Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
País de afiliação
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Memory ; 23(2): 167-77, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24499200

RESUMO

People often pick up incorrect information about the world from movies, novels and other fictional sources. The question asked here is whether such sources are a particularly potent source of misinformation. On the one hand, story-reading involves transportation into a fictional world, with a possible reduction in access to one's prior knowledge (likely reducing the chances that the reader will notice errors). On the other hand, stories encourage relational processing as readers create mental models, decreasing the likelihood that they will encode and remember more peripheral details like erroneous facts. To test these ideas, we examined suggestibility after readers were exposed to misleading references embedded in stories and lists that were matched on a number of dimensions. In two experiments, suggestibility was greater following exposure to misinformation in a list of sentences rather than a coherent story, even though the story was rated as more engaging than the list. Furthermore, processing the story with an item-specific processing task (inserting missing letters) increased later suggestibility, whereas this task had no impact on suggestibility when misinformation was presented within a list. The type of processing used when reading a text affects suggestibility more than engagement with the text.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Compreensão , Aprendizagem , Literatura Moderna , Humanos , Sugestão
2.
Memory ; 22(5): 481-92, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23705952

RESUMO

Many people respond "two" to the question "How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?", even though they know the reference should be to Noah. The Moses Illusion demonstrates a failure to apply stored knowledge (Erickson & Mattson, 1981). Of interest was whether older adults' robust knowledge bases would protect them from vulnerability to this illusion. Of secondary interest were any age differences in the memorial consequences of the illusion, and whether older adults' prior knowledge would protect them from later reproducing information from distorted questions (e.g., later saying that Moses took two animals of each kind on the ark). Surprisingly, older adults fell for the Moses Illusion more often than did younger adults. However, falling for the illusion did not affect older adults' later memory; they were less suggestible than young adults. Most importantly, older adults were more likely to recover from exposure to distorted questions and respond correctly. Explanations of these findings, drawing on theories of cognitive ageing, are discussed.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Ilusões/psicologia , Memória , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Conhecimento , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
3.
Mem Cognit ; 39(5): 791-805, 2011 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21264634

RESUMO

Low-frequency (LF) words produce higher hit rates and lower false alarm rates than high-frequency (HF) words. This word frequency mirror pattern has been interpreted within dual-process models of recognition, which assume the contributions of a slower recollective process and a relatively fast-acting familiarity process. In the present experiments, recollection and familiarity were placed in opposition using Jacoby, L. L., Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513-541 (1991), two-list exclusion paradigm with HF and LF words. Exclusion errors to LF words exceeded those to HF words at fast deadlines, whereas the reverse occurred at slow deadlines. In Experiments 2 and 3, false alarms to HF nonpresented lures were higher than to LF nonpresented lures, indicating the use of baseline familiarity for totally new items. Combined, these results indicate that in addition to baseline familiarity and recollection, a third process involving the assessment of a relative change in familiarity is involved in recognition performance. Both relative changes in familiarity and recollection processes have distinct time courses and are engaged when there is diagnostic list information available, whereas baseline familiarity is used when there is no diagnostic information available (e.g., for nonpresented lure items).


Assuntos
Atenção , Formação de Conceito , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Aprendizagem Verbal , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Prática Psicológica , Tempo de Reação , Leitura , Semântica , Percepção da Fala
4.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 14(3): 479-83, 2007 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17874592

RESUMO

Of interest was whether prior testing of related words primes false memories in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. After studying lists of related words, subjects made old-new judgments about zero, three, or six related items before being tested on critical nonpresented lures. When the recognition test was self-paced, prior testing of list items led to faster false recognition judgments, but did not increase the rate of false alarms to lures from studied lists. Critically, this pattern changed when decision making at test was speeded. When forced to respond quickly--presumably precluding the use of monitoring processes--clear test-induced priming effects were observed in the rate of false memories. The results are consistent with an activation-monitoring explanation of false memories and support that retrieving veridical memories can be a source of memory error.


Assuntos
Memória , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Repressão Psicológica , Adulto , Aprendizagem por Associação , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , North Carolina , Testes Psicológicos , Tempo de Reação , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Retenção Psicológica , Aprendizagem Verbal , Vocabulário
5.
Psychol Aging ; 19(1): 134-44, 2004 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15065937

RESUMO

In 3 experiments, the authors examined part-set cuing effects in younger and older adults. Participants heard lists of category exemplars and later recalled them. Recall was uncued or cued with a subset of studied items. In Experiment 1, participants were cued with some of the category names, and they remembered fewer never-cued categories than a free-recall condition. In Experiment 2, a similar effect was observed for category exemplar cues. There was also an age difference: By some measures, a small number of cues impaired older adults more than younger. Experiment 3 replicated this result and found that older adults were disproportionately slow in the presence of cues. Across experiments, older adults showed robust part-set cuing effects, and sometimes, they were disproportionately impaired by cues.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/epidemiologia , Rememoração Mental , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tempo de Reação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA