proportion of trials on which responses are erroneously directed to sudden onsets reflects the quality of information about the visual display at a given point in time. Oculomotor capture appears to be a specific instance of a more general attentional capture effect. Differences and similarities between the two types of capture can be explained by the critical idea that the quality of information about a visual display changes over time and that different response systems tend to access this information at different moments in time.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción , Reflejo/fisiología
15.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 78(7): 1926-34, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27206553

RESUMEN

Previous studies have shown that a sudden color change is typically less salient in capturing attention than the onset of a new object. Von Mühlenen, Rempel, and Enns (Psychological Science 16: 979-986, 2005) showed that a color change can capture attention as effectively as the onset of a new object given that it occurs during a period of temporal calm, where no other display changes happen. The current study presents a series of experiments that further investigate the conditions under which a change in color captures attention, by disentangling the change signal from the onset of a singleton. The results show that the item changing color receives attentional priority irrespective of whether this change goes along with the appearance of a singleton or not.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción de Color , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
16.
J Anxiety Disord ; 19(1): 117-26, 2005.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15488371

RESUMEN

The present study is aimed at replicating and extending previous results by Nelson et al. [Psychiatry Res. 49 (1993) 183], who found decreased inhibition of return (IOR) in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Thirty OCD patients, 14 psychiatric, and 14 healthy controls participated in a visual cueing experiment. The task required detection of a target stimulus at one of two possible locations. Prior to the target, an uninformative cue appeared at one of these two locations. The Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA) between the cue and the target was systematically varied. We were especially interested in whether severity of OCD symptoms would be negatively correlated with inhibition for previously occupied locations. In accordance with prior research on healthy participants all groups displayed a comparable response pattern: facilitation at the short SOA condition and increasing IOR for the longer SOA conditions. Medication, comorbid depression, and OCD severity did not consistently moderate these effects.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Inhibición Psicológica , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/psicología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Trastorno Obsesivo Compulsivo/diagnóstico , Tiempo de Reacción , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Percepción Visual
17.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 19(2): 206-8, 2004 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15019717

RESUMEN

Reflexive responses are often in direct competition with voluntary control. We test two opposing explanations for how this competition is resolved with respect to eye movements. One states that the quickest activation wins. The other states that the strongest activation wins. We show that an eye movement is executed according to the strongest activation, with the competition being staged at a common subcortical site.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Neurológicos , Reflejo/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Volición/fisiología , Humanos
18.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 30(4): 733-45, 2004 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15301621

RESUMEN

A brief target that is visible when displayed alone can be rendered invisible by a trailing stimulus (metacontrast masking). It has been difficult to determine the temporal dynamics of masking to date because increments in stimulus duration have been invariably confounded with apparent brightness (Bloch's law). In the research reported here, stimulus luminance was adjusted to maintain constant brightness across all durations. Increasing target duration yielded classical U-shaped masking functions, whereas increasing mask duration yielded monotonic decreasing functions. These results are compared with predictions from 6 theoretical models, with the lateral inhibition model providing the best overall fit. It is tentatively suggested that different underlying mechanisms may mediate the U-shaped and monotonic functions obtained with increasing durations of target and mask, respectively.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste , Percepción de Forma , Luz , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Humanos , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
19.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 76(2): 508-18, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24264514

RESUMEN

Sunny and von Mühlenen (Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1050-1056, 2011) showed that an onset of motion captured attention only when the motion was jerky (refreshed at 8 or 17 Hz), but not when it was smooth (33 or 100 Hz). However, it remained unclear why the onset of jerky motion captures attention. In the present study, we systematically tested the role of different aspects of jerky motion in capturing attention. Simple flicker without motion did not capture attention in the same way as jerky motion (Exp. 1). An abrupt displacement between 0.26° and 1.05° captured attention, irrespective of whether the stimulus subsequently continued to move smoothly (Exp. 2) or whether it remained stationary (Exps. 3 and 4). A displaced stimulus that was preceded briefly at the new location by a figure-8 placeholder did not capture attention (Exp. 5). These results are explained within a masking account, according to which abrupt onsets and abrupt displacements receive a processing advantage because they escape forward masking by the preceding figure-8 placeholders.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Humanos , Movimiento (Física) , Tiempo de Reacción
20.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 67(6): 1242-8, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24313265

RESUMEN

Repeated contexts allow us to find relevant information more easily. Learning such contexts has been proposed to depend upon either global processing of the repeated contexts, or alternatively processing of the local region surrounding the target information. In this study, we measured the extent to which observers were by default biased to process towards a more global or local level. The findings showed that the ability to use context to help guide their search was strongly related to an observer's local/global processing bias. Locally biased people could use context to help improve their search better than globally biased people. The results suggest that the extent to which context can be used depends crucially on the observer's attentional bias and thus also to factors and influences that can change this bias.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Sesgo , Señales (Psicología) , Detección de Señal Psicológica/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Formación de Concepto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estadística como Asunto , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA