Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 7.566
Filtrar
Más filtros

Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Annu Rev Neurosci ; 43: 297-314, 2020 07 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32097575

RESUMEN

An enduring problem in neuroscience is determining whether cases of amnesia result from eradication of the memory trace (storage impairment) or if the trace is present but inaccessible (retrieval impairment). The most direct approach to resolving this question is to quantify changes in the brain mechanisms of long-term memory (BM-LTM). This approach argues that if the amnesia is due to a retrieval failure, BM-LTM should remain at levels comparable to trained, unimpaired animals. Conversely, if memories are erased, BM-LTM should be reduced to resemble untrained levels. Here we review the use of BM-LTM in a number of studies that induced amnesia by targeting memory maintenance or reconsolidation. The literature strongly suggests that such amnesia is due to storage rather than retrieval impairments. We also describe the shortcomings of the purely behavioral protocol that purports to show recovery from amnesia as a method of understanding the nature of amnesia.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Memoria a Largo Plazo/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Mantenimiento , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología
2.
J Neurosci ; 44(14)2024 Apr 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38351000

RESUMEN

Research on the role of the hippocampus in memory acquisition has generally focused on active learning. But to understand memory, it is at least as important to understand processes that happen offline, during both wake and sleep. In a study of patients with amnesia, we previously demonstrated that although a functional hippocampus is not necessary for the acquisition of procedural motor memory during training session, it is required for its offline consolidation during sleep. Here, we investigated whether an intact hippocampus is also required for the offline consolidation of procedural motor memory while awake. Patients with amnesia due to hippocampal damage (n = 4, all male) and demographically matched controls (n = 10, 8 males) trained on the finger tapping motor sequence task. Learning was measured as gains in typing speed and was divided into online (during task execution) and offline (during interleaved 30 s breaks) components. Amnesic patients and controls showed comparable total learning, but differed in the pattern of performance improvement. Unlike younger adults, who gain speed across breaks, both groups gained speed only while typing. Only controls retained these gains over the breaks; amnesic patients slowed down and compensated for these losses during subsequent typing. In summary, unlike their peers, whose motor performance remained stable across brief breaks in typing, amnesic patients showed evidence of impaired access to motor procedural memory. We conclude that in addition to being necessary for the offline consolidation of motor memories during sleep, the hippocampus maintains access to motor memory across brief offline periods during wake.


Asunto(s)
Consolidación de la Memoria , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Memoria , Sueño , Amnesia , Hipocampo
3.
J Neurosci ; 44(8)2024 Feb 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38228367

RESUMEN

Subconcussive head impacts are associated with the development of acute and chronic cognitive deficits. We recently reported that high-frequency head impact (HFHI) causes chronic cognitive deficits in mice through synaptic changes. To better understand the mechanisms underlying HFHI-induced memory decline, we used TRAP2/Ai32 transgenic mice to enable visualization and manipulation of memory engrams. We labeled the fear memory engram in male and female mice exposed to an aversive experience and subjected them to sham or HFHI. Upon subsequent exposure to natural memory recall cues, sham, but not HFHI, mice successfully retrieved fearful memories. In sham mice the hippocampal engram neurons exhibited synaptic plasticity, evident in amplified AMPA:NMDA ratio, enhanced AMPA-weighted tau, and increased dendritic spine volume compared with nonengram neurons. In contrast, although HFHI mice retained a comparable number of hippocampal engram neurons, these neurons did not undergo synaptic plasticity. This lack of plasticity coincided with impaired activation of the engram network, leading to retrograde amnesia in HFHI mice. We validated that the memory deficits induced by HFHI stem from synaptic plasticity impairments by artificially activating the engram using optogenetics and found that stimulated memory recall was identical in both sham and HFHI mice. Our work shows that chronic cognitive impairment after HFHI is a result of deficiencies in synaptic plasticity instead of a loss in neuronal infrastructure, and we can reinstate a forgotten memory in the amnestic brain by stimulating the memory engram. Targeting synaptic plasticity may have therapeutic potential for treating memory impairments caused by repeated head impacts.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia , Memoria , Masculino , Ratones , Femenino , Animales , Ácido alfa-Amino-3-hidroxi-5-metil-4-isoxazol Propiónico , Memoria/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Ratones Transgénicos
4.
Learn Mem ; 31(1-2)2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38286522

RESUMEN

We investigated whether retrograde amnesia for the stress-induced impairment of extinction retrieval shares similar characteristics with original acquisition memories. The first experiment demonstrated that cycloheximide administered shortly after a single restraint stress session alleviated the impairment of extinction retrieval but not when administered following a longer delay (i.e., the amnesia for stress is time-dependent). A second experiment showed that the retrograde amnesia for stress could be alleviated by a second brief exposure to the stressor. These results demonstrating that amnesia for stress shares characteristics similar to original memories are explained using a retrieval-based memory integration model of retrograde amnesia.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Retrógrada , Trastornos de la Memoria , Humanos , Amnesia Retrógrada/inducido químicamente , Amnesia , Cicloheximida/farmacología
5.
J Neurosci ; 43(31): 5710-5722, 2023 08 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37463727

RESUMEN

Temporal discounting (TD) represents the mental devaluation of rewards that are available after a delay. Whether the hippocampus is critical for TD remains unclear, with marked discrepancies between animal and human studies: although animals with discrete hippocampal lesions display impaired TD, human participants with similar lesions show intact performance on classic intertemporal choice tasks. A candidate explanation for this discrepancy is that delays and rewards are experienced in the moment in animal studies but tend to be hypothetical in human studies. We tested this hypothesis by examining the performance of amnesic participants with hippocampal lesions (one female, six males) on a novel experiential intertemporal choice task that used interesting photographs occluded by thick lines as rewards (Patt et al., 2021). Using a logistic function to model indifference points data, we compared performance to that on a classic intertemporal choice task with hypothetical outcomes. Participants with hippocampal lesions displayed impaired patterns of choices in the experiential task but not in the hypothetical task. Specifically, hippocampal lesions were associated with decreased amplitude of the delay-reward trade-off, with persistent choice of the delayed option despite delay increase. These results help explain previous discrepancies across animal and human studies, indicating that the hippocampus plays a critical role in temporal discounting when the outcomes of decisions are experienced in the moment, but not necessarily when they are hypothetical.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Impaired temporal discounting (TD) has been related to maladaptive behaviors, including substance dependence and nonadherence to medical treatment. There is consensus that TD recruits the brain valuation network but whether the hippocampal memory system is additionally recruited remains unclear. This study examined TD in hippocampal amnesia, providing a unique opportunity to explore the role of the hippocampus in cognition. Whereas most human studies have used hypothetical outcomes, this study used a novel experiential task with real-time delays and rewards. Results demonstrated hippocampal involvement in the experiential task, but not in a classic hypothetical task administered for comparison. These findings elucidate previous discrepancies between animal and human TD studies. This reconciliation is critical as animals serve as models of human neurocognition.


Asunto(s)
Descuento por Demora , Trastornos Relacionados con Sustancias , Masculino , Humanos , Femenino , Recompensa , Hipocampo , Amnesia , Conducta de Elección
6.
J Neurosci ; 43(36): 6268-6279, 2023 09 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37586871

RESUMEN

A well orchestrated coupling hierarchy of slow waves and spindles during slow-wave sleep supports memory consolidation. In old age, the duration of slow-wave sleep and the number of coupling events decrease. The coupling hierarchy deteriorates, predicting memory loss and brain atrophy. Here, we investigate the dynamics of this physiological change in slow wave-spindle coupling in a frontocentral electroencephalography position in a large sample (N = 340; 237 females, 103 males) spanning most of the human life span (age range, 15-83 years). We find that, instead of changing abruptly, spindles gradually shift from being driven by slow waves to driving slow waves with age, reversing the coupling hierarchy typically seen in younger brains. Reversal was stronger the lower the slow-wave frequency, and starts around midlife (age range, ∼40-48 years), with an established reversed hierarchy between 56 and 83 years of age. Notably, coupling strength remains unaffected by age. In older adults, deteriorating slow wave-spindle coupling, measured using the phase slope index (PSI) and the number of coupling events, is associated with blood plasma glial fibrillary acidic protein levels, a marker for astrocyte activation. Data-driven models suggest that decreased sleep time and higher age lead to fewer coupling events, paralleled by increased astrocyte activation. Counterintuitively, astrocyte activation is associated with a backshift of the coupling hierarchy (PSI) toward a "younger" status along with increased coupling occurrence and strength, potentially suggesting compensatory processes. As the changes in coupling hierarchy occur gradually starting at midlife, we suggest there exists a sizable window of opportunity for early interventions to counteract undesirable trajectories associated with neurodegeneration.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Evidence accumulates that sleep disturbances and cognitive decline are bidirectionally and causally linked, forming a vicious cycle. Improving sleep quality could break this cycle. One marker for sleep quality is a clear hierarchical structure of sleep oscillations. Previous studies showed that sleep oscillations decouple in old age. Here, we show that, rather, the hierarchical structure gradually shifts across the human life span and reverses in old age, while coupling strength remains unchanged. This shift is associated with markers for astrocyte activation in old age. The shifting hierarchy resembles brain maturation, plateau, and wear processes. This study furthers our comprehension of this important neurophysiological process and its dynamic evolution across the human life span.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Sueño de Onda Lenta , Femenino , Masculino , Humanos , Anciano , Adolescente , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Sueño , Longevidad , Amnesia
7.
Hippocampus ; 34(8): 438-451, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39016331

RESUMEN

Studies of the impact of brain injury on memory processes often focus on the quantity and episodic richness of those recollections. Here, we argue that the organization of one's recollections offers critical insights into the impact of brain injury on functional memory. It is well-established in studies of word list memory that free recall of unrelated words exhibits a clear temporal organization. This temporal contiguity effect refers to the fact that the order in which word lists are recalled reflects the original presentation order. Little is known, however, about the organization of recall for semantically rich materials, nor how recall organization is impacted by hippocampal damage and memory impairment. The present research is the first study, to our knowledge, of temporal organization in semantically rich narratives in three groups: (1) Adults with bilateral hippocampal damage and severe declarative memory impairment, (2) adults with bilateral ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) damage and no memory impairment, and (3) demographically matched non-brain-injured comparison participants. We find that although the narrative recall of adults with bilateral hippocampal damage reflected the temporal order in which those narratives were experienced above chance levels, their temporal contiguity effect was significantly attenuated relative to comparison groups. In contrast, individuals with vmPFC damage did not differ from non-brain-injured comparison participants in temporal contiguity. This pattern of group differences yields insights into the cognitive and neural systems that support the use of temporal organization in recall. These data provide evidence that the retrieval of temporal context in narrative recall is hippocampal-dependent, whereas damage to the vmPFC does not impair the temporal organization of narrative recall. This evidence of limited but demonstrable organization of memory in participants with hippocampal damage and amnesia speaks to the power of narrative structures in supporting meaningfully organized recall despite memory impairment.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia , Hipocampo , Recuerdo Mental , Humanos , Hipocampo/patología , Hipocampo/diagnóstico por imagen , Hipocampo/fisiopatología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Amnesia/fisiopatología , Amnesia/patología , Amnesia/psicología , Adulto , Narración , Anciano , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Factores de Tiempo , Corteza Prefrontal/patología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiopatología , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/lesiones
8.
Mol Psychiatry ; 28(8): 3332-3342, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37369719

RESUMEN

Alzheimer's Disease (AD) is a prevalent neurodegenerative disease characterized by tau hyperphosphorylation, Aß1-42 aggregation and cognitive dysfunction. Therapeutic agents directed at mitigating tau aggregation and clearing Aß1-42, and delivery of growth factor genes (BDNF, FGF2), have ameliorated cognitive deficits, but these approaches did not prevent or stop AD progression. Here we report that viral-(AAV) delivery of Neurotrophic Factor-α1/Carboxypeptidase E (NF-α1/CPE) gene in hippocampus at an early age prevented later development of cognitive deficits as assessed by Morris water maze and novel object recognition assays, neurodegeneration, and tau hyperphosphorylation in male 3xTg-AD mice. Additionally, amyloid precursor protein (APP) expression was reduced to near non-AD levels, and insoluble Aß1-42 was reduced significantly. Pro-survival proteins: mitochondrial Bcl2 and Serpina3g were increased; and mitophagy inhibitor Plin4 and pro-inflammatory protein Card14 were decreased in AAV-NF-α1/CPE treated versus untreated AD mice. Thus NF-α1/CPE gene therapy targets many regulatory components to prevent cognitive deficits in 3xTg-AD mice and has implications as a new therapy to prevent AD progression by promoting cell survival, inhibiting APP overexpression and tau hyperphosphorylation.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Amiloidosis , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas , Ratones , Masculino , Animales , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/metabolismo , Carboxipeptidasa H/genética , Carboxipeptidasa H/metabolismo , Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas/metabolismo , Péptidos beta-Amiloides/metabolismo , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Trastornos de la Memoria/genética , Trastornos de la Memoria/prevención & control , Trastornos de la Memoria/metabolismo , Precursor de Proteína beta-Amiloide/genética , Precursor de Proteína beta-Amiloide/metabolismo , Factores de Crecimiento Nervioso/metabolismo , Amiloidosis/genética , Amiloidosis/metabolismo , Amnesia/metabolismo , Ratones Transgénicos , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Proteínas tau/genética , Proteínas tau/metabolismo
9.
Neurochem Res ; 49(2): 363-378, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37814133

RESUMEN

Cannabidiol (CBD) is a promising neurological agent with potential beneficial effects on memory and cognitive function. The combination of CBD and topiramate in the treatment of some neurological diseases has been of great interest. Since Topiramate-induced memory loss is a major drawback of its clinical application and the overall effect of the combination of CBD and topiramate on memory is still unclear, here we investigated the effect of CBD on topiramate-induced memory loss and the underlying molecular mechanisms. A one trial step-through inhibitory test was used to evaluate memory consolidation in rats. Moreover, the role of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) in the combination of CBD and topiramate in memory consolidation was evaluated through the intra-CA1 administration of MK-801 and NMDA. Western blot analysis was used to evaluate variations in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and phosphorylated cyclic AMP response element-binding protein (pCREB)/CREB ratio in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus (HPC). While the intraperitoneal (i.p.) administration of topiramate (50, 75, and 100 mg/kg) significantly reduced inhibitory time latency, the i.p. administration of CBD (20 and 40 mg/kg) could effectively reverse these effects. Similarly, the sub-effective doses of NMDA plus CBD (10 mg/kg) could improve the topiramate-induced memory loss along with an enhancement in BDNF and pCREB expression in the PFC and HPC. Contrarily, the administration of sub-effective doses of the NMDAR antagonist (MK-801) diminished the protective effects of CBD (20 mg/kg) on topiramate-induced memory loss associated with decreased BDNF and pCREB levels in the PFC and HPC. These findings suggest that CBD can improve topiramate-induced memory impairment, partially by the NMDARs of the PFC and HPC, possibly regulated by the CREB/BDNF signaling pathway.


Asunto(s)
Factor Neurotrófico Derivado del Encéfalo , Cannabidiol , Ratas , Animales , Topiramato/uso terapéutico , Topiramato/farmacología , Factor Neurotrófico Derivado del Encéfalo/metabolismo , Receptores de N-Metil-D-Aspartato/metabolismo , Cannabidiol/farmacología , Cannabidiol/uso terapéutico , Maleato de Dizocilpina/farmacología , Maleato de Dizocilpina/uso terapéutico , Maleato de Dizocilpina/metabolismo , N-Metilaspartato/metabolismo , Hipocampo/metabolismo , Transducción de Señal , Corteza Prefrontal/metabolismo , Trastornos de la Memoria/inducido químicamente , Trastornos de la Memoria/tratamiento farmacológico , Trastornos de la Memoria/metabolismo , Amnesia/metabolismo , Proteína de Unión a Elemento de Respuesta al AMP Cíclico/metabolismo
10.
Eur J Neurol ; 31(2): e16134, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37955550

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Opioid-associated amnestic syndrome (OAS) and transient global amnesia (TGA) are conditions with clinical overlap. We therefore sought to determine whether opioid use might be associated with TGA. METHODS: Data from the Massachusetts Department of Public Health Syndromic Surveillance program were queried to ascertain the frequency of opioid use among emergency department (ED) encounters for TGA compared to that for all other ED visits between January 2019 and June 2023. RESULTS: A total of 13,188,630 ED visits were identified during the study period. Of 1417 visits for TGA, one visit met the exposure definition for opioid use. There were 13,187,213 visits for other indications, 57,638 of which were considered opioid-exposed. The odds ratio for the relationship between opioid use and TGA was 0.16 (95% confidence interval 0.02, 1.14). CONCLUSION: Despite the clinical overlap between OAS and TGA, surveillance data from ED visits in Massachusetts do not suggest that opioid use is a risk factor for TGA, indicating that OAS and TGA are distinct entities.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia Global Transitoria , Humanos , Amnesia Global Transitoria/inducido químicamente , Amnesia Global Transitoria/epidemiología , Analgésicos Opioides/efectos adversos , Factores de Riesgo , Servicio de Urgencia en Hospital , Amnesia
11.
BMC Infect Dis ; 24(1): 36, 2024 Jan 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38166694

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The purpose of this study is to analyze the influencing factors associated with Long-COVID in patients infected with Omicron variant of COVID-19 in Changchun City, Jilin Province, China three months after discharge in March 2022. METHODS: In this study, we conducted a telephone follow-up based on the real-world data collected from the Affiliated Hospital to Changchun University of Chinese Medicine, Changchun Tongyuan Shelter Hospital and Changchun Infectious Disease Hospital during the COVID-19 epidemic in Changchun in March 2022. We used the Global COVID-19 Clinical Platform Case Report Form for Post COVID condition as a follow-up questionnaire to collect the general information, past medical history, clinical symptoms, COVID-19 vaccine inoculation doses, and other relevant information to analyze the symptom characteristics of COVID-19 patients three months after discharge from the hospital and related factors affecting Long COVID. RESULTS: A total of 1,806 patients with COVID-19 were included in this study, 977 males and 829 females, with a mean age of 38.5 [30.0, 49.4] years, and the number of female patients suffering from Long COVID (50.87%) was greater than male patients (p = 0.023). The binary logistic regression analysis of factors influencing Long COVID showed that smoking history (OR (95%CI) = 0.551(0.425-0.714), p < 0.001, taking never smoking as a reference), allergy history (OR (95%CI) = 1.618 (1.086-2.413), p-value 0.018, taking no allergy as a reference), first symptoms (OR (95%CI) = 0.636 (0.501-0.807), p < 0.001, with no first symptoms as reference) and COVID-19 vaccine inoculation doses (OR (95%CI) = 1.517 (1.190-1.933), p-value 0.001, with ≤ 2 doses of COVID-19 vaccine inoculation doses as reference) constituted its influencing factors. The first symptoms of patients on admission mainly included fever (512 cases, 71.81%), cough (279 cases, 39.13%) and dry or itchy throat (211 cases, 29.59%). The most common symptoms of Long COVID were persistent fatigue (68 cases), amnesia (61 cases), insomnia (50 cases) and excessive sweating (50 cases). CONCLUSION: The first symptoms on admission were predominantly fever, cough and dry or itchy throat. The most common symptoms of Long COVID were persistent fatigue, amnesia, insomnia and excessive sweating, and female patients were at a higher risk of Long COVID.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19 , Trastornos del Inicio y del Mantenimiento del Sueño , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Amnesia , Tos , COVID-19/epidemiología , Vacunas contra la COVID-19 , Estudios Transversales , Fatiga , Fiebre/epidemiología , Alta del Paciente , Síndrome Post Agudo de COVID-19 , SARS-CoV-2 , Persona de Mediana Edad
12.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci ; 36(1): 45-52, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37415502

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Spontaneous confabulation is a symptom in which false memories are conveyed by the patient as true. The purpose of the study was to identify the neuroanatomical substrate of this complex symptom and evaluate the relationship to related symptoms, such as delusions and amnesia. METHODS: Twenty-five lesion locations associated with spontaneous confabulation were identified in a systematic literature search. The network of brain regions functionally connected to each lesion location was identified with a large connectome database (N=1,000) and compared with networks derived from lesions associated with nonspecific (i.e., variable) symptoms (N=135), delusions (N=32), or amnesia (N=53). RESULTS: Lesions associated with spontaneous confabulation occurred in multiple brain locations, but they were all part of a single functionally connected brain network. Specifically, 100% of lesions were connected to the mammillary bodies (familywise error rate [FWE]-corrected p<0.05). This connectivity was specific for lesions associated with confabulation compared with lesions associated with nonspecific symptoms or delusions (FWE-corrected p<0.05). Lesions associated with confabulation were more connected to the orbitofrontal cortex than those associated with amnesia (FWE-corrected p<0.05). CONCLUSIONS: Spontaneous confabulation maps to a common functionally connected brain network that partially overlaps, but is distinct from, networks associated with delusions or amnesia. These findings lend new insight into the neuroanatomical bases of spontaneous confabulation.


Asunto(s)
Conectoma , Trastornos de la Memoria , Humanos , Amnesia/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/patología , Corteza Prefrontal/patología , Conjuntos de Datos como Asunto
13.
Epilepsy Behav ; 150: 109570, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38070412

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Epidemiological studies have reported an association between epilepsy and dementia. However, the causal relationship between epilepsy and the risk of dementia is not clear. We aimed to inspect the causal effect of epilepsy on memory loss and dementia. METHODS: We analyzed summary data of epilepsy, memory loss, and dementia from the genome-wide association study (GWAS) using the two-sample Mendelian randomization (MR) method. We used the estimated odds ratio of memory loss and dementia associated with each of the genetically defined traits to infer evidence for a causal relationship with the following exposures: all epilepsy, focal epilepsy (including focal epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis, lesion-negative focal epilepsy, and focal epilepsy with other lesions), and genetic generalized epilepsy (including childhood absence epilepsy, generalized tonic-clonic seizures alone, Juvenile absence epilepsy, and Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy). RESULTS: According to the result of MR using the inverse variance weighted method (IVW), we found that genetically predicted epilepsy did not causally increase the risk of memory loss and dementia (p > 0.05). Results of the MR-Egger and weighted median method were consistent with the IVW method. CONCLUSIONS: No evidence has been found to support the notion that epilepsy can result in memory loss and dementia. The associations observed in epidemiological studies could be attributed, in part, to confounding or nongenetic determinants.


Asunto(s)
Demencia , Epilepsias Parciales , Epilepsia Tipo Ausencia , Humanos , Niño , Análisis de la Aleatorización Mendeliana , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo , Epilepsia Tipo Ausencia/complicaciones , Epilepsia Tipo Ausencia/epidemiología , Epilepsia Tipo Ausencia/genética , Amnesia , Demencia/complicaciones , Demencia/epidemiología , Demencia/genética
14.
Metab Brain Dis ; 39(4): 589-609, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38351421

RESUMEN

This study aimed to investigate the action of two different formulations of curcumin (Cur)-loaded nanocapsules (Nc) (Eudragit [EUD] and poly (ɛ-caprolactone) [PCL]) in an amnesia mice model. We also investigated the formulations' effects on scopolamine-induced (SCO) depressive- and anxiety-like comorbidities, the cholinergic system, oxidative parameters, and inflammatory markers. Male Swiss mice were randomly divided into five groups (n = 8): group I (control), group II (Cur PCL Nc 10 mg/kg), group III (Cur EUD Nc 10 mg/kg), group IV (free Cur 10 mg/kg), and group V (SCO). Treatments with Nc or Cur (free) were performed daily or on alternate days. After 30 min of treatment, the animals received the SCO and were subjected to behavioral tests 30 min later (Barnes maze, open-field, object recognition, elevated plus maze, tail suspension tests, and step-down inhibitory avoidance tasks). The animals were then euthanized and tissue was removed for biochemical assays. Our results demonstrated that Cur treatment (Nc or free) protected against SCO-induced amnesia and depressive-like behavior. The ex vivo assays revealed lower acetylcholinesterase (AChE) and catalase (CAT) activity, reduced thiobarbituric species (TBARS), reactive species (RS), and non-protein thiols (NSPH) levels, and reduced interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumor necrosis factor (TNF) expression. The treatments did not change hepatic markers in the plasma of mice. After treatments on alternate days, Cur Nc had a more significant effect than the free Cur protocol, implying that Cur may have prolonged action in Nc. This finding supports the concept that it is possible to achieve beneficial effects in nanoformulations, and treatment on alternate days differs from the free Cur protocol regarding anti-amnesic effects in mice.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia , Curcumina , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Nanocápsulas , Animales , Curcumina/farmacología , Curcumina/administración & dosificación , Curcumina/uso terapéutico , Ratones , Masculino , Amnesia/tratamiento farmacológico , Amnesia/inducido químicamente , Estrés Oxidativo/efectos de los fármacos , Escopolamina
15.
Memory ; 32(8): 1083-1099, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39146469

RESUMEN

This article examines continuing misunderstanding about memory function especially for trauma, across three UK samples (N = 717). Delayed allegations of child sexual and physical abuse are prevalent in Western legal systems and often rely upon uncorroborated memory testimony to prove guilt. U.K. legal professionals and jurors typically assess the reliability of such memory recall via common sense, yet decades of scientific research show common sense beliefs often conflict with science. Recent international surveys show controversial notions of repression and accurate memory recovery remain strongly endorsed. In historical cases, these notions may lead to wrongful convictions. The current study surveyed the U.K. public, lawyers, and mental health professionals' beliefs about repression, dissociative amnesia and false memories. Study findings give unique data on judges' and barristers' beliefs. Overall, the study findings reinforce international scientists' concerns of a science - knowledge-gap. Repression was strongly endorsed by lay, legal and clinical participants (> 78%) as was dissociative amnesia (> 87%). Moreover, suboptimal professional legal education and juror guidance may increase misunderstanding. Correcting beliefs about memory function, and extending the contribution of memory science in the courtroom remains an important quest for cognitive scientists.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia , Abogados , Represión Psicológica , Humanos , Amnesia/psicología , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto , Reino Unido , Abogados/psicología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Recuerdo Mental , Anciano
16.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(19)2021 05 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33952702

RESUMEN

A degraded, black-and-white image of an object, which appears meaningless on first presentation, is easily identified after a single exposure to the original, intact image. This striking example of perceptual learning reflects a rapid (one-trial) change in performance, but the kind of learning that is involved is not known. We asked whether this learning depends on conscious (hippocampus-dependent) memory for the images that have been presented or on an unconscious (hippocampus-independent) change in the perception of images, independently of the ability to remember them. We tested five memory-impaired patients with hippocampal lesions or larger medial temporal lobe (MTL) lesions. In comparison to volunteers, the patients were fully intact at perceptual learning, and their improvement persisted without decrement from 1 d to more than 5 mo. Yet, the patients were impaired at remembering the test format and, even after 1 d, were impaired at remembering the images themselves. To compare perceptual learning and remembering directly, at 7 d after seeing degraded images and their solutions, patients and volunteers took either a naming test or a recognition memory test with these images. The patients improved as much as the volunteers at identifying the degraded images but were severely impaired at remembering them. Notably, the patient with the most severe memory impairment and the largest MTL lesions performed worse than the other patients on the memory tests but was the best at perceptual learning. The findings show that one-trial, long-lasting perceptual learning relies on hippocampus-independent (nondeclarative) memory, independent of any requirement to consciously remember.


Asunto(s)
Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Amnesia/fisiopatología , Femenino , Hipocampo/fisiopatología , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología
17.
Brain Inj ; 38(2): 142-149, 2024 01 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38328966

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The aim of this scoping review was to identify behavioral disturbances exhibited by patients in post-traumatic amnesia (PTA). While behavioral disturbances are common in PTA, research into their presentation and standardized measures for their assessment are limited. DESIGN: The study protocol was registered with PROSPERO (CRD42021268275). A scoping review of databases was performed according to pre-determined criteria on 29 July 2021 and updated on 13 July 2022. A conventional content analysis was used to examine and categorize behavioral disturbances. RESULTS: Thirty papers met the inclusion criteria, of which 27 reported observations and/or scores obtained on behavioral scales, and 3 on clinician interviews and surveys. None focused exclusively on children. Agitation was the most frequently assessed behavior, and Agitated Behavior Scale was the most used instrument. Content analysis, however, bore eight broad behavioral categories: disinhibition, agitation, aggression, lability, lethargy/low mood, perceptual disturbances/psychotic symptoms, personality change and sleep disturbances. CONCLUSION: Our study revealed that while standardized assessments of behavior of patients in PTA are often limited to agitation, clinical descriptions include a range of behavioral disturbances. Our study highlights a significant gap in the systematic assessment of a wide range of behavioral disturbances observed in PTA.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo , Problema de Conducta , Niño , Humanos , Amnesia/etiología , Amnesia/diagnóstico , Amnesia Retrógrada , Ansiedad , Agresión
18.
Clin J Sport Med ; 34(1): 38-43, 2024 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37058611

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: 1) Evaluate the frequency of same-year, repeat concussions; (2) assess predictors of sustaining a repeat concussion; and (3) compare outcomes of athletes with repeat concussions with athletes with single concussion. DESIGN: A retrospective, case-control study. SETTING: Regional sports concussion center. PATIENTS: Adolescents sustaining a sport-related concussions (SRC) from November 2017 to October 2020. INDEPENDENT VARIABLES: Participants were dichotomized into 2 groups: (1) athletes with a single concussion; and (2) athletes with repeat concussions. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Between group and within group analyses were completed to look for differences in demographics, personal and family history, concussion history, and recovery metrics between the 2 groups. RESULTS: Of 834 athletes with an SRC, 56 (6.7%) sustained a repeat concussion and 778 (93.3%) had a single concussion. Between group: Personal history of migraines (19.6% vs 9.5%, χ 2 = 5.795, P = 0.02), family history of migraines (37.5% vs 24.5%, χ 2 = 4.621, P = 0.03), and family history of psychiatric disorders (25% vs 13.1%, χ 2 = 6.224, P = 0.01) were significant predictors of sustaining a repeat concussion. Within group: Among those with a repeat concussion, initial symptom severity was greater (Z = -2.422; P = 0.02) during the repeat concussion and amnesia was more common (χ 2 = 4.775, P = 0.03) after the initial concussion. CONCLUSIONS: In a single-center study of 834 athletes, 6.7% suffered a same-year, repeat concussion. Risk factors included personal/family migraine history and family psychiatric history. For athletes with repeat concussions, initial symptom score was higher after the second concussion, yet amnesia was more common after the initial concussion.


Asunto(s)
Traumatismos en Atletas , Conmoción Encefálica , Trastornos Migrañosos , Adolescente , Humanos , Traumatismos en Atletas/diagnóstico , Estudios Retrospectivos , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Conmoción Encefálica/complicaciones , Conmoción Encefálica/epidemiología , Conmoción Encefálica/diagnóstico , Amnesia/etiología , Atletas , Trastornos Migrañosos/complicaciones
19.
Alzheimers Dement ; 20(1): 437-446, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37671801

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Alzheimer's disease studies often lack ethnic diversity. METHODS: We evaluated associations between plasma biomarkers commonly studied in Alzheimer's (p-tau181, GFAP, and NfL), clinical diagnosis (clinically normal, amnestic MCI, amnestic dementia, or non-amnestic MCI/dementia), and Aß-PET in Hispanic and non-Hispanic older adults. Hispanics were predominantly of Cuban or South American ancestry. RESULTS: Three-hundred seventy nine participants underwent blood draw (71.9 ± 7.8 years old, 60.2% female, 57% Hispanic of which 88% were Cuban or South American) and 240 completed Aß-PET. P-tau181 was higher in amnestic MCI (p = 0.004, d = 0.53) and dementia (p < 0.001, d = 0.97) than in clinically normal participants and discriminated Aß-PET[+] and Aß-PET[-] (AUC = 0.86). P-tau181 outperformed GFAP and NfL. There were no significant interactions with ethnicity. Among amnestic MCI, Hispanics had lower odds of elevated p-tau181 than non-Hispanic (OR = 0.41, p = 0.006). DISCUSSION: Plasma p-tau181 informs etiological diagnosis of cognitively impaired Hispanic and non-Hispanic older adults. Hispanic ethnicity may relate to greater likelihood of non-Alzheimer's contributions to memory loss. HIGHLIGHTS: Alzheimer's biomarkers were measured in Hispanic and non-Hispanic older adults. Plasma p-tau181 related to amnestic cognitive decline and brain amyloid burden. AD biomarker associations did not differ between Hispanic and non-Hispanic ethnicity. Hispanic individuals may be more likely to have non-Alzheimer causes of memory loss.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer , Disfunción Cognitiva , Femenino , Humanos , Anciano , Persona de Mediana Edad , Masculino , Proteínas Amiloidogénicas , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Amnesia , Biomarcadores , Péptidos beta-Amiloides , Proteínas tau
20.
Int J Mol Sci ; 25(7)2024 Mar 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38612521

RESUMEN

The beneficial effects of increasing histamine levels on memory have acquired special interest due to their applicability to psychiatric conditions that cause memory impairments. In addition, by employing drug repurposing approaches, it was demonstrated that dihydroergotamine (DHE), an FDA drug approved to treat migraines, inhibits Histamine N Methyl Transferase (HNMT), the enzyme responsible for the inactivation of histamine in the brain. For this reason, in the present work, the effect of DHE on histamine levels in the hippocampus and its effects on memory was evaluated, employing the scopolamine-induced amnesia model, the Novel Object Recognition (NOR) paradigm, and the Morris Water Maze (MWM). Furthermore, the role of histamine 1 receptor (H1R) and histamine 2 receptor (H2R) antagonists in the improvement in memory produced by DHE in the scopolamine-induced amnesia model was evaluated. Results showed that the rats that received DHE (10 mg/kg, i.p.) showed increased histamine levels in the hippocampus after 1 h of administration but not after 5 h. In behavioral assays, it was shown that DHE (1 mg/kg, i.p.) administered 20 min before the training reversed the memory impairment produced by the administration of scopolamine (2 mg/kg, i.p.) immediately after the training in the NOR paradigm and MWM. Additionally, the effects in memory produced by DHE were blocked by pre-treatment with pyrilamine (20 mg/kg, i.p.) administered 30 min before the training in the NOR paradigm and MWM. These findings allow us to demonstrate that DHE improves memory in a scopolamine-induced amnesia model through increasing histamine levels at the hippocampus due to its activity as an HNMT inhibitor.


Asunto(s)
Dihidroergotamina , Escopolamina , Animales , Ratas , Histamina , Amnesia/inducido químicamente , Amnesia/tratamiento farmacológico , Encéfalo , Trastornos de la Memoria/inducido químicamente , Trastornos de la Memoria/tratamiento farmacológico , Antagonistas de los Receptores H2 de la Histamina
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA