Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 43
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Biomed Inform ; 141: 104349, 2023 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37015304

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Clinical work involves performing overlapping, time-sensitive tasks that frequently require clinicians to switch their attention between multiple tasks. We developed a methodological approach using EHR-based audit logs to determine switch costs-the cognitive burden associated with task switching-and assessed its magnitude during routine EHR-based clinical tasks. METHOD: Physician trainees (N = 75) participated in a longitudinal study where they provided access to their EHR-based audit logs. Physicians' audit log actions were used to create a taxonomy of EHR tasks. These tasks were transformed into task sequences and the time spent on each task in a sequence was computed. Within these task sequences, instances of task switching (i.e., switching from one task to the next) and non-switching were identified. The primary outcome of interest was the time spent on a post-switch task. Using a mixed-effects regression model, we compared the durations of post-switch and non-switch tasks. RESULTS: 2,781,679 audit log events over 117,822 sessions from 75 physicians were analyzed. Physicians spent most time on chart review (Median (IQR) = 5,439 (2,492-8,336) seconds), note review (1,936 (827-3,321) seconds), and navigating the EHR interface (1,048 (365.5-2,006) seconds) daily. Post task switch activity times were greater for documentation (Median increase = 5 s), order entry (Median increase = 3 s) and results review (Median increase = 3 s). Mixed-effects regression showed that time spent on tasks were longer following a task switch (ß = 0.03; 95% CIlower = 0.027, CIupper = 0.034), with greater post-swtich task times for imaging, order entry, note review, handoff, note entry, chart review and best practice advisory tasks. DISCUSSION: Increased task switching time-an indicator of the cognitive burden associated with switching between tasks-is prevalent in routine EHR-based tasks. We discuss the cumulative impact of incremental switch costs have on overall EHR workload, wellness, and error rates. Relying on theoretical cognitive foundations, we suggest pragmatic design considerations for mitigating the effects of cognitive burden associated with task switching.


Asunto(s)
Médicos , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Carga de Trabajo , Factores de Tiempo , Registros Electrónicos de Salud , Cognición
2.
Cogn Process ; 24(3): 415-424, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37079201

RESUMEN

Previous research on language switching has debated whether high-proficient bilinguals exhibit symmetrical costs and one underlying reason for which may be the potential influence of cross-linguistic characteristics. The previous conflicting findings suggest their impact on language switching needs to be further investigated. In this study, we recruited 36 high-proficient Chinese-English bilinguals and investigated the effect of cross-linguistic similarity on the switching of quantifier expressions under three switch conditions. The results showed that switch costs were significantly greater when the quantifier expression was similar between Chinese and English than when it was different. Larger switch costs were found in the alternate switch condition than those in the non-switch or random switch conditions. In addition, participants exhibited larger switch costs when switching to the first language than when switching to the second language. The results suggest that the similarity of quantifier expressions between the first language and the second language would create more competition and thus induce larger switch costs in phrase-level language switching, which may be derived from the inner word recognition system of the mental lexicon. This study further improves the relevant theories on the origin of switch costs by supporting the Language Non-Specific Selection Hypothesis.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Lingüística , Multilingüismo , Habla , Humanos , Pueblos del Este de Asia , Lenguaje
3.
Psychol Res ; 84(4): 1112-1125, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30361810

RESUMEN

Little is known about how stimulus- and response-based interference might interact to contribute to the costs of switching between cognitive tasks. We analyzed switch costs in a novel cued task-switching/card-matching paradigm in a large study (N = 95). We reasoned that interference from previously active task sets may be contingent upon the retrieval of these task sets via stimulus processing, or alternatively, via response processing. We examined the efficacy of these two factors through eligibility manipulations. That is, stimulus/response features that were capable of retrieving task sets from the previous trial remained eligible (or not) on the current trial. We report three main findings: first, no switch costs were found when neither stimulus features, nor response features, were adequate for the retrieval of the previously executed task sets. Second, we found substantial switch costs when, on switch trials, stimulus features kept the previously executed task eligible, and we found roughly equivalent switch costs when the previously executed response remained eligible. Third, evidence for stimulus-induced switch costs was exclusively observed when previously executed responses remained ineligible. These data indicate that stimulus-based interference, and of importance, response-based interference, contribute comparably to switch costs. Possible interpretations of non-additive switch costs are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
4.
Eur J Neurosci ; 50(7): 3205-3220, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31081574

RESUMEN

An integral aspect of human cognition is the ability to inhibit stimulus-driven, habitual responses, in favour of complex, voluntary actions. In addition, humans can also alternate between different tasks. This comes at the cost of degraded performance when compared to repeating the same task, a phenomenon called the "task-switch cost." While task switching and inhibitory control have been studied extensively, the interaction between them has received relatively little attention. Here, we used the SERIA model, a computational model of antisaccade behaviour, to draw a bridge between them. We investigated task switching in two versions of the mixed antisaccade task, in which participants are cued to saccade either in the same or in the opposite direction to a peripheral stimulus. SERIA revealed that stopping a habitual action leads to increased inhibitory control that persists onto the next trial, independently of the upcoming trial type. Moreover, switching between tasks induces slower and less accurate voluntary responses compared to repeat trials. However, this only occurs when participants lack the time to prepare the correct response. Altogether, SERIA demonstrates that there is a reconfiguration cost associated with switching between voluntary actions. In addition, the enhanced inhibition that follows antisaccade but not prosaccade trials explains asymmetric switch costs. In conclusion, SERIA offers a novel model of task switching that unifies previous theoretical accounts by distinguishing between inhibitory control and voluntary action generation and could help explain similar phenomena in paradigms beyond the antisaccade task.


Asunto(s)
Inhibición Psicológica , Modelos Neurológicos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Tiempo de Reacción
5.
Exp Brain Res ; 237(4): 977-987, 2019 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30694342

RESUMEN

Functional behaviour affords that we form goals to integrate sensory information about the world around us with suitable motor actions, such as when we plan to grab an object with a hand. However, much research has tested grasping in static scenarios where goals are pursued with repetitive movements, whereas dynamic contexts require goals to be pursued even when changes in the environment require a change in the actions to attain them. To study grasp goals in dynamic environments here, we employed a task where the goal remained the same but the execution of the movement changed; we primed participants to grasp objects either with their right or left hand, and occasionally they had to switch to grasping with both. Switch costs should be minimal if grasp goal representations were used continuously, for example, within the left dominant hemisphere. But remapped or re-computed goal representations should delay movements. We found that switching from right-hand grasping to bimanual grasping delayed reaction times but switching from left-hand grasping to bimanual grasping did not. Further, control experiments showed that the lateralized switch costs were not caused by asymmetric inhibition between hemispheres or switches between usual and unusual tasks. Our results show that the left hemisphere does not serve a general role of sensorimotor grasp goal representation. Instead, sensorimotor grasp goals appear to be represented at intermediate levels of abstraction, downstream from cognitive task representations, yet upstream from the control of the grasping effectors.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Objetivos , Mano/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
7.
Brain Lang ; 248: 105367, 2024 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38113600

RESUMEN

Chinese-English bilinguals read paragraphs with language switches using a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm silently while ERPs were measured (Experiment 1) or read them aloud (Experiment 2). Each paragraph was written in either Chinese or English with several function or content words switched to the other language. In Experiment 1, language switches elicited an early, long-lasting positivity when switching from the dominant language to the nondominant language, but when switching to the dominant language, the positivity started later, and was never larger than when switching to the nondominant language. In addition, switch effects on function words were not significantly larger than those on content words in any analyses. In Experiment 2, participants produced more cross-language intrusion errors when switching to the dominant than to the nondominant language, and more errors on function than content words. These results implicate different control mechanisms in bilingual language selection across comprehension and production.


Asunto(s)
Multilingüismo , Humanos , Comprensión , Lectura , Lenguaje
8.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 78(10): 1651-1658, 2023 10 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37330623

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Cognitive flexibility declines with aging and is usually indicated by task switch costs including global and local switch costs. Cognitive flexibility in aging is associated with alterations in functional connectivity. However, whether different task-modulated connectivity mechanisms underlying global and local switch costs remain unclear. METHODS: Here we use the support vector machine to identify age-related functional connectivity in global and local switch costs between older (n = 32) and young adults (n = 33). Participants completed a cued task-switching task during the functional magnetic resonance imaging scan. RESULTS: Results show an age-related decline behaviorally in global but not in local switch costs. Moreover, distinct patterns of age-related alterations of connectivity were observed for each cost. Specifically, only multivariate changes in connectivity patterns were observed for local switch cost, whereas specific age-related connections were revealed for global switch cost. In older adults, the task-modulated left dorsal premotor cortex-left precuneus connectivity decreased, and the left inferior frontal junction-left inferior parietal sulcus connectivity correlated with decreased global switch cost. DISCUSSION: This study provides novel evidence for different neural patterns in global and local switch costs by illuminating connectivity mechanisms underlying cognitive flexibility in aging.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Anciano , Tiempo de Reacción , Envejecimiento/psicología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Cognición , Encéfalo
9.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 18(1)2023 02 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36226894

RESUMEN

The control of emotions is of potentially great clinical relevance. Accordingly, there has been increasing interest in understanding the cognitive mechanisms underlying the ability to switch efficiently between the processing of affective and non-affective information. Reports of asymmetrically increased switch costs when switching toward the more salient emotion task indicate specific demands in the flexible control of emotion. The neural mechanisms underlying affective task switching, however, are so far not fully understood. Using functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) (N = 57), we observed that affective task switching was accompanied by increased activity in domain-general fronto-parietal control systems. Blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) activity in the posterior medial frontal and anterolateral prefrontal cortex was directly related to affective switch costs, indicating that these regions play a particular role in individual differences in (affective) task-switching ability. Asymmetric switch costs were associated with increased activity in the right inferior frontal and dorsal anterior medial prefrontal cortex, two brain regions critical for response inhibition. This suggests that asymmetric switch costs might-to a great extent-reflect higher demands on inhibitory control of the dominant emotion task. These results contribute to a refined understanding of brain systems for the flexible control of emotions and thereby identify valuable target systems for future clinical research.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Corteza Prefrontal , Humanos , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Mapeo Encefálico , Cognición/fisiología
10.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(5): 1398-1408, 2023 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36854926

RESUMEN

Attention can be defined as a mechanism for the selection and prioritization of elements among many. When attention is directed to a specific piece of information, this information is assumed to be in the focus of attention. On a day-to-day basis, we need to rely on efficient switching between information we are holding in working memory (internal modality) and information presented in the world around us (external modality). A recent set of studies investigated between-modality attentional switches and found that there is an asymmetrical switch cost for switching between the internal and external focus of attention (Verschooren et al., 2020, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 46[9], 912-925; Verschooren, Liefooghe, et al., 2019a, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45[10], 1399-1414). In particular, participants switched on a trial-by-trial basis between an internal task using stimuli retrieved from memory and an external task using on-screen presented stimuli. A larger cost was found when switching from the external modality towards the internal modality than the other way around. The authors found that this cost asymmetry could be best explained in terms of associative interference (i.e., differences in shielding efficiency against the memory traces from the competing task set). The present study aimed to replicate the asymmetrical switch cost (Experiment 1) and investigate whether an alternative explanation in terms of stimulus strength can account for the asymmetrical switch cost (Experiment 2). Overall, the results confirm the presence of a subtle, asymmetrical switch cost, but we observed little to no contribution of stimulus strength.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Psicología Experimental , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Percepción , Tiempo de Reacción
11.
Front Psychol ; 13: 923123, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36687953

RESUMEN

The relation between linguistic experience and cognitive function has been of great interest, but recent investigations of this question have produced widely disparate results, ranging from proposals for a "bilingual advantage," to a "bilingual disadvantage," to claims of no difference at all as a function of language. There are many possible sources for this lack of consensus, including the heterogeneity of bilingual populations, and the choice of different tasks and implementations across labs. We propose that another reason for this inconsistency is the task demands of transferring from linguistic experience to laboratory tasks can differ greatly as the task is modified. In this study, we show that task modality (visual, audio, and orthographic) can yield different patterns of performance between monolingual and multilingual participants. The very same task can show similarities or differences in performance, as a function of modality. In turn, this may be explained by the distance of transfer - how close (or far) the laboratory task is to the day to day lived experience of language usage. We suggest that embodiment may provide a useful framework for thinking about task transfer by helping to define the processes of linguistic production and comprehension in ways that are easily connected to task manipulations.

12.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 74(4): 598-609, 2021 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33084523

RESUMEN

We investigated how people balance cognitive constraints (switch costs) against environmental constraints (stimulus availabilities) to optimise their voluntary task switching performance and explored individual differences in their switching behaviour. Specifically, in a self-organised task-switching environment, the stimulus needed for a task repetition was delayed by a stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) that increased with each consecutive repetition until a task switch reset the SOA. As predicted, participants switched tasks when the SOA in task switches approximately matched their individual switch costs and thus they optimised local task performance. Interestingly, self-reports (confirmed by behavioural switching patterns) revealed two individual strategies: some participants (N = 34) indicated to guide their task selection behaviour based on preplanned task sequences over several trials, whereas others (N = 42) indicated to primarily decide on a trial-by-trial basis whether to switch or to repeat tasks. Exploration of switching behaviour based on the two strategy groups revealed additional insights into how people achieved adaptive task selection behaviour in this task environment. Overall, the present findings suggest that individuals select tasks with the aim of improving task performance when dealing with multiple task requirements, but they differ in their preferred individual task selection strategies.


Asunto(s)
Desempeño Psicomotor , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Conducta de Elección , Cognición , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción , Autoinforme
13.
J Cogn ; 4(1): 8, 2021 Jan 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33554029

RESUMEN

Previous studies on voluntary task switching using the self-organized task switching paradigm suggest that task performance and task selection in multitasking are related. When deciding between two tasks, the stimulus associated with a task repetition occurred with a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) that continuously increased with the number of repetitions, while the stimulus associated with a task switch was immediately available. Thus, the waiting time for the repetition stimulus increased with number of consecutive task repetitions. Two main results were shown: first, switch costs and voluntary switch rates correlated negatively - the smaller the switch costs, the larger the switch rates. Second, participants switched tasks when switch costs and waiting time for the repetition stimulus were similar. In the present study, we varied the SOA that increased with number of task repetitions (SOA increment) and also varied the size of the switch costs by varying the intertrial interval. We examined which combination of SOA increment and switch costs maximizes participants' attempts to balance waiting time and switch costs in self-organized task switching. We found that small SOA increments allow for fine-grained adaptation and that participants can best balance their switch costs and waiting times in settings with medium switch costs and small SOA increments. In addition, correlational analyses indicate relations between individual switch costs and individual switch rates across participants.

14.
Front Neurosci ; 15: 730744, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35153653

RESUMEN

This study investigates effects of spatial auditory cues on human listeners' response strategy for identifying two alternately active talkers ("turn-taking" listening scenario). Previous research has demonstrated subjective benefits of audio spatialization with regard to speech intelligibility and talker-identification effort. So far, the deliberate activation of specific perceptual and cognitive processes by listeners to optimize their task performance remained largely unexamined. Spoken sentences selected as stimuli were either clean or degraded due to background noise or bandpass filtering. Stimuli were presented via three horizontally positioned loudspeakers: In a non-spatial mode, both talkers were presented through a central loudspeaker; in a spatial mode, each talker was presented through the central or a talker-specific lateral loudspeaker. Participants identified talkers via speeded keypresses and afterwards provided subjective ratings (speech quality, speech intelligibility, voice similarity, talker-identification effort). In the spatial mode, presentations at lateral loudspeaker locations entailed quicker behavioral responses, which were significantly slower in comparison to a talker-localization task. Under clean speech, response times globally increased in the spatial vs. non-spatial mode (across all locations); these "response time switch costs," presumably being caused by repeated switching of spatial auditory attention between different locations, diminished under degraded speech. No significant effects of spatialization on subjective ratings were found. The results suggested that when listeners could utilize task-relevant auditory cues about talker location, they continued to rely on voice recognition instead of localization of talker sound sources as primary response strategy. Besides, the presence of speech degradations may have led to increased cognitive control, which in turn compensated for incurring response time switch costs.

15.
Exp Gerontol ; 135: 110934, 2020 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32224222

RESUMEN

Task switching performance was assessed in a group of healthy young, healthy old, and MCI-diagnosed participants. Highly significant RT-related local switch costs were found in the MCI group. This contrasts the typical finding that in normal aging local switch costs show no age-related deficit. Local switch costs deficits may be a diagnostic tool in differentiating normal and pathological cognitive aging.


Asunto(s)
Disfunción Cognitiva , Envejecimiento , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Humanos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Tiempo de Reacción
16.
Brain Lang ; 204: 104754, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32113072

RESUMEN

When switching languages, bilinguals recruit a language control network that overlaps with brain regions known to support general cognitive control, but it is unclear whether these same regions are recruited in passive comprehension of language switches. Using fMRI with a blocked design, 24 Spanish-English bilinguals silently read 36 paragraphs in which the default language was Spanish or English, and that had either (1) no switches, (2) function word switches or (3) content word switches. Relative to no switches, function switches activated the right IFG, bilateral MFG, and left IPL/SMG. In contrast, switching on content words produced limited neural switching costs observed only in the left IFG. Switching into the dominant language was more costly in the right SMG than switching into the nondominant language, and neural switching costs were correlated with switching costs in the dominant language in cued picture-naming. Seemingly passive reading comprehension involves brain regions known to support cognitive control in active switching during production, possibly reflecting the operation of a modality-general switch mechanism.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Comprensión , Multilingüismo , Lectura , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
17.
Front Psychol ; 11: 791, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32425858

RESUMEN

Language switching involves multiple processing stages. Previous studies have not dissociated the cognitive process underlying language form switches and concept switches. Here, we examined the two factors using a novel language-switching paradigm. Chinese-English bilinguals named individually presented pictures in either Chinese or English according to a language cue. Pictures in two consecutive trials represented either identical, semantically related, or unrelated concepts. Results showed both language (form) switch costs and concept switch costs. The interaction between these two factors suggested that the effects were additive, with the longest naming response times observed when two pictures were semantically unrelated and involved a switch between languages. These findings suggest that the functional loci of the language control mechanism occur at multiple processing stages. Implications of the findings are discussed within current models of language processing in bilinguals.

18.
J Cogn ; 3(1): 22, 2020 Sep 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32964181

RESUMEN

The Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model is a neural network for simulating human performance in speeded response time tasks. It learns with an exemplar-based memory store and it is capable of modelling findings from various subdomains of cognition. In this paper, we show how the PEP model can be designed to follow instructions (e.g., task rules and goals). The extended PEP model is then used to simulate a number of key findings from the task switching domain. These include the switch cost, task-rule congruency effects, response repetition asymmetries, cue repetition benefits, and the full pattern of means from a recent feature integration decomposition of cued task switching (Schmidt & Liefooghe, 2016). We demonstrate that the PEP model fits the participant data well, that the model does not possess the flexibility to match any pattern of results, and that a number of competing task switching models fail to account for key observations that the PEP model produces naturally. Given the parsimony and unique explanatory power of the episodic account presented here, our results suggest that feature-integration biases have a far greater power in explaining task-switching performance than previously assumed.

19.
J Cogn ; 3(1): 28, 2020 Sep 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32964186

RESUMEN

In our recent article (Schmidt, Liefooghe, & De Houwer, 2020, this volume), we presented an adaptation of the Parallel Episodic Processing (PEP) model for simulating instruction following and task-switching behaviour. In this paper, we respond to five commentaries on our article: Monsell & McLaren (2020), Koch & Lavric (2020), Meiran (2020), Longman (2020), and Pfeuffer (2020). The commentaries discuss potential future modelling goals, deeper reflections on cognitive control, and some potential challenges for our theoretical perspective and associated model. We focus primarily on the latter. In particular, we clarify that we (a) acknowledge the role of cognitive control in task switching, and (b) are arguing that certain task-switching effects do not serve as a good measure of said cognitive control. We also discuss some ambiguities in terminological uses (e.g., the meaning of "task-set reconfiguration"), along with some future experimental and modelling research directions.

20.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(8): 2056-2067, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30672384

RESUMEN

In a two-component switching paradigm, in which participants switched between two auditory attention selection criteria (attention component: left vs. right ear) and two judgements (judgement component: number vs. letter judgement), we found high judgement switch costs in attention criterion repetitions, but low costs in attention criterion switches. This finding showed an interaction of components. Previous two-component switching studies observed differently emphasised interaction patterns. In the present study, we explored whether the strength of the interaction pattern reflects the strength of the binding of target location and judgement. Specifically, we investigated whether exogenous target location cueing led to weaker binding than endogenous cueing, and whether preparation for ear selection could influence the binding. Attention switches with auditory exogenous target location cues did not affect the component interaction pattern, whereas a prolonged preparation interval led to a more emphasised pattern. Binding between target location and judgement may therefore be rather automatic and may not necessarily require concurrent component processing. Sufficient time for target location switches with long preparation time may activate the previous trial's episode or facilitate switches of the subsequent judgement.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA